CLASSICS (Bonus Episode) - Darby Orcutt & 'Squatchy Science!

CLASSICS (Bonus Episode) - Darby Orcutt & 'Squatchy Science!

Released Friday, 11th April 2025
Good episode? Give it some love!
CLASSICS (Bonus Episode) - Darby Orcutt & 'Squatchy Science!

CLASSICS (Bonus Episode) - Darby Orcutt & 'Squatchy Science!

CLASSICS (Bonus Episode) - Darby Orcutt & 'Squatchy Science!

CLASSICS (Bonus Episode) - Darby Orcutt & 'Squatchy Science!

Friday, 11th April 2025
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

Big foot and be

0:02

on with Cliff and

0:05

Bobo. These guys are

0:07

your favorites. So like

0:10

you're subscribing rated. Five

0:12

stars. Big foot and

0:15

be on. The greatest

0:18

podcast. Wherever you're listening

0:20

or watching. James

0:29

Bobofay. Hey, Bob's, what's happening? Hey,

0:31

Clint, how much how's it going? It's going

0:33

all right. Just another beautiful day hanging out,

0:36

gonna talk to a great guest today about

0:38

a great subject. I'm kind of excited about

0:40

it. But we should probably let Darby talk

0:42

about that. So why don't we just bring

0:45

Darby on? We'll start talking. How are you?

0:47

Hi, I'm doing well. How are you? Good. Doing

0:49

right, pushing forward. So thank you so

0:51

much for coming on. You have such

0:53

an exciting project here, but before we

0:55

get going on that, I was looking

0:57

up, I was going to try to

0:59

familiarize myself with your biography. And the

1:01

nearest, I mean, correct me if I'm

1:04

wrong, but the nearest, I can figure

1:06

is, you're kind of, you're a library

1:08

and some sort, you're a research of

1:10

a variety of subjects, and it seems

1:12

like you're a facilitator, more than, say,

1:14

say, an instructor, or a good way

1:16

to categorize you. Well, I am an

1:19

instructor as well. Yeah, I'm

1:21

on faculty at North Carolina

1:23

State University. My primary faculty

1:26

appointment is as a

1:28

librarian, but as you know, Cliff,

1:30

not doing the sorts of things

1:32

that most people think of when

1:34

you think of a librarian, but

1:36

also I do teach and I

1:39

am very much involved with research.

1:41

And my work is a

1:43

librarian, especially for many, many

1:45

years. My job has been to

1:47

help put together and support highly

1:49

interdisciplinary research teams

1:52

to tackle really tough questions. So

1:54

to put together the right people

1:57

and to help translate between fields.

1:59

in such a way that people

2:01

can deal with real-world

2:04

issues that require expertise

2:06

that's coming from a

2:08

number of different areas. Now

2:10

is this, is this, is your

2:13

role here needed because the

2:15

disciplines don't tend to speak

2:17

to one another? That is

2:20

sometimes the case. Experts in

2:22

different disciplines oftentimes either don't

2:24

know how to speak to

2:26

one another or sometimes don't

2:29

realize that they're asking questions

2:31

along similar lines or about

2:33

similar subjects in ways where

2:35

they could be working together

2:37

and getting further that way.

2:40

Sometimes they don't know how

2:42

to speak one another's language.

2:44

And so I play this

2:46

role of matchmaking and translating

2:48

and facilitating and facilitating. For

2:50

you, you must be kind of

2:53

familiar with a huge number of

2:55

disciplines then to even know what's

2:58

going on to any of them

3:00

and realize that there might be

3:02

connections that could be made between

3:05

them. Yeah, very much so. In

3:07

fact, I'm usually the person in

3:09

the room who knows the most

3:12

about everything and the least about

3:14

any given subject. I am also

3:16

one of those people, which is

3:18

why I became an elementary school

3:21

teacher. I was pretty good at

3:23

everything, but not really great at

3:25

anything. In your career path

3:27

or in your personal life,

3:29

because obviously these things intersect

3:31

on a regular basis for

3:33

everyone, what led you down the

3:35

path to Saswatch? Well, I had been

3:38

teaching courses for a number

3:40

of years and particularly been

3:42

teaching courses about... how science works,

3:44

not just the sorts of things

3:47

that say you might have taught

3:49

your students in elementary school

3:51

of how science says it

3:54

works, no hypothesis and testing

3:56

and experimentation, those sorts of

3:59

things. but also culturally

4:01

speaking, how science works,

4:03

what are the economic structures

4:06

that underlie scientific research, what

4:08

are the ways that people

4:11

communicate with one another, how

4:13

do scientists communicate with the

4:15

general public as well as

4:18

with one another, with funding

4:20

agencies, with government, with industry,

4:23

and how science's scientific

4:26

discoveries are applied. in

4:28

the real world. So there's

4:30

a lot that goes into

4:32

science that oftentimes even scientists

4:35

are not fully aware of

4:37

themselves. And so I've been

4:39

teaching classes on that for a

4:41

great many years. And I

4:44

came upon this idea that

4:46

I thought would be really

4:48

interesting of looking at fringe

4:50

sciences or people doing scientific

4:53

work in areas that are oftentimes

4:55

not considered. science that

4:57

might be might be

4:59

referred to as pseudo science or

5:02

dismissed in that way. And I

5:04

thought it would be very interesting

5:06

to teach a course, and this

5:09

is what I started doing, that

5:11

looked at some of these areas

5:13

and asked these questions

5:16

of why are these

5:18

subjects dismissed by quote

5:20

unquote mainstream science or

5:22

the general public? Are

5:24

they being dismissed for

5:26

good reason? What can that

5:28

teach us about the ways

5:30

that we make decisions, the

5:32

ways that cultural aspects play

5:35

into our scientific questions and

5:37

practices? And so I started

5:39

engaging with some of

5:41

these fields, including the

5:43

field of Sasquatch Research

5:45

or Sasquatch Investigation. And

5:47

it was very, very

5:49

interesting to me, and one of

5:52

the things that I really wanted to

5:54

do with my students. was to challenge

5:56

them to look at the very best

5:59

evidence. in each of these

6:01

areas. Because as you know, there

6:03

are lots of people making

6:06

lots of claims and you

6:08

can find lots of silly

6:10

things in any of

6:12

these fringe or paranormal

6:14

or edge science fields.

6:16

You can find plenty of

6:18

things to make fun of,

6:20

but it's much more challenging

6:23

to deal with the best

6:25

potential evidence in these areas.

6:28

and see what it says

6:30

because any potential evidence is

6:32

actually evidence of something. So the

6:35

question is really, let's figure out

6:37

what it is. Do you have

6:39

a list of the best evidence

6:42

that you're going to focus on

6:44

right now? Well, you know, I

6:46

think that there are a number

6:48

of areas where there are interesting

6:50

questions to be asked

6:52

with regard to potential

6:55

Sasquatch investigation. In my current

6:57

study, the one that I'm leading

6:59

with my colleagues now that we're

7:02

talking about today, I really

7:04

wanted to look at

7:06

the physical biological evidence

7:08

that could potentially yield

7:10

DNA. And the reason for

7:12

that is that DNA is

7:15

really the gold standard for

7:17

the establishment of a species.

7:19

And that is, of course, one

7:22

of the big questions or one

7:24

of the big hypotheses. that the

7:27

saucewatch phenomenon could

7:29

potentially be explained,

7:31

at least in large part, by

7:34

a unique biological species. You

7:36

don't know, unless you look

7:38

at the evidence, unless

7:40

you examine the evidence, what

7:43

it is. And that's something

7:45

that I found, was that

7:47

there were people who were

7:49

finding hairs, that were people

7:51

who were finding other sorts

7:54

of physical samples, and... They

7:56

were asking me, where can I send to

7:58

have this tested? How do I... How do

8:00

I go about that? And I really

8:02

found that I couldn't find

8:05

a commercial lab or a university

8:07

or anywhere really that I

8:09

could really feel confident saying,

8:11

yeah, this is the place to

8:14

send this if you have a

8:16

potential unknown. And so that's

8:19

why I got together a team

8:21

and created that myself with

8:23

a study. So this is

8:25

a university-sponsored study. Is

8:28

that correct? Well, it's

8:30

university sponsored in

8:33

the sense that most

8:35

studies are university sponsored.

8:37

What that means really

8:39

is that in this

8:42

case, there are four

8:44

of us, faculty colleagues,

8:46

who are collaborating on

8:48

this and who have

8:50

set the scope and

8:53

the parameters of the study.

8:55

And we are all. on faculty

8:57

at NC State University.

9:00

So it's university sponsored

9:02

in that sense. The

9:04

other thing is that it

9:07

is an official study, and

9:09

so I've gone through the

9:11

paperwork. I had to

9:13

go through what we

9:15

call institutional review board

9:18

approval, which is our

9:20

ethics committee that makes

9:22

sure that when we do

9:24

any research. that we are treating

9:26

both humans and animals with

9:28

respect and care and properly

9:31

ethically. So you had to

9:33

fill out a probably a

9:35

mountain of paperwork for this

9:37

thing and then somehow still squeeze

9:39

it back, or squeeze it through

9:41

rather, knowing that this is kind

9:43

of a Sasquatch study, but you

9:45

couldn't put Sasquatch in the forefront,

9:48

right? So how did you get

9:50

around that? You know, this is and

9:52

this isn't. a Sasquatch study. And what

9:54

we're really interested in in this case,

9:56

and this is what the name of

9:59

the study is, It's a

10:01

study of allegedly

10:04

morphologically anomalous physical

10:06

samples. So what does that

10:09

mean? Basically that means

10:11

this is a study

10:13

of biological specimens that

10:16

people have found that

10:18

they claim have

10:20

unusual traits or

10:22

unusual appearances or

10:24

something unusual about them.

10:26

And that is what we're

10:29

studying. This comes from

10:31

my own hearing and

10:33

talking with folks, particularly

10:36

in the Bigfoot community,

10:38

who have found samples,

10:41

for example, found

10:43

hair samples, that folks

10:45

who are actually very

10:48

good at identifying hair

10:50

of known species in

10:52

North America cannot identify.

10:55

There are certain properties

10:57

to these hairs that

11:00

they reportedly have in

11:02

common, and yet they don't

11:04

look like those of known

11:06

species. That's an interesting

11:09

question. What are these?

11:11

So these are the types

11:13

of questions that we're trying

11:15

to answer. What are these things?

11:18

Every physical sample

11:20

that will be submitted

11:22

to us is a sample of

11:24

something. We don't know what it

11:26

is unless we look at it. Why

11:29

wouldn't we want to look at these?

11:31

Stay tuned for more Bigfoot and

11:33

Beyond with Cliffin Bobo. We'll

11:36

be right back after these

11:38

messages. So three to six months

11:40

doesn't seem like a long time,

11:42

right? In Bigfoot Land, it's not

11:45

very long because we've been looking

11:47

for Sasquatches for decades and decades

11:49

and decades, but... What do you

11:51

think about what can happen in

11:53

three to six months? Like what

11:55

would you do in three to

11:57

six months? I'm giving you six months.

11:59

Go. Squaw. Squat, of course, of course,

12:01

and how much do you think

12:03

is going to get done during

12:05

that time in Squat, are you going

12:08

to see one in six months?

12:10

Probably not. But in three to

12:12

six months, what you can see,

12:14

if you're not going to see

12:17

a Sasquatch, is thicker, fuller hair.

12:19

Regrown from hymns, our sponsor,

12:21

hymns, not bad for just

12:23

three to six months. Yeah, because

12:25

hymns will give you a convenient access

12:28

to a range of hair-lost treatments that

12:30

work, all from the comfort of your couch.

12:32

They make it pretty simple, man. They

12:34

use real doctors. It's clinically proven ingredients.

12:37

They use like finesteride and monoxibil. And

12:39

I love that idea. No doctor visits,

12:41

because first of all, doctors, who wants

12:43

to go see a doctor, nothing personal,

12:45

nothing personal, personal doctors. But also you

12:47

make an appointment now, you get in

12:50

August or something, or something, forget or

12:52

something. and I know you have access

12:54

to the internet because you're listening

12:56

to us right now. You can pick how

12:58

you want to do it like chewables, orals,

13:01

spray, serum treatments, whatever you want. I mean

13:03

they can work with you and customize it

13:05

to your preferences. Yeah, like it could

13:07

be a one-time thing, it could be

13:09

ongoing care. No insurance is needed at

13:11

all and it's all at one low

13:13

price and that covers everything from these

13:15

treatments to the ongoing care if you

13:17

so choose. Well they always said

13:20

the proofs and the pudin

13:22

and there's hundreds of thousands

13:24

of subscribers that trust it and

13:27

you know give it positive review

13:29

so there you go there's your pudin.

13:31

Thick hairy pudding. Search your

13:33

free online visit today

13:36

at hymns.com/beyond for

13:38

your personalized hair

13:40

loss treatment options.

13:42

hymns.com/beyond. Results vary based on

13:44

studies of topical and oral monoxidil

13:47

and finasteride. Prescription products require an

13:49

online consultation with a health care

13:51

provider who will determine if prescription

13:53

is appropriate. Restrictions apply. See website

13:55

for full details and important safety

13:57

information. So at this point what we've done

13:59

we just launched the online intake

14:02

survey at the tail end

14:04

of May. So we started,

14:06

we've had a trickle of

14:08

offers coming in thus far.

14:11

It's just started to

14:13

pick up over the last

14:15

couple of weeks. So at

14:17

this stage, as of this

14:20

recording, I am starting to

14:22

reach out to those folks, starting

14:25

to arrange brief interviews

14:27

with them. I have some

14:29

questions to learn and make

14:32

sure that we understand a

14:34

bit more about the samples

14:36

and the circumstances under which

14:39

they were found. And then

14:41

at that point, we'll be

14:43

giving those folks instructions as

14:45

to how to physically submit

14:47

the samples that we're

14:50

prioritizing at this time.

14:52

So I imagine that certainly by...

14:54

early September or so, I imagine

14:56

we will have the first samples

14:59

in hand on campus and in

15:01

the lab. So let's think about

15:03

it from a user perspective first and

15:06

then we'll talk about it from

15:08

the more the mechanical side, I

15:10

guess your side of things. So

15:12

if someone like say I found

15:14

Saswatch footprints and they went by

15:16

a tree and there were a

15:18

number of strange hairs on the

15:21

tree and I collected them. Now,

15:23

of course, I'm perhaps slightly better

15:25

equipped than your average Joe in

15:27

the forest. I would put these

15:29

underneath the microscope and take a

15:31

look at him. If I couldn't

15:34

easily identify them as bear or deer

15:36

or anything like that, and I wanted to

15:38

submit them, what would I do? Well, what

15:40

you would do is you would go to

15:42

the intake survey form that's online. Okay, and

15:44

by the way, we will have that, we'll

15:47

have that link in the show notes below.

15:49

We'll have that link in the show notes

15:51

below. Thank you very much for that.

15:53

I appreciate y'all getting the word out.

15:55

So the first thing you would do

15:57

is fill out. Well, I mean, the

15:59

first. thing you would do obviously

16:02

is collect the sample and

16:04

folks asked me hey what's

16:06

the best way to collect

16:08

the sample and there are

16:11

certainly lots of folks out

16:13

there who who describe good

16:15

techniques for sample collection but

16:17

the most important things to

16:20

do are to try to

16:22

collect the sample if you

16:24

can in a sterile way

16:26

so with sterile gloves or

16:29

sterile instruments most samples if

16:31

they're hair or something like

16:33

that moist wet, you know,

16:35

like blood or something, but

16:37

hairs, most of the samples

16:40

that folks are offering this

16:42

far, it's best to collect

16:44

those into a paper envelope

16:46

and stick them in the

16:49

refrigerator in the freezer. So

16:51

that's the best way to

16:53

store samples, collect and store

16:55

samples. So then you would

16:58

fill out the online survey,

17:00

at which point... Like I

17:02

say, I'll be reaching out

17:04

to people to then schedule

17:07

a brief interview by phone

17:09

or on the computer or

17:11

what have you. And at

17:13

that point, we'll be asking

17:16

folks, we'll be identifying which

17:18

samples we want to get

17:20

in some priority order. And

17:22

we'll be testing those. Anyone

17:25

who submits a sample in

17:27

that form can choose whether

17:29

they want to be identified.

17:31

publicly identified with their sample

17:34

or whether they want to

17:36

be completely anonymous because some

17:38

people want to be identified

17:40

and some people just want

17:42

to know what something is

17:45

and they don't want anybody

17:47

to know that they had

17:49

anything to do with Sasquatch.

17:51

Either one is fine. Under

17:54

no circumstances will we be

17:56

disclosing specific locations where samples

17:58

were samples were acquired. Just.

18:00

will be giving general locations,

18:03

but everybody who submits

18:05

a sample will receive

18:07

the results for their

18:09

sample. And what sort

18:11

of timeline would that be?

18:14

Well, here's the thing, Cliff.

18:16

Real science is not a

18:18

speedy process. So I'm

18:20

trying not to promise. a

18:22

specific timeline to folks, other

18:24

than we'll do it as well,

18:27

we will do these things as

18:29

quickly as we can, but that

18:32

could depend on a large

18:34

number of factors. So if

18:36

the sample, if the sample

18:38

that's submitted is something that

18:41

we come in, we look

18:43

at it, we run a test, and

18:45

oh, it pops up. Oh, this

18:48

is a black bear and it's

18:50

very clearly black bear and you

18:52

know, you may get a response

18:54

in a matter of a couple

18:56

of months. If it's a sample

18:58

that is more degraded

19:00

that we have or

19:02

having trouble pulling sequencing

19:05

information out of, we may be

19:07

working with it for a while. It

19:09

really just depends on

19:11

so many things that are

19:13

beyond. our control or the

19:16

people submitting these things as

19:18

to how long it can take.

19:20

But I do promise you,

19:22

you will receive a response.

19:24

You will receive a report

19:26

for your sample. Yeah, you

19:28

would have to be

19:30

extraordinarily careful, of course,

19:33

because if you say, for

19:35

example, it happened, a Sasquatch

19:37

hair is submitted, it's in

19:39

good shape. passes the microscopic

19:41

analysis and the DNA comes

19:43

back is what the hell

19:45

is this, right? Even then

19:47

it would have to be

19:49

tested and retested because a

19:51

false positive in this in

19:53

this particular scenario could sink

19:55

everything. They could just make

19:57

the subject and the study

19:59

just a laughing stock. So

20:01

you'd have to be

20:03

extraordinarily careful and just

20:05

very, very conservative in

20:07

how you assess the

20:10

samples, especially if it seems that

20:12

we're on to the right thing

20:14

for the first time. Well, absolutely.

20:16

And if that is a finding

20:18

of the study, that's one of

20:21

the reasons that I have

20:23

recruited the team that I

20:25

have, because these folks are

20:27

Absolutely tremendous. And this team

20:30

is when you receive a

20:32

report, you're going to know

20:34

that that's what it is.

20:36

If what you said, if

20:39

there is a Sasquatch out

20:41

there to be found and

20:43

there are samples submitted,

20:45

and we find that, well, this

20:47

team is going to, you know,

20:49

very carefully document

20:52

that and present

20:54

that. Again, if that's if

20:56

that's the finding. You know, at

20:58

this stage we haven't looked

21:01

at any samples. The salient

21:03

point here is that we

21:05

don't know what the samples

21:07

that we'll be receiving are

21:10

until we look at them. And

21:12

that's what makes this really

21:14

exciting is that there are

21:16

a lot of rather interesting

21:19

sounding samples out

21:21

there that have not been looked

21:23

at. by a credible

21:25

scientific team. And so

21:28

I'm really excited to see

21:30

what our results are.

21:32

So you said there's four

21:34

of you on the team?

21:37

Who are there three people?

21:39

Well, there are four of

21:41

us on the core team,

21:44

myself, and then two geneticists,

21:46

genomists, and one microscopist. So,

21:49

microscope expert, think

21:51

of that. And so this

21:53

is the core team and

21:55

this team also has

21:57

a lot of expertise. in

22:00

forensics and wildlife forensics as

22:02

well. So this is the

22:04

core team, but the fun

22:06

thing about this is is

22:08

I also have a whole

22:10

host of experts in particular

22:12

areas who are ready to

22:14

come on and join us

22:16

if and when we find

22:18

an interesting specimen in their

22:20

area of expertise. And these

22:22

are these are faculty researchers

22:24

not just from NC State,

22:26

but from across the country.

22:28

So for example, I have

22:30

someone on board who is

22:32

an physical anthropologist and an

22:34

expert in all things osteo.

22:36

So if a bone or

22:38

tooth sample that is interesting

22:41

is submitted for this study,

22:43

she is ready to jump

22:45

on board. and get involved

22:47

and really excited about it.

22:49

But again, if we don't

22:51

have something in her area

22:53

of expertise, well, she's going

22:55

to stay in the Bigfoot

22:57

closet for now. For that

22:59

genome, for doing the full

23:01

genome, yeah, you always hear

23:03

stuff like last time we

23:05

talked to Melzier, we said

23:07

about 430,000, 440,000 for a

23:09

full genome sequencing. Is that

23:11

like, is going to ask

23:13

about your budget, like how

23:15

it got approved, like, how

23:17

much they give you give

23:19

you and then? Like when

23:21

you hear those high numbers,

23:23

is that going to be

23:25

cut way down since these

23:27

guys are on faculty, staff,

23:29

like they're already paid through

23:31

the university? How does that

23:33

work? I'm sorry, but specifically,

23:35

Meldrum said in the six-figure

23:37

range, and most of that

23:40

would be for personnel. Right.

23:42

And so that is probably

23:44

pretty reasonable to expect if

23:46

you were going to go

23:48

out and hire and hire

23:50

the work done by someone

23:52

who might know what they're

23:54

doing. Now in this case...

23:56

since we have the we

23:58

have the machines on campus

24:00

that are available. to us

24:02

where we can do nuclear

24:04

DNA sequencing for those samples

24:06

that may warrant it, then

24:08

you know we have an

24:11

in-house price schedule that looks

24:13

quite a bit different than

24:15

that to be honest. And

24:18

because my colleagues are the

24:20

ones with the expertise not

24:22

having to hire my colleagues

24:25

to work on our own

24:27

study, that actually reduces the

24:29

cost dramatically. How many do you

24:31

think you could run like for the,

24:34

with your budget you have for

24:36

the year, like how many samples

24:38

do you run like 20 or 50

24:40

or? Well, see this is the thing,

24:42

we don't have a set budget. for

24:45

this project. And the reason

24:47

we don't is that there

24:49

are quite a few different

24:51

needs and quite a few

24:53

different goals that this project

24:56

can potentially help us to achieve.

24:58

One of those that's

25:00

really important is training

25:02

of PhD students in

25:04

genetics. So our PhD

25:07

students in genetics who

25:09

work in the labs of my

25:11

two colleagues. who are collaborators,

25:13

they need to teach these

25:15

students anyhow. And they see

25:18

this as a really interesting

25:20

way of having the students

25:22

work with true unknowns or

25:24

with unusual samples. And so

25:26

this is a great way

25:29

for them to be able

25:31

to train their PhD students,

25:33

like I say, which they

25:35

need to do anyway, right?

25:37

They need to be studying

25:40

samples of something. So why

25:42

not? The samples that

25:44

are associated with

25:46

this study. In addition,

25:48

the team we've talked

25:50

about ideas that might

25:53

help push forensic understanding

25:56

forward so that might

25:58

help us. develop new

26:00

forensic techniques that could be

26:03

useful. Again, not just for

26:05

this study or for this

26:07

topic, but could be very

26:09

useful in the field for

26:11

all kinds of things. And

26:13

these are just a couple

26:15

of examples. There are some

26:18

other things that some other

26:20

outcomes that we are imagining

26:22

from the types of samples

26:24

that we'll be looking at.

26:26

So a lot of the

26:28

costs of the study are

26:31

costs that are already being

26:33

born anyhow. If you have

26:35

to, you know, if you

26:37

have to have a PhD

26:39

student studying a sample of

26:41

something, well, it's going to

26:43

be the same cost whether

26:46

studying a sample from this

26:48

study or a sample from

26:50

some other study, right? How

26:52

hard of a cell is

26:54

it to the university to

26:56

fund this? Yeah, well, it's

26:59

interesting. I mean, again, it's

27:01

not a funding issue, per

27:03

se, because the funding is

27:05

the funding that is already

27:07

happening through the labs. Like

27:09

the institutional reputation, are they

27:11

worried about that? There is

27:14

not really a concern with

27:16

institutional reputation because of the

27:18

way that we are framing

27:20

the study. Again, because we're

27:22

looking at this, we're looking

27:24

at samples, we're looking at

27:27

specimens that are something, right?

27:29

And it's a matter of

27:31

figuring out what they are.

27:33

We are not going out

27:35

there. This is not a

27:37

study about looking for Bigfoot.

27:39

This is a study of

27:42

looking at seemingly unusual physical

27:44

samples to determine what they

27:46

are. Now, we know that

27:48

a number of these samples

27:50

are coming from the Bigfoot

27:52

community. They are the result

27:55

of folks having an experience.

27:57

They feel is a softwatch

27:59

and then finding. these specimens

28:01

in connection with that? Well,

28:03

there's still interesting specimens. So

28:05

we certainly don't want to

28:07

be prejudiced against those specimens.

28:10

In fact, I think if

28:12

anything, the experiences that people

28:14

are having in conjunction with

28:16

finding these types of samples

28:18

make them all the more

28:20

interesting to me for us

28:23

to take a look at.

28:26

Stay tuned for more Bigfoot and Beyond

28:28

with Clifin Bobo. We'll be right back

28:30

after these messages. So we talked a

28:32

little bit about the user side, like

28:34

someone like me who might get a

28:37

sample and want to submit it. We

28:39

talked a little bit about the budget

28:41

and how you got this. past the

28:43

gatekeepers at the university and basically how

28:45

you sold the sold the idea which

28:47

is brilliant of course then it is

28:49

fantastic because working with unknowns that could

28:52

yield a real fantastic mind altering discovery

28:54

is just brilliant but what does it

28:56

look like from your side like your

28:58

team when after I fill out I

29:00

fill out the computer survey thing, my

29:02

name, this and that, whatever, I still

29:04

have the sample, you get that email,

29:07

what happens in, from that, and we'll

29:09

just use an example of a hair,

29:11

because obviously if you have a bone

29:13

or a flesh or blood tissue sample,

29:15

it would be probably a slightly different

29:17

process, but we'll just deal with hair

29:19

for now, because that's probably... I'm guessing

29:22

that'll probably be what the majority of

29:24

the samples you get are, but hair

29:26

samples. So what would that look like

29:28

from your end, from when you receive

29:30

that email to when, I don't know,

29:32

when you put the sample down, I

29:34

guess, I don't know. Well, again, we

29:37

have to prioritize the samples that we're

29:39

looking at. How would you prioritize something

29:41

like that? We don't have a. Folks

29:43

ask me, you know, what's your formula?

29:45

We don't have a set formula for

29:47

that. But certainly what we're interested in

29:49

is we're interested in looking at.

29:52

First of all, we're

29:54

interested in looking

29:56

at samples that appear

29:58

to be the

30:00

most interesting and that

30:02

appear to be

30:04

the freshest, most likely

30:07

to yield genetic

30:09

information. For example, if

30:11

you were to

30:13

offer us a hair

30:15

that you found

30:17

20 years ago and

30:19

it's been in

30:22

your garage, that's going

30:24

to be a

30:26

much lower priority than

30:28

someone who says,

30:30

hey, here's a hair

30:32

that I found

30:34

last week. And

30:36

by the way, I put it under a microscope

30:39

and it's got a little bit of flesh attached

30:41

to it. And it doesn't look like a known

30:43

animal. Sure, sure.

30:45

But now the user, the

30:47

users aren't going to be able

30:49

to determine that. That would

30:51

have to be, I mean, on

30:53

your end, someone like me

30:55

or something. Yeah, sure. I've got

30:57

a microscope. I'm that level

30:59

of nerd. But the average Joe,

31:01

who may have seen a

31:03

Sasquatch and wanted to submit a

31:05

sample, they wouldn't know that.

31:07

So you just get a hair

31:09

and a story, essentially, on

31:11

an email. No, absolutely. And to

31:13

give you an idea, so

31:15

there's a sample that was offered

31:17

to us that sounds interesting

31:19

because the person offering it reported

31:21

seeing a Sasquatch one night.

31:23

He said it was peeking out

31:25

at him from behind a

31:27

pine tree. He went back the

31:29

next morning and in the

31:31

sap of the pine tree found

31:33

a hair stuck there. And

31:35

so he collected that. And that

31:38

was just several months ago.

31:40

That to me is much more

31:42

interesting than, and this is

31:44

not one that was offered, but

31:46

this was something that somebody

31:48

talked with me about a couple

31:50

of years ago. Someone who

31:52

says, Hey, I have a clump

31:54

of hair that I think

31:56

is Sasquatch hair. And I say,

31:58

Well, what makes you think

32:00

it's Sasquatch hair? Why is it interesting to you?

32:02

They say, well, I found it stuck

32:04

15 feet up in a tree, and

32:06

what else would be 15 feet up

32:08

in a tree? Could it be the

32:11

hair of a saucequatch? Sure,

32:13

of course, you don't know until

32:15

you look at it, but of

32:17

course, there are plenty of other

32:19

things that climb trees, there is

32:22

wind, there are birds that use

32:24

clumps of hairs nesting material,

32:26

and they can... fall down, you

32:28

know, there are lots of ways

32:31

that hair could get 15 feet up

32:33

in a tree. So again, we have

32:35

to look at and it'll depend

32:37

on how many things are offered

32:39

to us, but we have to

32:41

be able to put these in

32:43

some sort of priority order because

32:45

we're not looking to just test

32:47

any and every. hair that someone finds

32:50

in the forest? Of course not. So

32:52

say you have a good sample. Say

32:54

this person who saw the Sasquatch and

32:56

there's a hair associated with it or

32:58

you know you get another sample that

33:00

seems very promising to you. What is

33:02

the process at that point? I'm assuming,

33:04

I mean I'm just guessing here to

33:06

you'd probably put under a microscope first

33:08

talk to your microscope dude or a

33:10

gal and then move forward from there.

33:13

What actually does happen after that point?

33:15

Absolutely. Well, the first thing

33:17

is, obviously, yes, we will be

33:19

looking at it microscopically. If we

33:21

get a specimen in and it

33:23

looks, you know, like for example,

33:25

this this hair that was found

33:27

in the in the sap of

33:30

the pine tree, if we put

33:32

it under a microscope and it

33:34

looks exactly like the hair of a gray

33:36

squirrel, which are common to that

33:38

area and climb up trees all

33:40

the time, if it looks exactly

33:42

like that. It's a classic

33:45

presentation. Well, we've identified

33:47

it at that point. We don't

33:49

need to move on to genetic

33:51

analysis to say, that's a great

33:53

squirrel. But then, you know, what

33:55

we'll be doing is we'll be

33:57

looking at these things in stepwise.

34:00

a stepwise way throughout. So

34:02

we'll be looking first. We'll

34:04

be looking first at the

34:07

morphology. So again, the appearance

34:09

of these through the microscope,

34:11

seeing what is readily identified.

34:13

If we find things

34:15

that aren't readily identified

34:17

or that look somewhat

34:19

unusual or really unusual

34:22

or whatever it may

34:24

be, that's when we'll

34:26

move on to genetic analysis.

34:28

And we're going to proceed

34:30

with that very cautiously because

34:32

one of the things that

34:34

we don't want to do

34:36

is we don't want to

34:39

be destructive any more than

34:41

we need to be. We

34:43

don't want to be destructive

34:45

of the samples that we

34:47

receive. And this is one thing

34:49

that told me that I had

34:52

recruited the right team was at

34:54

our very first team meeting back

34:56

when It was one of my

34:59

colleagues who said, we need to

35:01

treat every sample that comes to

35:03

us as precious, because these

35:05

samples are going to be

35:07

precious to the people who

35:09

are submitting them. And I

35:12

was like, wow, I couldn't

35:14

have said that better myself.

35:16

And so we're going to

35:18

be proceeding very cautiously in

35:20

the least destructive ways possible.

35:23

And we're going to be, we will

35:25

go as far. with the analysis as

35:27

we need to go in order

35:29

to determine what the sample

35:31

is. So with a hair sample, we'll,

35:34

you know, certainly looking

35:36

at it microscopically first,

35:38

looking at the mitochondrial

35:40

DNA, and then again, if

35:43

warranted, taking that to the

35:45

new clear DNA, looking at

35:47

the full genome. So with hair samples,

35:49

and again, correct me if I'm wrong,

35:51

I don't know very much in general,

35:53

but there's no nuclear DNA, this just

35:55

mitochondrial DNA. Do you think that, unless

35:58

of course you have the follicle, if

36:00

I remember correctly. But let's

36:02

just say that you have

36:04

a partial hair sample and

36:07

it, could the mitochondrial

36:09

be enough to indicate

36:11

the presence of a

36:14

new species? Potentially, yes.

36:16

Absolutely. You know, I do think

36:18

that we would want to try

36:20

to have multiple samples,

36:23

certainly, where we would find

36:25

that. I do think that having

36:28

like one sample with

36:30

mitochondrial DNA was different

36:32

would be really really

36:34

interesting, but probably not

36:36

enough, you know, certainly

36:38

to establish a species.

36:40

You know, the interesting

36:42

thing is, is that, you

36:44

know, when people submit a

36:46

hair sample, oftentimes, yes, we

36:48

don't know, will that sample

36:50

have follicle in it? Will that

36:52

sample actually have a skin

36:54

cell still attached? that came out of

36:57

the body. And certainly the skin

36:59

cell would contain a complete

37:01

genetic sequence. So again, you're absolutely

37:03

right. Hair is generally not going

37:06

to give us a full genome,

37:08

but a sample that one thinks

37:11

of as hair could potentially do

37:13

so. And I guess this doesn't

37:15

come down to one sample at

37:17

the end of the day,

37:19

because this is science and

37:22

you want repeatability. And certainly...

37:24

Sasswatches are real animals and

37:26

people start sending you a

37:28

flood of hairs a very

37:30

small percentage of those hairs

37:32

will probably represent Sasquatch and

37:34

I'm assuming by comparing the

37:36

results of these anomalous samples

37:39

that's where the real the

37:41

real positive affirmation is really

37:43

going to lie is the

37:45

repeatability of the same novel

37:47

sequences. Is that correct? Absolutely. If

37:49

we just get one novel sequence...

37:51

Again, that's a really fascinating result.

37:54

That would be, that alone would

37:56

be enough to keep us looking

37:59

and looking and looking. perhaps, but

38:01

yes, it's the idea

38:03

of having multiple of

38:05

that. And so in

38:07

the ideal world, at

38:09

a minimum, I think,

38:11

if you're, if the idea

38:14

is, if you're asking

38:16

me, what would it

38:18

take to establish a

38:20

species, I think that

38:22

actually as few as,

38:24

say, three samples coming

38:27

from three different.

38:29

locations, but that

38:31

share the same novel sequence,

38:34

I would think that that

38:36

in and of itself,

38:38

something as simple as

38:40

that, simple, ha ha, right? Something

38:43

is, you know, as little

38:45

as three samples could could be

38:47

enough to essentially prove species.

38:49

Fantastic. Now, one of the things

38:51

that we spoke about when

38:53

we were speaking in person in

38:56

Tennessee, where I met you face

38:58

to face, something I believe that

39:01

sets the study apart from the

39:03

many, many other studies that

39:05

have kind of come up empty

39:07

after all this time, is

39:09

that this study isn't dependent upon

39:11

you. And I think that's an important thing

39:14

we probably want to put out there because

39:16

I imagine a lot of our perhaps slightly

39:18

more conspiratorial-minded listeners out there are thinking, well,

39:20

this is just going to have more positive

39:23

results and the results are going to go

39:25

missing. You know, the Smithsonian is going to

39:27

take them, the Rothschilds are going to send

39:30

their spy team in and take them or

39:32

whatever sort of, you know, fantasies are out

39:34

there. I don't know. I'm not a conspiracy

39:36

guy. I don't buy any lot of that

39:39

stuff. What would stop the samples after they're

39:41

submitted, especially the positive hits

39:44

if such a thing

39:46

does exist, what would stop

39:48

them from being lost or

39:51

mishandled or going into

39:53

the Smithsonian where all the

39:55

Illuminati of the world will

39:57

burn them in some sort

39:59

of ritual? catastrophe and you know

40:02

we don't need to talk about a

40:04

nuclear event or something like that but

40:06

no I mean this is this is

40:08

why I was determined to set this

40:11

up as a as a as a

40:13

real study. This is not me as

40:15

a faculty member at NC State

40:18

saying hey send me these samples

40:20

I'd like to take a look

40:22

at them. I spent years putting

40:24

together the right team going through

40:26

the paperwork with the university.

40:28

As I said, I had to

40:31

go through this ethics review.

40:33

Part of that ethics review

40:35

is having a plan so

40:38

that when somebody submits a

40:40

sample and we say we're

40:42

going to give you your

40:45

results, there is a plan

40:47

in place that that person

40:49

will receive those results,

40:51

even if something happens

40:53

to me, you will

40:56

still get your results.

40:58

And that's something that's

41:00

very important. You say about

41:02

all these studies that have

41:04

happened in the past, but

41:07

there aren't actually all these

41:09

studies that have happened in

41:11

the past, Cliff, in terms

41:13

of looking at them as

41:16

actual studies. There was the

41:18

Brian Sykes study, which is

41:20

the only other university

41:22

study of any alleged Sasquatch

41:25

DNA ever. And that was

41:28

a very limited study. They

41:30

looked at a very small

41:32

number of samples. Other

41:35

than that, folks looking

41:37

at these things

41:39

in university settings

41:41

have been individual

41:43

researchers who have been doing

41:45

this on the side out of

41:48

personal interest. as a

41:50

favor to someone. In a

41:53

few cases, universities, I know

41:55

some universities offer a similar

41:57

service to a commercial lab.

42:00

do where they'll look at a

42:02

sample for the general if the

42:04

general public pays to have those

42:06

samples examined. But there

42:09

hasn't been a large-scale

42:11

university study period that's

42:13

been inclusive of this

42:15

potential evidence. And that

42:17

makes a huge difference because I

42:20

was talking with a Bigfoot

42:22

investigator a couple of months

42:24

ago who had collected

42:26

some very interesting samples. a

42:29

few years ago. A faculty

42:31

at a university I shall

42:34

not name, said, hey I'd like

42:36

to take a look at those

42:38

in my lab, and he sent

42:40

them off to this faculty member

42:43

who was very well intentioned,

42:45

and just never got back

42:48

to him, never got back

42:50

to him. And so I asked

42:52

him, I said, well, who was

42:54

this? Let me try to reach

42:56

out. faculty of faculty. And

42:58

I found out that this faculty

43:01

member was deceased. And I'm

43:03

trying to, I'm trying to get

43:05

to the right person in his

43:07

former lab to find out whether

43:09

these samples are still around. But

43:11

I'm almost certain we're going to

43:13

find that that when he passed away

43:16

they weren't part of an official

43:18

study. So they just got discarded

43:20

is what I'm sure. would have

43:23

happened. When something is not

43:25

part of an official study,

43:27

you're really reliant on,

43:30

you're really relying on

43:32

the individual. And again,

43:34

I think a lot

43:37

of very well-intentioned faculty

43:39

have taken samples from

43:41

folks and not given back

43:43

the type of report, the

43:46

type of information. or

43:48

any information in some

43:50

cases, again, despite the

43:52

best of intentions. Stay

43:55

tuned for more Bigfoot and

43:57

Beyond with Clifen Bobo.

43:59

back after these messages.

44:01

So Cliff, you may

44:04

recall several years ago,

44:06

we had some conversations online

44:08

and you had guessed it in

44:10

my university class and thank you

44:12

again for that. That was a

44:14

wonderful time for the students. I

44:17

am still an educator at heart.

44:19

Well, it shows and it's great.

44:21

But at that time, you and

44:23

I had discussed one-on-one, you had

44:26

come into possession of a very

44:28

interesting sample. And I know you

44:30

were saying to me, hey, I'd

44:32

love to send this to you.

44:34

I'd love for your folks to

44:37

look at this. And I know

44:39

at the time I said to

44:41

you, I said, hold on, I

44:43

don't want to take it yet.

44:46

I did not want to be

44:48

that person who takes samples, again,

44:50

with really great intentions. but outside

44:52

of an official study. Because I

44:55

feel like I owe it to

44:57

folks that if they're going to

44:59

submit, they need to know that

45:01

they're going to get back good

45:03

results. They're going to get them

45:06

back regardless, again, regardless of me

45:08

and that they're going to be

45:10

treated with respect. And was that

45:12

the mattress prince? Is that what

45:15

you're referring to? Indeed, yeah, and

45:17

I know we're we're talking about

45:19

ways that you might be able

45:21

to submit part of that now.

45:24

I would be talking that ever

45:26

with my team at our next

45:28

meeting to see how we might

45:30

work with a sample like that.

45:33

I was thinking masking and like

45:35

the other night as falling asleep

45:37

and these are the sort of

45:39

things that put me to sleep

45:41

I guess at the end of

45:44

the night I guess although it

45:46

gets me too excited and worried

45:48

about stuff so I can't actually

45:50

sleep it's a horrible circular thing

45:53

I live in but I was

45:55

thinking maybe send you like a

45:57

cube like a square centimeter or

45:59

something like that of a place

46:02

where I know that it stepped

46:04

and I don't know there's lots

46:06

ways we can do that. And

46:08

also, I did have the foresight

46:10

of collecting the soil off of

46:13

the fabric itself, and I still

46:15

have that in my files. It

46:17

has been refrigerated, unfortunately. I wasn't

46:19

aware of that component, but it's

46:22

in my files in a paper

46:24

bag. So there's also that. I'll

46:26

talk about that later off the

46:28

air, of course. Back to the

46:31

question I was going to ask.

46:33

Is that you mentioned various other,

46:35

I said studies, but perhaps that's

46:37

not the right word, projects that

46:39

had been done over time. And

46:42

I didn't you mention to me

46:44

when we were in Tennessee a

46:46

few weeks ago about using or

46:48

looking at least at other people's

46:51

data to see, kind of like

46:53

in the same way that Dr.

46:55

Haskill Hart did with the Saswatch

46:57

genome project. They just used their

47:00

data and came up with different

47:02

results. Are you looking for other

47:04

former... projects, I guess, and trying

47:06

to get a hold of their

47:09

data as well? And if so,

47:11

for what purpose? Well, yes, I

47:13

have been trying to do that.

47:15

And that has been, I've been

47:17

trying to do that over the

47:20

last several years. And that's been

47:22

a rather frustrating process cliff. And

47:24

it's been rather frustrating because What

47:26

I find is that there really

47:29

haven't been that many analyses done,

47:31

period. There really isn't data to

47:33

look at, hardly at all. In

47:35

fact, when Haskill Hart did his

47:38

interpretation of the Ketchum at all

47:40

study data that you're referring to,

47:42

ironically, that is the only real...

47:44

data, sequencing data that has been

47:46

made available by anybody who's looked

47:49

at alleged saucewatch samples. So one

47:51

thing that the Ketchum team did

47:53

right was that eventually, at least

47:55

after they self-published their paper, they

47:58

did release that data so that

48:00

Haskell and myself and others can

48:02

take a look at that. Now,

48:04

in the case of that study,

48:07

with the data in hand, it's

48:09

easy to see that the interpretation

48:11

that was given by that team

48:13

is wrong. Now, Haskell has looked

48:16

at that and noted a few

48:18

interesting, potentially interesting things in the

48:20

data, but these are really just

48:22

questions at this point. Yeah, and

48:24

I think that's interesting because at

48:27

the end of the day, Haskell

48:29

is not a geneticist. He's a

48:31

chemist. He's a retired chemist and

48:33

just realize that, well, biology and

48:36

DNA, we're just big old bags

48:38

of chemistry walking around, talking to

48:40

each other at the end of

48:42

the day. That's what life is,

48:45

just chemistry, essentially. And I think

48:47

he kind of taught himself what

48:49

he needed to know to kind

48:51

of do the examination of the

48:53

data. It's neat that someone outside

48:56

the discipline, I think, paid attention

48:58

to it and is perhaps drawing

49:00

attention to these things. And it's

49:02

maybe because he is outside of

49:05

the discipline, it's a perfectly normal

49:07

thing that he just doesn't have

49:09

that knowledge to explain, or maybe

49:11

it's just a new set of

49:14

eyes looking at something from a

49:16

different direction that you wouldn't get

49:18

from the inside of that discipline.

49:20

Yeah, so what Haskell has done

49:22

there is he's raised a really

49:25

interesting question, but it's a question

49:27

that simply cannot be answered with

49:29

the data that we currently have.

49:31

The data might be available for

49:34

a couple of these previous studies,

49:36

right? So does the data do

49:38

you any good or do you

49:40

actually need the physical samples to

49:43

make any progress on this? Well,

49:45

the data in one sense, the

49:47

data could do some good. And

49:49

I think that one of the

49:52

things, though, is that I found

49:54

that the data just simply isn't

49:56

there. There are, on the one

49:58

hand, I've found that there are

50:00

far more rumors of genetic analyses

50:03

having been done. In fact, overwhelming...

50:05

more rumors of genetic

50:11

analysis.

50:13

And

50:16

so

50:18

when

50:21

someone

50:25

says, oh,

50:28

You know, my friend had a sample

50:30

tested and it came back from

50:32

the lab as such and such.

50:34

I've said, okay, well, who's your

50:36

friend? May I contact them? And

50:39

again and again, what I find

50:41

is I contact the friend and they

50:43

say, oh, no, that wasn't me, that

50:45

was my other friend. Like, oh,

50:47

okay, well, what's your other friend's

50:50

name? And then I go and I

50:52

talk with that person and they

50:54

say, No, no, no. I didn't

50:56

say that I had a genetic

50:59

analysis done. I said the sample

51:01

that I found looked like the

51:03

sample that was featured

51:05

on one of the

51:07

Snellgrove Lake episodes of

51:10

Monster Quest where they got

51:12

such and such a result.

51:14

And I kid you not, half a

51:16

dozen of the specific

51:18

DNA analyses that

51:21

had been shared with me.

51:23

verbally, I traced back to

51:25

that one same episode of

51:27

Monster Quest, where someone was

51:30

talking about that, and it

51:32

had become misinterpreted in a

51:35

game of telephone. Again, I

51:37

think everybody trying to be

51:40

sincere, but everybody believing that

51:42

it was their friend who

51:44

had this sample tested.

51:47

I have found actually

51:49

very few instances where...

51:51

There was a sample

51:53

actually tested or somebody

51:55

claims that they actually

51:57

had it tested and.

51:59

far, far fewer than

52:01

that, where somebody can

52:04

actually show me a report. And

52:06

so they really don't seem to

52:08

be that many, practically

52:10

none, that have been done.

52:13

So that's really interesting.

52:15

They're really, we're operating

52:17

in a space where there isn't

52:19

really any data at all.

52:22

And so that's why we need

52:24

to produce some. Absolutely,

52:26

and will the data from this

52:28

study be shared publicly? Or how

52:30

will it be packaged up and

52:32

shared with other scientists even for

52:35

that matter? Yeah, definitely the data

52:37

from this study will be shared.

52:39

I'm not promising a particular timeline

52:42

for that. It depends on a

52:44

number of factors and certainly depends

52:46

on what we find as to

52:48

how we go about sharing that.

52:51

It will be public, yes, indeed.

52:53

Now, will the negative hits also be

52:55

made public? Like if you get a

52:57

black bear sample, the black bear sample

52:59

and that'll be put out as black

53:02

bear? Yes. Yes. To the public as

53:04

well as the sample owner? Yes, that's

53:06

the idea. Yeah, we're planning and we're

53:08

certainly planning publication out of this. You

53:11

know, what we obviously what we publish

53:13

and where we publish and when we

53:15

publish, all of these things are going

53:17

to depend on, you know, what are

53:20

the results that we are getting. There

53:22

are a... quite a few rather interesting

53:25

results that we could have.

53:27

There are lots of things

53:29

that can help push science

53:31

forward. I know that your

53:34

listenership is particularly

53:36

interested in what if we

53:39

were to find unknown species.

53:41

And in that case, I

53:43

think that's where it's particularly

53:46

interesting because in that case,

53:48

we probably would hold back.

53:51

at least for a while on

53:53

sharing publicly, we would certainly have

53:55

it peer-reviewed, but

53:57

from sharing publicly the

54:00

the sequencing, the sequence data

54:02

of a novel species, simply

54:04

because it wouldn't necessarily be

54:06

ethical to put that information

54:08

out there publicly until we

54:10

understood something about that species,

54:13

what are its numbers, what

54:15

are the factors that may

54:17

be impacting it, what it,

54:19

you know. What are its

54:21

social customs? All these sorts

54:23

of things, the culture, if

54:25

you will. Those things would

54:27

all be very important because

54:29

we don't know at this

54:31

page. I mean, what, we

54:33

don't have any idea. I

54:36

mean, I don't have any

54:38

idea whether such a species

54:40

even exists. If it does,

54:42

what is it like? Is

54:44

it endangered? Do we need

54:46

to make sure it's on

54:48

the endangered species list before

54:50

we release this information? What

54:52

are those steps? Need to

54:54

think very carefully because we

54:57

want to be ethical about

54:59

how we how we release

55:01

not the information that exists,

55:03

but release specific information that

55:05

could potentially be used to

55:07

threaten that species. a positive

55:09

and my God there's something

55:11

out there there's a thing

55:13

out there and it's it's

55:15

it's it's it's here on

55:17

the evolutionary tree and blah

55:20

blah blah you had all

55:22

this that that you had

55:24

all the stuff that DNA

55:26

could tell us about the

55:28

next step the actually the

55:30

actual the actual ecological study

55:32

of these things would Basically

55:34

have to start from ground

55:36

zero because of the dubious

55:38

nature of the vast majority

55:41

of the data that has

55:43

been gathered thus far. Even

55:45

citing reports. Now luckily there's

55:47

probably enough citing reports that

55:49

statistically you could probably do

55:51

some stuff and squeeze some

55:53

information out of it because

55:55

the outliers would separate themselves.

55:57

But between the the cultural...

55:59

and just the filters that

56:01

the investigators who listened to

56:04

these reports, who recorded these

56:06

reports, even their cultural perspective

56:08

and all that, comes into

56:10

play here. And this isn't

56:12

a, this is not an

56:14

objective database. There's a lot

56:16

of subjectivity in here, as

56:18

there would be with any

56:20

soft evidence like testimony. I

56:22

imagine a thorough ecological study

56:25

of these critters would be

56:27

five or 10 years down

56:29

the road. because scientists who've

56:31

been denying their existence for

56:33

so long know far less

56:35

than you know us amateur

56:37

investigators do at this point

56:39

even though much of probably

56:41

what the vast majority of

56:43

Bigfooters think will probably be

56:46

proven wrong at some point

56:48

you know or certainly vastly

56:50

refined are you suggesting that

56:52

if you did get a

56:54

hit that you think would

56:56

be strong enough to prove

56:58

the species at least to

57:00

geneticists who understand the sort

57:02

of data that it may

57:04

not be announced for until

57:06

an ecological study is done

57:09

on the animal which could

57:11

take five or ten years?

57:13

No, no, I'm not suggesting

57:15

that. What I'm suggesting though

57:17

is that if, for example,

57:19

such a thing were shown

57:21

and there were specific novel

57:23

DNA sequences that identified this

57:25

species versus any other species

57:27

on the planet, right? Which

57:30

is, that's what DNA does

57:32

when you have a, you

57:34

know, full genome, you have

57:36

a new species, you have

57:38

a known species. The problem

57:40

then is that releasing the

57:42

particular sequence could potentially put

57:44

it in the hands of

57:46

individuals or potentially, potentially corporations

57:48

to create primers specific for

57:50

that species that could. that

57:53

could enable the species to

57:55

be tracked down, hunted, so

57:57

on and so forth. So

57:59

again, we're talking, we're talking

58:01

a what if scenario here, but

58:03

it wouldn't be responsible

58:05

of me to, even if I think

58:08

that there's a very small chance of

58:10

that happening, it wouldn't be responsible of

58:12

me to not think a little bit

58:14

down the road about that. But even

58:17

thinking about the, just think,

58:19

for example, about the pharmaceutical

58:21

industry. Well, the pharmaceutical

58:24

companies might be competing with one

58:26

another to try to track down. such

58:28

a species that would

58:30

presumably be closely related

58:33

to human because of

58:35

the possible discoveries

58:37

to be to be had there and

58:39

the possible money. Yeah, so

58:41

I think the bottom line is

58:43

just that, you know, not the

58:46

not the fact of a new

58:48

species, but specific information about that

58:50

species that could that could

58:53

compromise a population. That's

58:55

the information that

58:57

might need to be kept

58:59

very close to the

59:01

vest for a while. What's

59:03

interesting to

59:05

me about this is the

59:08

range of folks who are

59:10

really excited about this study.

59:13

And I get it. I

59:15

know that some people are

59:18

using the term historic, and

59:20

I have to confess, yes,

59:22

it really is. To have...

59:24

The team that's working on

59:27

this to have these things

59:29

looked at in a scientifically

59:32

credible way, that this is

59:34

not just we're going to look

59:36

at 8 or 12 or 15

59:38

samples and call it a day. It

59:40

is historic. I find

59:42

support on the one hand

59:44

from folks in the

59:46

Bigfoot community who are

59:49

convinced that this will be

59:51

the study that's finally going

59:53

to prove. Bigfoot as

59:55

a species. I also have folks

59:58

who are more skeptical. in

1:00:00

their ideas who think, ah, this

1:00:02

is finally going to be the

1:00:04

study that looks at lots and

1:00:06

lots of the quote unquote best

1:00:08

samples and figures out that they're

1:00:10

all known species and maybe this

1:00:12

Bigfoot thing will go away. I

1:00:14

have people who say, oh, this

1:00:16

is gonna find that Bigfoot is

1:00:18

descended from the Gibbon family. I

1:00:20

have others who say, aha, this

1:00:22

is what's finally gonna show that

1:00:24

it's. descended from paranthropists. Everybody has

1:00:26

their ideas, everybody has their thoughts

1:00:28

as to what we'll find. I'm

1:00:30

just really curious. I'm just really

1:00:32

curious to see what do we

1:00:34

have here? And it will be

1:00:36

what it is. Yeah, to summarize

1:00:38

John Green into Hindon, something is

1:00:40

making these footprints. And it sounds

1:00:42

to me like you're just on

1:00:44

a quest to find out what

1:00:46

it is, except now we're living

1:00:48

in a more modern age. It's

1:00:50

no longer just footprints. It's actually

1:00:52

DNA and biological material. So fantastic.

1:00:54

I'm excited. Well Darby thank you

1:00:56

so much for coming on and

1:00:58

sharing about the project this is

1:01:00

an exciting thing I think any

1:01:03

big footer who is looking at

1:01:05

the goalpost here and wondering can

1:01:07

we can we move the ball

1:01:09

well I shouldn't use sports metaphors

1:01:11

you know me but man any

1:01:13

big footer that wants want some

1:01:15

sort of resolution to this should

1:01:17

be excited about this project. And

1:01:19

I can't do anything else, but

1:01:21

wish you the best of luck,

1:01:23

but I'll tell you, I'm going

1:01:25

to put my boots on the

1:01:27

ground in my best areas, and

1:01:29

I'm going to put the word

1:01:31

out to all the best researchers

1:01:33

I personally know to participate in

1:01:35

this. And of course, it is

1:01:37

open to anybody, if you have

1:01:39

bigfoots on the property, this is

1:01:41

an opportunity for you to participate

1:01:43

as well. Who knows? It could

1:01:45

be your sample to actually win

1:01:47

the game here. So, the links

1:01:49

are in the show notes in

1:01:51

the places you can submit. samples,

1:01:53

like if you want to fill

1:01:55

out the entry form for Darby's

1:01:57

project, the link is in the

1:01:59

show notes, and also something we

1:02:01

didn't mention, this is an ongoing

1:02:03

project. I mean, Bart Darby said

1:02:05

it several times, there is no,

1:02:07

there's not necessarily a budget that

1:02:09

is going to run out here,

1:02:11

but it's not like they couldn't

1:02:13

use a little extra money either.

1:02:15

And there is a donation form.

1:02:17

We'll put that link in the

1:02:19

show notes as well. If you

1:02:21

want to throw 50 bucks or

1:02:24

5 bucks even, how about a

1:02:26

dollar? Anything towards it, it goes

1:02:28

straight to the university and this

1:02:30

study, is that a way people

1:02:32

can help out and participate? the

1:02:34

North Carolina State University and they

1:02:36

are earmarked for my research area

1:02:38

as you'll see online. Fantastic. We're

1:02:40

not shaking our tin cup but

1:02:42

some people out there may not

1:02:44

have samples or may have no

1:02:46

hope of getting any and maybe

1:02:48

they want to participate in some

1:02:50

other way as well. So there

1:02:52

is that. Any final thoughts Darby

1:02:54

before we get going? Well I

1:02:56

really appreciate you all getting the

1:02:58

word out. I think that we

1:03:00

you know folks need to understand

1:03:02

that we We can't find what

1:03:04

we're not offered samples of. So

1:03:06

this is the opportunity for those

1:03:08

who feel like they have something

1:03:10

compelling. This is the opportunity to

1:03:12

present it. This is the opportunity

1:03:14

to find out what it is.

1:03:16

Bottom line, this is the opportunity.

1:03:18

And it depends on you. Thank

1:03:20

you very much, Darby. Really appreciate

1:03:22

your expertise, your talents, and the

1:03:24

opportunity to maybe get this thing

1:03:26

done. And thank you so much

1:03:28

for coming on the podcast. Yeah,

1:03:30

thanks for what you're doing. Thanks

1:03:32

for coming on, Sharon. All right,

1:03:34

folks, you heard here. We got

1:03:36

Darby Arca from NC State University.

1:03:38

They're on the case, so let's

1:03:40

get them some samples. And until

1:03:42

next week, y'all know what to

1:03:45

do. Keep it squauchy. Thanks

1:03:49

for listening to this week's episode of Bigfoot and

1:03:51

Beyond. If you liked what you heard, please rate

1:03:54

and review us on iTunes. Subscribe to Bigfoot and

1:03:56

Beyond wherever you get your podcast. and follow

1:03:58

us on Facebook and Instagram

1:04:00

at Big Foot at Bigfoot and

1:04:02

Beyond You can find us

1:04:05

on Twitter us on Twitter at Bigfoot

1:04:07

that's an N in

1:04:09

the middle, an and the us

1:04:11

your thoughts and questions

1:04:13

with thoughts and questions with the hashtag Bigfoot and

1:04:16

beyond.

Rate

Join Podchaser to...

  • Rate podcasts and episodes
  • Follow podcasts and creators
  • Create podcast and episode lists
  • & much more

Episode Tags

Do you host or manage this podcast?
Claim and edit this page to your liking.
,

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features