Episode Transcript
Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.
Use Ctrl + F to search
0:00
Big foot and be
0:02
on with Cliff and
0:05
Bobo. These guys are
0:07
your favorites. So like
0:10
you're subscribing rated. Five
0:12
stars. Big foot and
0:15
be on. The greatest
0:18
podcast. Wherever you're listening
0:20
or watching. James
0:29
Bobofay. Hey, Bob's, what's happening? Hey,
0:31
Clint, how much how's it going? It's going
0:33
all right. Just another beautiful day hanging out,
0:36
gonna talk to a great guest today about
0:38
a great subject. I'm kind of excited about
0:40
it. But we should probably let Darby talk
0:42
about that. So why don't we just bring
0:45
Darby on? We'll start talking. How are you?
0:47
Hi, I'm doing well. How are you? Good. Doing
0:49
right, pushing forward. So thank you so
0:51
much for coming on. You have such
0:53
an exciting project here, but before we
0:55
get going on that, I was looking
0:57
up, I was going to try to
0:59
familiarize myself with your biography. And the
1:01
nearest, I mean, correct me if I'm
1:04
wrong, but the nearest, I can figure
1:06
is, you're kind of, you're a library
1:08
and some sort, you're a research of
1:10
a variety of subjects, and it seems
1:12
like you're a facilitator, more than, say,
1:14
say, an instructor, or a good way
1:16
to categorize you. Well, I am an
1:19
instructor as well. Yeah, I'm
1:21
on faculty at North Carolina
1:23
State University. My primary faculty
1:26
appointment is as a
1:28
librarian, but as you know, Cliff,
1:30
not doing the sorts of things
1:32
that most people think of when
1:34
you think of a librarian, but
1:36
also I do teach and I
1:39
am very much involved with research.
1:41
And my work is a
1:43
librarian, especially for many, many
1:45
years. My job has been to
1:47
help put together and support highly
1:49
interdisciplinary research teams
1:52
to tackle really tough questions. So
1:54
to put together the right people
1:57
and to help translate between fields.
1:59
in such a way that people
2:01
can deal with real-world
2:04
issues that require expertise
2:06
that's coming from a
2:08
number of different areas. Now
2:10
is this, is this, is your
2:13
role here needed because the
2:15
disciplines don't tend to speak
2:17
to one another? That is
2:20
sometimes the case. Experts in
2:22
different disciplines oftentimes either don't
2:24
know how to speak to
2:26
one another or sometimes don't
2:29
realize that they're asking questions
2:31
along similar lines or about
2:33
similar subjects in ways where
2:35
they could be working together
2:37
and getting further that way.
2:40
Sometimes they don't know how
2:42
to speak one another's language.
2:44
And so I play this
2:46
role of matchmaking and translating
2:48
and facilitating and facilitating. For
2:50
you, you must be kind of
2:53
familiar with a huge number of
2:55
disciplines then to even know what's
2:58
going on to any of them
3:00
and realize that there might be
3:02
connections that could be made between
3:05
them. Yeah, very much so. In
3:07
fact, I'm usually the person in
3:09
the room who knows the most
3:12
about everything and the least about
3:14
any given subject. I am also
3:16
one of those people, which is
3:18
why I became an elementary school
3:21
teacher. I was pretty good at
3:23
everything, but not really great at
3:25
anything. In your career path
3:27
or in your personal life,
3:29
because obviously these things intersect
3:31
on a regular basis for
3:33
everyone, what led you down the
3:35
path to Saswatch? Well, I had been
3:38
teaching courses for a number
3:40
of years and particularly been
3:42
teaching courses about... how science works,
3:44
not just the sorts of things
3:47
that say you might have taught
3:49
your students in elementary school
3:51
of how science says it
3:54
works, no hypothesis and testing
3:56
and experimentation, those sorts of
3:59
things. but also culturally
4:01
speaking, how science works,
4:03
what are the economic structures
4:06
that underlie scientific research, what
4:08
are the ways that people
4:11
communicate with one another, how
4:13
do scientists communicate with the
4:15
general public as well as
4:18
with one another, with funding
4:20
agencies, with government, with industry,
4:23
and how science's scientific
4:26
discoveries are applied. in
4:28
the real world. So there's
4:30
a lot that goes into
4:32
science that oftentimes even scientists
4:35
are not fully aware of
4:37
themselves. And so I've been
4:39
teaching classes on that for a
4:41
great many years. And I
4:44
came upon this idea that
4:46
I thought would be really
4:48
interesting of looking at fringe
4:50
sciences or people doing scientific
4:53
work in areas that are oftentimes
4:55
not considered. science that
4:57
might be might be
4:59
referred to as pseudo science or
5:02
dismissed in that way. And I
5:04
thought it would be very interesting
5:06
to teach a course, and this
5:09
is what I started doing, that
5:11
looked at some of these areas
5:13
and asked these questions
5:16
of why are these
5:18
subjects dismissed by quote
5:20
unquote mainstream science or
5:22
the general public? Are
5:24
they being dismissed for
5:26
good reason? What can that
5:28
teach us about the ways
5:30
that we make decisions, the
5:32
ways that cultural aspects play
5:35
into our scientific questions and
5:37
practices? And so I started
5:39
engaging with some of
5:41
these fields, including the
5:43
field of Sasquatch Research
5:45
or Sasquatch Investigation. And
5:47
it was very, very
5:49
interesting to me, and one of
5:52
the things that I really wanted to
5:54
do with my students. was to challenge
5:56
them to look at the very best
5:59
evidence. in each of these
6:01
areas. Because as you know, there
6:03
are lots of people making
6:06
lots of claims and you
6:08
can find lots of silly
6:10
things in any of
6:12
these fringe or paranormal
6:14
or edge science fields.
6:16
You can find plenty of
6:18
things to make fun of,
6:20
but it's much more challenging
6:23
to deal with the best
6:25
potential evidence in these areas.
6:28
and see what it says
6:30
because any potential evidence is
6:32
actually evidence of something. So the
6:35
question is really, let's figure out
6:37
what it is. Do you have
6:39
a list of the best evidence
6:42
that you're going to focus on
6:44
right now? Well, you know, I
6:46
think that there are a number
6:48
of areas where there are interesting
6:50
questions to be asked
6:52
with regard to potential
6:55
Sasquatch investigation. In my current
6:57
study, the one that I'm leading
6:59
with my colleagues now that we're
7:02
talking about today, I really
7:04
wanted to look at
7:06
the physical biological evidence
7:08
that could potentially yield
7:10
DNA. And the reason for
7:12
that is that DNA is
7:15
really the gold standard for
7:17
the establishment of a species.
7:19
And that is, of course, one
7:22
of the big questions or one
7:24
of the big hypotheses. that the
7:27
saucewatch phenomenon could
7:29
potentially be explained,
7:31
at least in large part, by
7:34
a unique biological species. You
7:36
don't know, unless you look
7:38
at the evidence, unless
7:40
you examine the evidence, what
7:43
it is. And that's something
7:45
that I found, was that
7:47
there were people who were
7:49
finding hairs, that were people
7:51
who were finding other sorts
7:54
of physical samples, and... They
7:56
were asking me, where can I send to
7:58
have this tested? How do I... How do
8:00
I go about that? And I really
8:02
found that I couldn't find
8:05
a commercial lab or a university
8:07
or anywhere really that I
8:09
could really feel confident saying,
8:11
yeah, this is the place to
8:14
send this if you have a
8:16
potential unknown. And so that's
8:19
why I got together a team
8:21
and created that myself with
8:23
a study. So this is
8:25
a university-sponsored study. Is
8:28
that correct? Well, it's
8:30
university sponsored in
8:33
the sense that most
8:35
studies are university sponsored.
8:37
What that means really
8:39
is that in this
8:42
case, there are four
8:44
of us, faculty colleagues,
8:46
who are collaborating on
8:48
this and who have
8:50
set the scope and
8:53
the parameters of the study.
8:55
And we are all. on faculty
8:57
at NC State University.
9:00
So it's university sponsored
9:02
in that sense. The
9:04
other thing is that it
9:07
is an official study, and
9:09
so I've gone through the
9:11
paperwork. I had to
9:13
go through what we
9:15
call institutional review board
9:18
approval, which is our
9:20
ethics committee that makes
9:22
sure that when we do
9:24
any research. that we are treating
9:26
both humans and animals with
9:28
respect and care and properly
9:31
ethically. So you had to
9:33
fill out a probably a
9:35
mountain of paperwork for this
9:37
thing and then somehow still squeeze
9:39
it back, or squeeze it through
9:41
rather, knowing that this is kind
9:43
of a Sasquatch study, but you
9:45
couldn't put Sasquatch in the forefront,
9:48
right? So how did you get
9:50
around that? You know, this is and
9:52
this isn't. a Sasquatch study. And what
9:54
we're really interested in in this case,
9:56
and this is what the name of
9:59
the study is, It's a
10:01
study of allegedly
10:04
morphologically anomalous physical
10:06
samples. So what does that
10:09
mean? Basically that means
10:11
this is a study
10:13
of biological specimens that
10:16
people have found that
10:18
they claim have
10:20
unusual traits or
10:22
unusual appearances or
10:24
something unusual about them.
10:26
And that is what we're
10:29
studying. This comes from
10:31
my own hearing and
10:33
talking with folks, particularly
10:36
in the Bigfoot community,
10:38
who have found samples,
10:41
for example, found
10:43
hair samples, that folks
10:45
who are actually very
10:48
good at identifying hair
10:50
of known species in
10:52
North America cannot identify.
10:55
There are certain properties
10:57
to these hairs that
11:00
they reportedly have in
11:02
common, and yet they don't
11:04
look like those of known
11:06
species. That's an interesting
11:09
question. What are these?
11:11
So these are the types
11:13
of questions that we're trying
11:15
to answer. What are these things?
11:18
Every physical sample
11:20
that will be submitted
11:22
to us is a sample of
11:24
something. We don't know what it
11:26
is unless we look at it. Why
11:29
wouldn't we want to look at these?
11:31
Stay tuned for more Bigfoot and
11:33
Beyond with Cliffin Bobo. We'll
11:36
be right back after these
11:38
messages. So three to six months
11:40
doesn't seem like a long time,
11:42
right? In Bigfoot Land, it's not
11:45
very long because we've been looking
11:47
for Sasquatches for decades and decades
11:49
and decades, but... What do you
11:51
think about what can happen in
11:53
three to six months? Like what
11:55
would you do in three to
11:57
six months? I'm giving you six months.
11:59
Go. Squaw. Squat, of course, of course,
12:01
and how much do you think
12:03
is going to get done during
12:05
that time in Squat, are you going
12:08
to see one in six months?
12:10
Probably not. But in three to
12:12
six months, what you can see,
12:14
if you're not going to see
12:17
a Sasquatch, is thicker, fuller hair.
12:19
Regrown from hymns, our sponsor,
12:21
hymns, not bad for just
12:23
three to six months. Yeah, because
12:25
hymns will give you a convenient access
12:28
to a range of hair-lost treatments that
12:30
work, all from the comfort of your couch.
12:32
They make it pretty simple, man. They
12:34
use real doctors. It's clinically proven ingredients.
12:37
They use like finesteride and monoxibil. And
12:39
I love that idea. No doctor visits,
12:41
because first of all, doctors, who wants
12:43
to go see a doctor, nothing personal,
12:45
nothing personal, personal doctors. But also you
12:47
make an appointment now, you get in
12:50
August or something, or something, forget or
12:52
something. and I know you have access
12:54
to the internet because you're listening
12:56
to us right now. You can pick how
12:58
you want to do it like chewables, orals,
13:01
spray, serum treatments, whatever you want. I mean
13:03
they can work with you and customize it
13:05
to your preferences. Yeah, like it could
13:07
be a one-time thing, it could be
13:09
ongoing care. No insurance is needed at
13:11
all and it's all at one low
13:13
price and that covers everything from these
13:15
treatments to the ongoing care if you
13:17
so choose. Well they always said
13:20
the proofs and the pudin
13:22
and there's hundreds of thousands
13:24
of subscribers that trust it and
13:27
you know give it positive review
13:29
so there you go there's your pudin.
13:31
Thick hairy pudding. Search your
13:33
free online visit today
13:36
at hymns.com/beyond for
13:38
your personalized hair
13:40
loss treatment options.
13:42
hymns.com/beyond. Results vary based on
13:44
studies of topical and oral monoxidil
13:47
and finasteride. Prescription products require an
13:49
online consultation with a health care
13:51
provider who will determine if prescription
13:53
is appropriate. Restrictions apply. See website
13:55
for full details and important safety
13:57
information. So at this point what we've done
13:59
we just launched the online intake
14:02
survey at the tail end
14:04
of May. So we started,
14:06
we've had a trickle of
14:08
offers coming in thus far.
14:11
It's just started to
14:13
pick up over the last
14:15
couple of weeks. So at
14:17
this stage, as of this
14:20
recording, I am starting to
14:22
reach out to those folks, starting
14:25
to arrange brief interviews
14:27
with them. I have some
14:29
questions to learn and make
14:32
sure that we understand a
14:34
bit more about the samples
14:36
and the circumstances under which
14:39
they were found. And then
14:41
at that point, we'll be
14:43
giving those folks instructions as
14:45
to how to physically submit
14:47
the samples that we're
14:50
prioritizing at this time.
14:52
So I imagine that certainly by...
14:54
early September or so, I imagine
14:56
we will have the first samples
14:59
in hand on campus and in
15:01
the lab. So let's think about
15:03
it from a user perspective first and
15:06
then we'll talk about it from
15:08
the more the mechanical side, I
15:10
guess your side of things. So
15:12
if someone like say I found
15:14
Saswatch footprints and they went by
15:16
a tree and there were a
15:18
number of strange hairs on the
15:21
tree and I collected them. Now,
15:23
of course, I'm perhaps slightly better
15:25
equipped than your average Joe in
15:27
the forest. I would put these
15:29
underneath the microscope and take a
15:31
look at him. If I couldn't
15:34
easily identify them as bear or deer
15:36
or anything like that, and I wanted to
15:38
submit them, what would I do? Well, what
15:40
you would do is you would go to
15:42
the intake survey form that's online. Okay, and
15:44
by the way, we will have that, we'll
15:47
have that link in the show notes below.
15:49
We'll have that link in the show notes
15:51
below. Thank you very much for that.
15:53
I appreciate y'all getting the word out.
15:55
So the first thing you would do
15:57
is fill out. Well, I mean, the
15:59
first. thing you would do obviously
16:02
is collect the sample and
16:04
folks asked me hey what's
16:06
the best way to collect
16:08
the sample and there are
16:11
certainly lots of folks out
16:13
there who who describe good
16:15
techniques for sample collection but
16:17
the most important things to
16:20
do are to try to
16:22
collect the sample if you
16:24
can in a sterile way
16:26
so with sterile gloves or
16:29
sterile instruments most samples if
16:31
they're hair or something like
16:33
that moist wet, you know,
16:35
like blood or something, but
16:37
hairs, most of the samples
16:40
that folks are offering this
16:42
far, it's best to collect
16:44
those into a paper envelope
16:46
and stick them in the
16:49
refrigerator in the freezer. So
16:51
that's the best way to
16:53
store samples, collect and store
16:55
samples. So then you would
16:58
fill out the online survey,
17:00
at which point... Like I
17:02
say, I'll be reaching out
17:04
to people to then schedule
17:07
a brief interview by phone
17:09
or on the computer or
17:11
what have you. And at
17:13
that point, we'll be asking
17:16
folks, we'll be identifying which
17:18
samples we want to get
17:20
in some priority order. And
17:22
we'll be testing those. Anyone
17:25
who submits a sample in
17:27
that form can choose whether
17:29
they want to be identified.
17:31
publicly identified with their sample
17:34
or whether they want to
17:36
be completely anonymous because some
17:38
people want to be identified
17:40
and some people just want
17:42
to know what something is
17:45
and they don't want anybody
17:47
to know that they had
17:49
anything to do with Sasquatch.
17:51
Either one is fine. Under
17:54
no circumstances will we be
17:56
disclosing specific locations where samples
17:58
were samples were acquired. Just.
18:00
will be giving general locations,
18:03
but everybody who submits
18:05
a sample will receive
18:07
the results for their
18:09
sample. And what sort
18:11
of timeline would that be?
18:14
Well, here's the thing, Cliff.
18:16
Real science is not a
18:18
speedy process. So I'm
18:20
trying not to promise. a
18:22
specific timeline to folks, other
18:24
than we'll do it as well,
18:27
we will do these things as
18:29
quickly as we can, but that
18:32
could depend on a large
18:34
number of factors. So if
18:36
the sample, if the sample
18:38
that's submitted is something that
18:41
we come in, we look
18:43
at it, we run a test, and
18:45
oh, it pops up. Oh, this
18:48
is a black bear and it's
18:50
very clearly black bear and you
18:52
know, you may get a response
18:54
in a matter of a couple
18:56
of months. If it's a sample
18:58
that is more degraded
19:00
that we have or
19:02
having trouble pulling sequencing
19:05
information out of, we may be
19:07
working with it for a while. It
19:09
really just depends on
19:11
so many things that are
19:13
beyond. our control or the
19:16
people submitting these things as
19:18
to how long it can take.
19:20
But I do promise you,
19:22
you will receive a response.
19:24
You will receive a report
19:26
for your sample. Yeah, you
19:28
would have to be
19:30
extraordinarily careful, of course,
19:33
because if you say, for
19:35
example, it happened, a Sasquatch
19:37
hair is submitted, it's in
19:39
good shape. passes the microscopic
19:41
analysis and the DNA comes
19:43
back is what the hell
19:45
is this, right? Even then
19:47
it would have to be
19:49
tested and retested because a
19:51
false positive in this in
19:53
this particular scenario could sink
19:55
everything. They could just make
19:57
the subject and the study
19:59
just a laughing stock. So
20:01
you'd have to be
20:03
extraordinarily careful and just
20:05
very, very conservative in
20:07
how you assess the
20:10
samples, especially if it seems that
20:12
we're on to the right thing
20:14
for the first time. Well, absolutely.
20:16
And if that is a finding
20:18
of the study, that's one of
20:21
the reasons that I have
20:23
recruited the team that I
20:25
have, because these folks are
20:27
Absolutely tremendous. And this team
20:30
is when you receive a
20:32
report, you're going to know
20:34
that that's what it is.
20:36
If what you said, if
20:39
there is a Sasquatch out
20:41
there to be found and
20:43
there are samples submitted,
20:45
and we find that, well, this
20:47
team is going to, you know,
20:49
very carefully document
20:52
that and present
20:54
that. Again, if that's if
20:56
that's the finding. You know, at
20:58
this stage we haven't looked
21:01
at any samples. The salient
21:03
point here is that we
21:05
don't know what the samples
21:07
that we'll be receiving are
21:10
until we look at them. And
21:12
that's what makes this really
21:14
exciting is that there are
21:16
a lot of rather interesting
21:19
sounding samples out
21:21
there that have not been looked
21:23
at. by a credible
21:25
scientific team. And so
21:28
I'm really excited to see
21:30
what our results are.
21:32
So you said there's four
21:34
of you on the team?
21:37
Who are there three people?
21:39
Well, there are four of
21:41
us on the core team,
21:44
myself, and then two geneticists,
21:46
genomists, and one microscopist. So,
21:49
microscope expert, think
21:51
of that. And so this
21:53
is the core team and
21:55
this team also has
21:57
a lot of expertise. in
22:00
forensics and wildlife forensics as
22:02
well. So this is the
22:04
core team, but the fun
22:06
thing about this is is
22:08
I also have a whole
22:10
host of experts in particular
22:12
areas who are ready to
22:14
come on and join us
22:16
if and when we find
22:18
an interesting specimen in their
22:20
area of expertise. And these
22:22
are these are faculty researchers
22:24
not just from NC State,
22:26
but from across the country.
22:28
So for example, I have
22:30
someone on board who is
22:32
an physical anthropologist and an
22:34
expert in all things osteo.
22:36
So if a bone or
22:38
tooth sample that is interesting
22:41
is submitted for this study,
22:43
she is ready to jump
22:45
on board. and get involved
22:47
and really excited about it.
22:49
But again, if we don't
22:51
have something in her area
22:53
of expertise, well, she's going
22:55
to stay in the Bigfoot
22:57
closet for now. For that
22:59
genome, for doing the full
23:01
genome, yeah, you always hear
23:03
stuff like last time we
23:05
talked to Melzier, we said
23:07
about 430,000, 440,000 for a
23:09
full genome sequencing. Is that
23:11
like, is going to ask
23:13
about your budget, like how
23:15
it got approved, like, how
23:17
much they give you give
23:19
you and then? Like when
23:21
you hear those high numbers,
23:23
is that going to be
23:25
cut way down since these
23:27
guys are on faculty, staff,
23:29
like they're already paid through
23:31
the university? How does that
23:33
work? I'm sorry, but specifically,
23:35
Meldrum said in the six-figure
23:37
range, and most of that
23:40
would be for personnel. Right.
23:42
And so that is probably
23:44
pretty reasonable to expect if
23:46
you were going to go
23:48
out and hire and hire
23:50
the work done by someone
23:52
who might know what they're
23:54
doing. Now in this case...
23:56
since we have the we
23:58
have the machines on campus
24:00
that are available. to us
24:02
where we can do nuclear
24:04
DNA sequencing for those samples
24:06
that may warrant it, then
24:08
you know we have an
24:11
in-house price schedule that looks
24:13
quite a bit different than
24:15
that to be honest. And
24:18
because my colleagues are the
24:20
ones with the expertise not
24:22
having to hire my colleagues
24:25
to work on our own
24:27
study, that actually reduces the
24:29
cost dramatically. How many do you
24:31
think you could run like for the,
24:34
with your budget you have for
24:36
the year, like how many samples
24:38
do you run like 20 or 50
24:40
or? Well, see this is the thing,
24:42
we don't have a set budget. for
24:45
this project. And the reason
24:47
we don't is that there
24:49
are quite a few different
24:51
needs and quite a few
24:53
different goals that this project
24:56
can potentially help us to achieve.
24:58
One of those that's
25:00
really important is training
25:02
of PhD students in
25:04
genetics. So our PhD
25:07
students in genetics who
25:09
work in the labs of my
25:11
two colleagues. who are collaborators,
25:13
they need to teach these
25:15
students anyhow. And they see
25:18
this as a really interesting
25:20
way of having the students
25:22
work with true unknowns or
25:24
with unusual samples. And so
25:26
this is a great way
25:29
for them to be able
25:31
to train their PhD students,
25:33
like I say, which they
25:35
need to do anyway, right?
25:37
They need to be studying
25:40
samples of something. So why
25:42
not? The samples that
25:44
are associated with
25:46
this study. In addition,
25:48
the team we've talked
25:50
about ideas that might
25:53
help push forensic understanding
25:56
forward so that might
25:58
help us. develop new
26:00
forensic techniques that could be
26:03
useful. Again, not just for
26:05
this study or for this
26:07
topic, but could be very
26:09
useful in the field for
26:11
all kinds of things. And
26:13
these are just a couple
26:15
of examples. There are some
26:18
other things that some other
26:20
outcomes that we are imagining
26:22
from the types of samples
26:24
that we'll be looking at.
26:26
So a lot of the
26:28
costs of the study are
26:31
costs that are already being
26:33
born anyhow. If you have
26:35
to, you know, if you
26:37
have to have a PhD
26:39
student studying a sample of
26:41
something, well, it's going to
26:43
be the same cost whether
26:46
studying a sample from this
26:48
study or a sample from
26:50
some other study, right? How
26:52
hard of a cell is
26:54
it to the university to
26:56
fund this? Yeah, well, it's
26:59
interesting. I mean, again, it's
27:01
not a funding issue, per
27:03
se, because the funding is
27:05
the funding that is already
27:07
happening through the labs. Like
27:09
the institutional reputation, are they
27:11
worried about that? There is
27:14
not really a concern with
27:16
institutional reputation because of the
27:18
way that we are framing
27:20
the study. Again, because we're
27:22
looking at this, we're looking
27:24
at samples, we're looking at
27:27
specimens that are something, right?
27:29
And it's a matter of
27:31
figuring out what they are.
27:33
We are not going out
27:35
there. This is not a
27:37
study about looking for Bigfoot.
27:39
This is a study of
27:42
looking at seemingly unusual physical
27:44
samples to determine what they
27:46
are. Now, we know that
27:48
a number of these samples
27:50
are coming from the Bigfoot
27:52
community. They are the result
27:55
of folks having an experience.
27:57
They feel is a softwatch
27:59
and then finding. these specimens
28:01
in connection with that? Well,
28:03
there's still interesting specimens. So
28:05
we certainly don't want to
28:07
be prejudiced against those specimens.
28:10
In fact, I think if
28:12
anything, the experiences that people
28:14
are having in conjunction with
28:16
finding these types of samples
28:18
make them all the more
28:20
interesting to me for us
28:23
to take a look at.
28:26
Stay tuned for more Bigfoot and Beyond
28:28
with Clifin Bobo. We'll be right back
28:30
after these messages. So we talked a
28:32
little bit about the user side, like
28:34
someone like me who might get a
28:37
sample and want to submit it. We
28:39
talked a little bit about the budget
28:41
and how you got this. past the
28:43
gatekeepers at the university and basically how
28:45
you sold the sold the idea which
28:47
is brilliant of course then it is
28:49
fantastic because working with unknowns that could
28:52
yield a real fantastic mind altering discovery
28:54
is just brilliant but what does it
28:56
look like from your side like your
28:58
team when after I fill out I
29:00
fill out the computer survey thing, my
29:02
name, this and that, whatever, I still
29:04
have the sample, you get that email,
29:07
what happens in, from that, and we'll
29:09
just use an example of a hair,
29:11
because obviously if you have a bone
29:13
or a flesh or blood tissue sample,
29:15
it would be probably a slightly different
29:17
process, but we'll just deal with hair
29:19
for now, because that's probably... I'm guessing
29:22
that'll probably be what the majority of
29:24
the samples you get are, but hair
29:26
samples. So what would that look like
29:28
from your end, from when you receive
29:30
that email to when, I don't know,
29:32
when you put the sample down, I
29:34
guess, I don't know. Well, again, we
29:37
have to prioritize the samples that we're
29:39
looking at. How would you prioritize something
29:41
like that? We don't have a. Folks
29:43
ask me, you know, what's your formula?
29:45
We don't have a set formula for
29:47
that. But certainly what we're interested in
29:49
is we're interested in looking at.
29:52
First of all, we're
29:54
interested in looking
29:56
at samples that appear
29:58
to be the
30:00
most interesting and that
30:02
appear to be
30:04
the freshest, most likely
30:07
to yield genetic
30:09
information. For example, if
30:11
you were to
30:13
offer us a hair
30:15
that you found
30:17
20 years ago and
30:19
it's been in
30:22
your garage, that's going
30:24
to be a
30:26
much lower priority than
30:28
someone who says,
30:30
hey, here's a hair
30:32
that I found
30:34
last week. And
30:36
by the way, I put it under a microscope
30:39
and it's got a little bit of flesh attached
30:41
to it. And it doesn't look like a known
30:43
animal. Sure, sure.
30:45
But now the user, the
30:47
users aren't going to be able
30:49
to determine that. That would
30:51
have to be, I mean, on
30:53
your end, someone like me
30:55
or something. Yeah, sure. I've got
30:57
a microscope. I'm that level
30:59
of nerd. But the average Joe,
31:01
who may have seen a
31:03
Sasquatch and wanted to submit a
31:05
sample, they wouldn't know that.
31:07
So you just get a hair
31:09
and a story, essentially, on
31:11
an email. No, absolutely. And to
31:13
give you an idea, so
31:15
there's a sample that was offered
31:17
to us that sounds interesting
31:19
because the person offering it reported
31:21
seeing a Sasquatch one night.
31:23
He said it was peeking out
31:25
at him from behind a
31:27
pine tree. He went back the
31:29
next morning and in the
31:31
sap of the pine tree found
31:33
a hair stuck there. And
31:35
so he collected that. And that
31:38
was just several months ago.
31:40
That to me is much more
31:42
interesting than, and this is
31:44
not one that was offered, but
31:46
this was something that somebody
31:48
talked with me about a couple
31:50
of years ago. Someone who
31:52
says, Hey, I have a clump
31:54
of hair that I think
31:56
is Sasquatch hair. And I say,
31:58
Well, what makes you think
32:00
it's Sasquatch hair? Why is it interesting to you?
32:02
They say, well, I found it stuck
32:04
15 feet up in a tree, and
32:06
what else would be 15 feet up
32:08
in a tree? Could it be the
32:11
hair of a saucequatch? Sure,
32:13
of course, you don't know until
32:15
you look at it, but of
32:17
course, there are plenty of other
32:19
things that climb trees, there is
32:22
wind, there are birds that use
32:24
clumps of hairs nesting material,
32:26
and they can... fall down, you
32:28
know, there are lots of ways
32:31
that hair could get 15 feet up
32:33
in a tree. So again, we have
32:35
to look at and it'll depend
32:37
on how many things are offered
32:39
to us, but we have to
32:41
be able to put these in
32:43
some sort of priority order because
32:45
we're not looking to just test
32:47
any and every. hair that someone finds
32:50
in the forest? Of course not. So
32:52
say you have a good sample. Say
32:54
this person who saw the Sasquatch and
32:56
there's a hair associated with it or
32:58
you know you get another sample that
33:00
seems very promising to you. What is
33:02
the process at that point? I'm assuming,
33:04
I mean I'm just guessing here to
33:06
you'd probably put under a microscope first
33:08
talk to your microscope dude or a
33:10
gal and then move forward from there.
33:13
What actually does happen after that point?
33:15
Absolutely. Well, the first thing
33:17
is, obviously, yes, we will be
33:19
looking at it microscopically. If we
33:21
get a specimen in and it
33:23
looks, you know, like for example,
33:25
this this hair that was found
33:27
in the in the sap of
33:30
the pine tree, if we put
33:32
it under a microscope and it
33:34
looks exactly like the hair of a gray
33:36
squirrel, which are common to that
33:38
area and climb up trees all
33:40
the time, if it looks exactly
33:42
like that. It's a classic
33:45
presentation. Well, we've identified
33:47
it at that point. We don't
33:49
need to move on to genetic
33:51
analysis to say, that's a great
33:53
squirrel. But then, you know, what
33:55
we'll be doing is we'll be
33:57
looking at these things in stepwise.
34:00
a stepwise way throughout. So
34:02
we'll be looking first. We'll
34:04
be looking first at the
34:07
morphology. So again, the appearance
34:09
of these through the microscope,
34:11
seeing what is readily identified.
34:13
If we find things
34:15
that aren't readily identified
34:17
or that look somewhat
34:19
unusual or really unusual
34:22
or whatever it may
34:24
be, that's when we'll
34:26
move on to genetic analysis.
34:28
And we're going to proceed
34:30
with that very cautiously because
34:32
one of the things that
34:34
we don't want to do
34:36
is we don't want to
34:39
be destructive any more than
34:41
we need to be. We
34:43
don't want to be destructive
34:45
of the samples that we
34:47
receive. And this is one thing
34:49
that told me that I had
34:52
recruited the right team was at
34:54
our very first team meeting back
34:56
when It was one of my
34:59
colleagues who said, we need to
35:01
treat every sample that comes to
35:03
us as precious, because these
35:05
samples are going to be
35:07
precious to the people who
35:09
are submitting them. And I
35:12
was like, wow, I couldn't
35:14
have said that better myself.
35:16
And so we're going to
35:18
be proceeding very cautiously in
35:20
the least destructive ways possible.
35:23
And we're going to be, we will
35:25
go as far. with the analysis as
35:27
we need to go in order
35:29
to determine what the sample
35:31
is. So with a hair sample, we'll,
35:34
you know, certainly looking
35:36
at it microscopically first,
35:38
looking at the mitochondrial
35:40
DNA, and then again, if
35:43
warranted, taking that to the
35:45
new clear DNA, looking at
35:47
the full genome. So with hair samples,
35:49
and again, correct me if I'm wrong,
35:51
I don't know very much in general,
35:53
but there's no nuclear DNA, this just
35:55
mitochondrial DNA. Do you think that, unless
35:58
of course you have the follicle, if
36:00
I remember correctly. But let's
36:02
just say that you have
36:04
a partial hair sample and
36:07
it, could the mitochondrial
36:09
be enough to indicate
36:11
the presence of a
36:14
new species? Potentially, yes.
36:16
Absolutely. You know, I do think
36:18
that we would want to try
36:20
to have multiple samples,
36:23
certainly, where we would find
36:25
that. I do think that having
36:28
like one sample with
36:30
mitochondrial DNA was different
36:32
would be really really
36:34
interesting, but probably not
36:36
enough, you know, certainly
36:38
to establish a species.
36:40
You know, the interesting
36:42
thing is, is that, you
36:44
know, when people submit a
36:46
hair sample, oftentimes, yes, we
36:48
don't know, will that sample
36:50
have follicle in it? Will that
36:52
sample actually have a skin
36:54
cell still attached? that came out of
36:57
the body. And certainly the skin
36:59
cell would contain a complete
37:01
genetic sequence. So again, you're absolutely
37:03
right. Hair is generally not going
37:06
to give us a full genome,
37:08
but a sample that one thinks
37:11
of as hair could potentially do
37:13
so. And I guess this doesn't
37:15
come down to one sample at
37:17
the end of the day,
37:19
because this is science and
37:22
you want repeatability. And certainly...
37:24
Sasswatches are real animals and
37:26
people start sending you a
37:28
flood of hairs a very
37:30
small percentage of those hairs
37:32
will probably represent Sasquatch and
37:34
I'm assuming by comparing the
37:36
results of these anomalous samples
37:39
that's where the real the
37:41
real positive affirmation is really
37:43
going to lie is the
37:45
repeatability of the same novel
37:47
sequences. Is that correct? Absolutely. If
37:49
we just get one novel sequence...
37:51
Again, that's a really fascinating result.
37:54
That would be, that alone would
37:56
be enough to keep us looking
37:59
and looking and looking. perhaps, but
38:01
yes, it's the idea
38:03
of having multiple of
38:05
that. And so in
38:07
the ideal world, at
38:09
a minimum, I think,
38:11
if you're, if the idea
38:14
is, if you're asking
38:16
me, what would it
38:18
take to establish a
38:20
species, I think that
38:22
actually as few as,
38:24
say, three samples coming
38:27
from three different.
38:29
locations, but that
38:31
share the same novel sequence,
38:34
I would think that that
38:36
in and of itself,
38:38
something as simple as
38:40
that, simple, ha ha, right? Something
38:43
is, you know, as little
38:45
as three samples could could be
38:47
enough to essentially prove species.
38:49
Fantastic. Now, one of the things
38:51
that we spoke about when
38:53
we were speaking in person in
38:56
Tennessee, where I met you face
38:58
to face, something I believe that
39:01
sets the study apart from the
39:03
many, many other studies that
39:05
have kind of come up empty
39:07
after all this time, is
39:09
that this study isn't dependent upon
39:11
you. And I think that's an important thing
39:14
we probably want to put out there because
39:16
I imagine a lot of our perhaps slightly
39:18
more conspiratorial-minded listeners out there are thinking, well,
39:20
this is just going to have more positive
39:23
results and the results are going to go
39:25
missing. You know, the Smithsonian is going to
39:27
take them, the Rothschilds are going to send
39:30
their spy team in and take them or
39:32
whatever sort of, you know, fantasies are out
39:34
there. I don't know. I'm not a conspiracy
39:36
guy. I don't buy any lot of that
39:39
stuff. What would stop the samples after they're
39:41
submitted, especially the positive hits
39:44
if such a thing
39:46
does exist, what would stop
39:48
them from being lost or
39:51
mishandled or going into
39:53
the Smithsonian where all the
39:55
Illuminati of the world will
39:57
burn them in some sort
39:59
of ritual? catastrophe and you know
40:02
we don't need to talk about a
40:04
nuclear event or something like that but
40:06
no I mean this is this is
40:08
why I was determined to set this
40:11
up as a as a as a
40:13
real study. This is not me as
40:15
a faculty member at NC State
40:18
saying hey send me these samples
40:20
I'd like to take a look
40:22
at them. I spent years putting
40:24
together the right team going through
40:26
the paperwork with the university.
40:28
As I said, I had to
40:31
go through this ethics review.
40:33
Part of that ethics review
40:35
is having a plan so
40:38
that when somebody submits a
40:40
sample and we say we're
40:42
going to give you your
40:45
results, there is a plan
40:47
in place that that person
40:49
will receive those results,
40:51
even if something happens
40:53
to me, you will
40:56
still get your results.
40:58
And that's something that's
41:00
very important. You say about
41:02
all these studies that have
41:04
happened in the past, but
41:07
there aren't actually all these
41:09
studies that have happened in
41:11
the past, Cliff, in terms
41:13
of looking at them as
41:16
actual studies. There was the
41:18
Brian Sykes study, which is
41:20
the only other university
41:22
study of any alleged Sasquatch
41:25
DNA ever. And that was
41:28
a very limited study. They
41:30
looked at a very small
41:32
number of samples. Other
41:35
than that, folks looking
41:37
at these things
41:39
in university settings
41:41
have been individual
41:43
researchers who have been doing
41:45
this on the side out of
41:48
personal interest. as a
41:50
favor to someone. In a
41:53
few cases, universities, I know
41:55
some universities offer a similar
41:57
service to a commercial lab.
42:00
do where they'll look at a
42:02
sample for the general if the
42:04
general public pays to have those
42:06
samples examined. But there
42:09
hasn't been a large-scale
42:11
university study period that's
42:13
been inclusive of this
42:15
potential evidence. And that
42:17
makes a huge difference because I
42:20
was talking with a Bigfoot
42:22
investigator a couple of months
42:24
ago who had collected
42:26
some very interesting samples. a
42:29
few years ago. A faculty
42:31
at a university I shall
42:34
not name, said, hey I'd like
42:36
to take a look at those
42:38
in my lab, and he sent
42:40
them off to this faculty member
42:43
who was very well intentioned,
42:45
and just never got back
42:48
to him, never got back
42:50
to him. And so I asked
42:52
him, I said, well, who was
42:54
this? Let me try to reach
42:56
out. faculty of faculty. And
42:58
I found out that this faculty
43:01
member was deceased. And I'm
43:03
trying to, I'm trying to get
43:05
to the right person in his
43:07
former lab to find out whether
43:09
these samples are still around. But
43:11
I'm almost certain we're going to
43:13
find that that when he passed away
43:16
they weren't part of an official
43:18
study. So they just got discarded
43:20
is what I'm sure. would have
43:23
happened. When something is not
43:25
part of an official study,
43:27
you're really reliant on,
43:30
you're really relying on
43:32
the individual. And again,
43:34
I think a lot
43:37
of very well-intentioned faculty
43:39
have taken samples from
43:41
folks and not given back
43:43
the type of report, the
43:46
type of information. or
43:48
any information in some
43:50
cases, again, despite the
43:52
best of intentions. Stay
43:55
tuned for more Bigfoot and
43:57
Beyond with Clifen Bobo.
43:59
back after these messages.
44:01
So Cliff, you may
44:04
recall several years ago,
44:06
we had some conversations online
44:08
and you had guessed it in
44:10
my university class and thank you
44:12
again for that. That was a
44:14
wonderful time for the students. I
44:17
am still an educator at heart.
44:19
Well, it shows and it's great.
44:21
But at that time, you and
44:23
I had discussed one-on-one, you had
44:26
come into possession of a very
44:28
interesting sample. And I know you
44:30
were saying to me, hey, I'd
44:32
love to send this to you.
44:34
I'd love for your folks to
44:37
look at this. And I know
44:39
at the time I said to
44:41
you, I said, hold on, I
44:43
don't want to take it yet.
44:46
I did not want to be
44:48
that person who takes samples, again,
44:50
with really great intentions. but outside
44:52
of an official study. Because I
44:55
feel like I owe it to
44:57
folks that if they're going to
44:59
submit, they need to know that
45:01
they're going to get back good
45:03
results. They're going to get them
45:06
back regardless, again, regardless of me
45:08
and that they're going to be
45:10
treated with respect. And was that
45:12
the mattress prince? Is that what
45:15
you're referring to? Indeed, yeah, and
45:17
I know we're we're talking about
45:19
ways that you might be able
45:21
to submit part of that now.
45:24
I would be talking that ever
45:26
with my team at our next
45:28
meeting to see how we might
45:30
work with a sample like that.
45:33
I was thinking masking and like
45:35
the other night as falling asleep
45:37
and these are the sort of
45:39
things that put me to sleep
45:41
I guess at the end of
45:44
the night I guess although it
45:46
gets me too excited and worried
45:48
about stuff so I can't actually
45:50
sleep it's a horrible circular thing
45:53
I live in but I was
45:55
thinking maybe send you like a
45:57
cube like a square centimeter or
45:59
something like that of a place
46:02
where I know that it stepped
46:04
and I don't know there's lots
46:06
ways we can do that. And
46:08
also, I did have the foresight
46:10
of collecting the soil off of
46:13
the fabric itself, and I still
46:15
have that in my files. It
46:17
has been refrigerated, unfortunately. I wasn't
46:19
aware of that component, but it's
46:22
in my files in a paper
46:24
bag. So there's also that. I'll
46:26
talk about that later off the
46:28
air, of course. Back to the
46:31
question I was going to ask.
46:33
Is that you mentioned various other,
46:35
I said studies, but perhaps that's
46:37
not the right word, projects that
46:39
had been done over time. And
46:42
I didn't you mention to me
46:44
when we were in Tennessee a
46:46
few weeks ago about using or
46:48
looking at least at other people's
46:51
data to see, kind of like
46:53
in the same way that Dr.
46:55
Haskill Hart did with the Saswatch
46:57
genome project. They just used their
47:00
data and came up with different
47:02
results. Are you looking for other
47:04
former... projects, I guess, and trying
47:06
to get a hold of their
47:09
data as well? And if so,
47:11
for what purpose? Well, yes, I
47:13
have been trying to do that.
47:15
And that has been, I've been
47:17
trying to do that over the
47:20
last several years. And that's been
47:22
a rather frustrating process cliff. And
47:24
it's been rather frustrating because What
47:26
I find is that there really
47:29
haven't been that many analyses done,
47:31
period. There really isn't data to
47:33
look at, hardly at all. In
47:35
fact, when Haskill Hart did his
47:38
interpretation of the Ketchum at all
47:40
study data that you're referring to,
47:42
ironically, that is the only real...
47:44
data, sequencing data that has been
47:46
made available by anybody who's looked
47:49
at alleged saucewatch samples. So one
47:51
thing that the Ketchum team did
47:53
right was that eventually, at least
47:55
after they self-published their paper, they
47:58
did release that data so that
48:00
Haskell and myself and others can
48:02
take a look at that. Now,
48:04
in the case of that study,
48:07
with the data in hand, it's
48:09
easy to see that the interpretation
48:11
that was given by that team
48:13
is wrong. Now, Haskell has looked
48:16
at that and noted a few
48:18
interesting, potentially interesting things in the
48:20
data, but these are really just
48:22
questions at this point. Yeah, and
48:24
I think that's interesting because at
48:27
the end of the day, Haskell
48:29
is not a geneticist. He's a
48:31
chemist. He's a retired chemist and
48:33
just realize that, well, biology and
48:36
DNA, we're just big old bags
48:38
of chemistry walking around, talking to
48:40
each other at the end of
48:42
the day. That's what life is,
48:45
just chemistry, essentially. And I think
48:47
he kind of taught himself what
48:49
he needed to know to kind
48:51
of do the examination of the
48:53
data. It's neat that someone outside
48:56
the discipline, I think, paid attention
48:58
to it and is perhaps drawing
49:00
attention to these things. And it's
49:02
maybe because he is outside of
49:05
the discipline, it's a perfectly normal
49:07
thing that he just doesn't have
49:09
that knowledge to explain, or maybe
49:11
it's just a new set of
49:14
eyes looking at something from a
49:16
different direction that you wouldn't get
49:18
from the inside of that discipline.
49:20
Yeah, so what Haskell has done
49:22
there is he's raised a really
49:25
interesting question, but it's a question
49:27
that simply cannot be answered with
49:29
the data that we currently have.
49:31
The data might be available for
49:34
a couple of these previous studies,
49:36
right? So does the data do
49:38
you any good or do you
49:40
actually need the physical samples to
49:43
make any progress on this? Well,
49:45
the data in one sense, the
49:47
data could do some good. And
49:49
I think that one of the
49:52
things, though, is that I found
49:54
that the data just simply isn't
49:56
there. There are, on the one
49:58
hand, I've found that there are
50:00
far more rumors of genetic analyses
50:03
having been done. In fact, overwhelming...
50:05
more rumors of genetic
50:11
analysis.
50:13
And
50:16
so
50:18
when
50:21
someone
50:25
says, oh,
50:28
You know, my friend had a sample
50:30
tested and it came back from
50:32
the lab as such and such.
50:34
I've said, okay, well, who's your
50:36
friend? May I contact them? And
50:39
again and again, what I find
50:41
is I contact the friend and they
50:43
say, oh, no, that wasn't me, that
50:45
was my other friend. Like, oh,
50:47
okay, well, what's your other friend's
50:50
name? And then I go and I
50:52
talk with that person and they
50:54
say, No, no, no. I didn't
50:56
say that I had a genetic
50:59
analysis done. I said the sample
51:01
that I found looked like the
51:03
sample that was featured
51:05
on one of the
51:07
Snellgrove Lake episodes of
51:10
Monster Quest where they got
51:12
such and such a result.
51:14
And I kid you not, half a
51:16
dozen of the specific
51:18
DNA analyses that
51:21
had been shared with me.
51:23
verbally, I traced back to
51:25
that one same episode of
51:27
Monster Quest, where someone was
51:30
talking about that, and it
51:32
had become misinterpreted in a
51:35
game of telephone. Again, I
51:37
think everybody trying to be
51:40
sincere, but everybody believing that
51:42
it was their friend who
51:44
had this sample tested.
51:47
I have found actually
51:49
very few instances where...
51:51
There was a sample
51:53
actually tested or somebody
51:55
claims that they actually
51:57
had it tested and.
51:59
far, far fewer than
52:01
that, where somebody can
52:04
actually show me a report. And
52:06
so they really don't seem to
52:08
be that many, practically
52:10
none, that have been done.
52:13
So that's really interesting.
52:15
They're really, we're operating
52:17
in a space where there isn't
52:19
really any data at all.
52:22
And so that's why we need
52:24
to produce some. Absolutely,
52:26
and will the data from this
52:28
study be shared publicly? Or how
52:30
will it be packaged up and
52:32
shared with other scientists even for
52:35
that matter? Yeah, definitely the data
52:37
from this study will be shared.
52:39
I'm not promising a particular timeline
52:42
for that. It depends on a
52:44
number of factors and certainly depends
52:46
on what we find as to
52:48
how we go about sharing that.
52:51
It will be public, yes, indeed.
52:53
Now, will the negative hits also be
52:55
made public? Like if you get a
52:57
black bear sample, the black bear sample
52:59
and that'll be put out as black
53:02
bear? Yes. Yes. To the public as
53:04
well as the sample owner? Yes, that's
53:06
the idea. Yeah, we're planning and we're
53:08
certainly planning publication out of this. You
53:11
know, what we obviously what we publish
53:13
and where we publish and when we
53:15
publish, all of these things are going
53:17
to depend on, you know, what are
53:20
the results that we are getting. There
53:22
are a... quite a few rather interesting
53:25
results that we could have.
53:27
There are lots of things
53:29
that can help push science
53:31
forward. I know that your
53:34
listenership is particularly
53:36
interested in what if we
53:39
were to find unknown species.
53:41
And in that case, I
53:43
think that's where it's particularly
53:46
interesting because in that case,
53:48
we probably would hold back.
53:51
at least for a while on
53:53
sharing publicly, we would certainly have
53:55
it peer-reviewed, but
53:57
from sharing publicly the
54:00
the sequencing, the sequence data
54:02
of a novel species, simply
54:04
because it wouldn't necessarily be
54:06
ethical to put that information
54:08
out there publicly until we
54:10
understood something about that species,
54:13
what are its numbers, what
54:15
are the factors that may
54:17
be impacting it, what it,
54:19
you know. What are its
54:21
social customs? All these sorts
54:23
of things, the culture, if
54:25
you will. Those things would
54:27
all be very important because
54:29
we don't know at this
54:31
page. I mean, what, we
54:33
don't have any idea. I
54:36
mean, I don't have any
54:38
idea whether such a species
54:40
even exists. If it does,
54:42
what is it like? Is
54:44
it endangered? Do we need
54:46
to make sure it's on
54:48
the endangered species list before
54:50
we release this information? What
54:52
are those steps? Need to
54:54
think very carefully because we
54:57
want to be ethical about
54:59
how we how we release
55:01
not the information that exists,
55:03
but release specific information that
55:05
could potentially be used to
55:07
threaten that species. a positive
55:09
and my God there's something
55:11
out there there's a thing
55:13
out there and it's it's
55:15
it's it's it's here on
55:17
the evolutionary tree and blah
55:20
blah blah you had all
55:22
this that that you had
55:24
all the stuff that DNA
55:26
could tell us about the
55:28
next step the actually the
55:30
actual the actual ecological study
55:32
of these things would Basically
55:34
have to start from ground
55:36
zero because of the dubious
55:38
nature of the vast majority
55:41
of the data that has
55:43
been gathered thus far. Even
55:45
citing reports. Now luckily there's
55:47
probably enough citing reports that
55:49
statistically you could probably do
55:51
some stuff and squeeze some
55:53
information out of it because
55:55
the outliers would separate themselves.
55:57
But between the the cultural...
55:59
and just the filters that
56:01
the investigators who listened to
56:04
these reports, who recorded these
56:06
reports, even their cultural perspective
56:08
and all that, comes into
56:10
play here. And this isn't
56:12
a, this is not an
56:14
objective database. There's a lot
56:16
of subjectivity in here, as
56:18
there would be with any
56:20
soft evidence like testimony. I
56:22
imagine a thorough ecological study
56:25
of these critters would be
56:27
five or 10 years down
56:29
the road. because scientists who've
56:31
been denying their existence for
56:33
so long know far less
56:35
than you know us amateur
56:37
investigators do at this point
56:39
even though much of probably
56:41
what the vast majority of
56:43
Bigfooters think will probably be
56:46
proven wrong at some point
56:48
you know or certainly vastly
56:50
refined are you suggesting that
56:52
if you did get a
56:54
hit that you think would
56:56
be strong enough to prove
56:58
the species at least to
57:00
geneticists who understand the sort
57:02
of data that it may
57:04
not be announced for until
57:06
an ecological study is done
57:09
on the animal which could
57:11
take five or ten years?
57:13
No, no, I'm not suggesting
57:15
that. What I'm suggesting though
57:17
is that if, for example,
57:19
such a thing were shown
57:21
and there were specific novel
57:23
DNA sequences that identified this
57:25
species versus any other species
57:27
on the planet, right? Which
57:30
is, that's what DNA does
57:32
when you have a, you
57:34
know, full genome, you have
57:36
a new species, you have
57:38
a known species. The problem
57:40
then is that releasing the
57:42
particular sequence could potentially put
57:44
it in the hands of
57:46
individuals or potentially, potentially corporations
57:48
to create primers specific for
57:50
that species that could. that
57:53
could enable the species to
57:55
be tracked down, hunted, so
57:57
on and so forth. So
57:59
again, we're talking, we're talking
58:01
a what if scenario here, but
58:03
it wouldn't be responsible
58:05
of me to, even if I think
58:08
that there's a very small chance of
58:10
that happening, it wouldn't be responsible of
58:12
me to not think a little bit
58:14
down the road about that. But even
58:17
thinking about the, just think,
58:19
for example, about the pharmaceutical
58:21
industry. Well, the pharmaceutical
58:24
companies might be competing with one
58:26
another to try to track down. such
58:28
a species that would
58:30
presumably be closely related
58:33
to human because of
58:35
the possible discoveries
58:37
to be to be had there and
58:39
the possible money. Yeah, so
58:41
I think the bottom line is
58:43
just that, you know, not the
58:46
not the fact of a new
58:48
species, but specific information about that
58:50
species that could that could
58:53
compromise a population. That's
58:55
the information that
58:57
might need to be kept
58:59
very close to the
59:01
vest for a while. What's
59:03
interesting to
59:05
me about this is the
59:08
range of folks who are
59:10
really excited about this study.
59:13
And I get it. I
59:15
know that some people are
59:18
using the term historic, and
59:20
I have to confess, yes,
59:22
it really is. To have...
59:24
The team that's working on
59:27
this to have these things
59:29
looked at in a scientifically
59:32
credible way, that this is
59:34
not just we're going to look
59:36
at 8 or 12 or 15
59:38
samples and call it a day. It
59:40
is historic. I find
59:42
support on the one hand
59:44
from folks in the
59:46
Bigfoot community who are
59:49
convinced that this will be
59:51
the study that's finally going
59:53
to prove. Bigfoot as
59:55
a species. I also have folks
59:58
who are more skeptical. in
1:00:00
their ideas who think, ah, this
1:00:02
is finally going to be the
1:00:04
study that looks at lots and
1:00:06
lots of the quote unquote best
1:00:08
samples and figures out that they're
1:00:10
all known species and maybe this
1:00:12
Bigfoot thing will go away. I
1:00:14
have people who say, oh, this
1:00:16
is gonna find that Bigfoot is
1:00:18
descended from the Gibbon family. I
1:00:20
have others who say, aha, this
1:00:22
is what's finally gonna show that
1:00:24
it's. descended from paranthropists. Everybody has
1:00:26
their ideas, everybody has their thoughts
1:00:28
as to what we'll find. I'm
1:00:30
just really curious. I'm just really
1:00:32
curious to see what do we
1:00:34
have here? And it will be
1:00:36
what it is. Yeah, to summarize
1:00:38
John Green into Hindon, something is
1:00:40
making these footprints. And it sounds
1:00:42
to me like you're just on
1:00:44
a quest to find out what
1:00:46
it is, except now we're living
1:00:48
in a more modern age. It's
1:00:50
no longer just footprints. It's actually
1:00:52
DNA and biological material. So fantastic.
1:00:54
I'm excited. Well Darby thank you
1:00:56
so much for coming on and
1:00:58
sharing about the project this is
1:01:00
an exciting thing I think any
1:01:03
big footer who is looking at
1:01:05
the goalpost here and wondering can
1:01:07
we can we move the ball
1:01:09
well I shouldn't use sports metaphors
1:01:11
you know me but man any
1:01:13
big footer that wants want some
1:01:15
sort of resolution to this should
1:01:17
be excited about this project. And
1:01:19
I can't do anything else, but
1:01:21
wish you the best of luck,
1:01:23
but I'll tell you, I'm going
1:01:25
to put my boots on the
1:01:27
ground in my best areas, and
1:01:29
I'm going to put the word
1:01:31
out to all the best researchers
1:01:33
I personally know to participate in
1:01:35
this. And of course, it is
1:01:37
open to anybody, if you have
1:01:39
bigfoots on the property, this is
1:01:41
an opportunity for you to participate
1:01:43
as well. Who knows? It could
1:01:45
be your sample to actually win
1:01:47
the game here. So, the links
1:01:49
are in the show notes in
1:01:51
the places you can submit. samples,
1:01:53
like if you want to fill
1:01:55
out the entry form for Darby's
1:01:57
project, the link is in the
1:01:59
show notes, and also something we
1:02:01
didn't mention, this is an ongoing
1:02:03
project. I mean, Bart Darby said
1:02:05
it several times, there is no,
1:02:07
there's not necessarily a budget that
1:02:09
is going to run out here,
1:02:11
but it's not like they couldn't
1:02:13
use a little extra money either.
1:02:15
And there is a donation form.
1:02:17
We'll put that link in the
1:02:19
show notes as well. If you
1:02:21
want to throw 50 bucks or
1:02:24
5 bucks even, how about a
1:02:26
dollar? Anything towards it, it goes
1:02:28
straight to the university and this
1:02:30
study, is that a way people
1:02:32
can help out and participate? the
1:02:34
North Carolina State University and they
1:02:36
are earmarked for my research area
1:02:38
as you'll see online. Fantastic. We're
1:02:40
not shaking our tin cup but
1:02:42
some people out there may not
1:02:44
have samples or may have no
1:02:46
hope of getting any and maybe
1:02:48
they want to participate in some
1:02:50
other way as well. So there
1:02:52
is that. Any final thoughts Darby
1:02:54
before we get going? Well I
1:02:56
really appreciate you all getting the
1:02:58
word out. I think that we
1:03:00
you know folks need to understand
1:03:02
that we We can't find what
1:03:04
we're not offered samples of. So
1:03:06
this is the opportunity for those
1:03:08
who feel like they have something
1:03:10
compelling. This is the opportunity to
1:03:12
present it. This is the opportunity
1:03:14
to find out what it is.
1:03:16
Bottom line, this is the opportunity.
1:03:18
And it depends on you. Thank
1:03:20
you very much, Darby. Really appreciate
1:03:22
your expertise, your talents, and the
1:03:24
opportunity to maybe get this thing
1:03:26
done. And thank you so much
1:03:28
for coming on the podcast. Yeah,
1:03:30
thanks for what you're doing. Thanks
1:03:32
for coming on, Sharon. All right,
1:03:34
folks, you heard here. We got
1:03:36
Darby Arca from NC State University.
1:03:38
They're on the case, so let's
1:03:40
get them some samples. And until
1:03:42
next week, y'all know what to
1:03:45
do. Keep it squauchy. Thanks
1:03:49
for listening to this week's episode of Bigfoot and
1:03:51
Beyond. If you liked what you heard, please rate
1:03:54
and review us on iTunes. Subscribe to Bigfoot and
1:03:56
Beyond wherever you get your podcast. and follow
1:03:58
us on Facebook and Instagram
1:04:00
at Big Foot at Bigfoot and
1:04:02
Beyond You can find us
1:04:05
on Twitter us on Twitter at Bigfoot
1:04:07
that's an N in
1:04:09
the middle, an and the us
1:04:11
your thoughts and questions
1:04:13
with thoughts and questions with the hashtag Bigfoot and
1:04:16
beyond.
Podchaser is the ultimate destination for podcast data, search, and discovery. Learn More