Episode Transcript
Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.
Use Ctrl + F to search
0:00
This is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar
0:02
Vedantan. All parents have
0:04
moments when their kids test their patience.
0:08
Lian Yang is no exception.
0:11
I often yell at my kids
0:13
for things that they did by
0:15
accident like spilling a smoothie
0:18
or leaving a cap off of
0:20
a permanent marker and you
0:22
know making black permanent stains all
0:24
over the sofa. When
0:26
this happens and the couch is covered
0:28
in black splotches or they're smoothie
0:30
on the floor, the
0:33
perpetrators inevitably offer this
0:35
defense. It was an
0:37
accident. It's not my
0:39
fault. I didn't mean to
0:41
do it. I shouldn't say this,
0:43
but I tell them it doesn't matter that you didn't
0:45
mean to do it. What matters is that you won't
0:47
do it again. Lian's
0:50
reaction, while understandable, is
0:52
deeply ironic. She's a
0:55
psychologist who studies how we
0:57
read other people's intentions. We
0:59
need to think about other people's minds in
1:01
order to figure out who our friends are,
1:03
who to avoid, whom to
1:05
punish, whether to punish. And
1:07
we need to read people's intentions
1:09
in any ordinary interaction, like having
1:11
a conversation and figuring out what
1:13
to say and how to respond.
1:19
As we go through life, we
1:21
are constantly making sense of
1:23
people's actions by interpreting their intentions.
1:27
Our ability to read what is
1:29
happening in other people's minds is like
1:32
an invisible compass guiding us through
1:34
life. But sometimes it
1:36
leads us astray. We
1:39
misread other people's intentions,
1:41
especially when we're angry or
1:44
hurt. In
1:46
the last few weeks in our Relationships
1:48
2 .0 series, we've looked at the importance
1:50
of human connection and how we can
1:53
become better negotiators. If
1:55
you missed those episodes, do check them out.
1:59
This week on Hidden Brain, how
2:01
our powers of observation allow us
2:03
to navigate our social worlds
2:05
until they don't. We
2:17
are constantly trying to read other
2:19
people's minds. When we
2:21
interact with friends, relatives, and coworkers,
2:24
we ask ourselves, what is going
2:26
on in this other person's head? What
2:28
does she want? What are his intentions?
2:32
Our ability to read other minds involves
2:34
an extraordinary feat of
2:36
cognition. Yet, it mostly
2:38
unfolds in our heads without us being
2:40
aware of it. Minus the
2:42
skill, the simplest of interactions would
2:44
be mired in confusion, and
2:46
misunderstanding. Lian
2:49
Yang is a psychologist and neuroscientist
2:51
at Boston College. She
2:53
has spent years studying this mental ability
2:55
and the profound effects it has
2:57
on our lives. Lian Yang,
3:00
welcome to Hidden Brain. Thanks
3:02
so much. It's good to be here, Shankar. I
3:04
want to start with a very simple example that
3:06
shows how important it is for us to
3:08
read what's happening in the minds of other people.
3:11
In the 1993 movie Mrs.
3:13
Doubtfire starring Robin Williams, the
3:16
characters Daniel and Miranda have split
3:18
up, and Daniel comes up with
3:20
this unconventional way to win Miranda
3:22
back. He returns to the house
3:24
in disguise as Mrs. Doubtfire, an
3:26
elderly widow who seeks the role
3:28
of nanny and housekeeper. Now, he
3:30
quickly wins the trust of the
3:32
family. Miranda
3:34
is asking Mrs. Doubtfire for life advice,
3:36
including whether to go on a
3:38
date with a man she's just met.
3:41
Mrs. Doubtfire, may I ask
3:43
your question? Oh, sir. How
3:45
long after Mr. Doubtfire
3:48
passed away, Winston, did
3:50
you feel any desire? Never.
3:54
Never? Never again. Never again? Once
3:56
the father of your children is
3:58
out of the picture. The
4:00
only solution is total
4:02
and life belongs celibacy. Celibacy?
4:05
Yes. So
4:07
Leanne, if we lack the capacity to
4:09
read what was happening inside the minds
4:11
of Daniel and Miranda, how would
4:13
that change how we understood this scene? Well,
4:16
I think we wouldn't be
4:18
able to appreciate the humor
4:20
and the irony in that
4:22
scene where Daniel is essentially,
4:24
he knows what is going
4:26
on with his wife and
4:28
he is trying to get
4:30
his wife to not date
4:32
this other man and of
4:34
course we know that the
4:36
wife doesn't know Daniel's true
4:38
identity as Daniel. She thinks
4:40
that he is this housekeeper
4:42
and we know that she
4:44
doesn't know and so there's
4:46
this very sort of
4:49
layered understanding that we need to have
4:51
as the audience to find the
4:53
scene funny. We can't find it funny
4:55
without realizing that she doesn't know
4:57
what he knows and who he is.
4:59
Right. So we're able to read
5:01
in some ways that he has an
5:03
agenda here because he wants to
5:05
keep his wife from dating other men.
5:08
And we also understand that she doesn't know what's
5:10
going on. But what's interesting to me, Leanne,
5:12
is that we intuit all of this effortlessly. No
5:14
one sits down as they're watching the movie
5:16
and actually says to themselves, all right,
5:18
this is who what's going through his head. This
5:20
is what's going through her head. It's the fact we're
5:22
able to take it in so effortlessly that allows
5:24
us to understand the scene. Yeah, so
5:26
we're able to and I remember
5:28
watching this movie as a as a
5:30
child Who of course hadn't had
5:32
the benefit of studying how theory of
5:35
mind works in the brains of
5:37
children and adults and I still found
5:39
it very funny I knew exactly
5:41
what was happening who was misunderstanding what
5:43
who knew what other people didn't
5:45
know and so on in order to
5:47
be able to Enjoy the scene
5:49
and really the entire movie So
5:52
you used the term just now, theory
5:54
of mind. It's a term that you
5:56
and other researchers have to describe our
5:58
capacity to understand what is happening in
6:00
the minds of other people. Can you
6:02
explain what that term means to me?
6:05
Yes. So I should say that
6:07
many psychologists and neuroscientists use a
6:09
number of different terms. Theory
6:11
of mind is one of
6:13
those terms and that describes the
6:15
theory that we all have,
6:17
ordinary people have, about other people's
6:20
minds. And what I mean
6:22
by that is how we understand
6:24
that other people have thoughts,
6:26
beliefs, desires, and intentions, mental
6:28
states in general. And so
6:30
other terms that have been used
6:32
for this general cognitive capacity include
6:34
mental state reasoning, mentalizing,
6:38
reasoning about intentions, and so on. And
6:40
again, the fact that we do it
6:42
so effortlessly, many of us don't even
6:44
realize that we are doing it. Many
6:46
of us don't realize that if we're
6:48
having a conversation and we were not
6:50
able to intuit what was happening in
6:52
someone else's mind, really difficult to have
6:54
a conversation. Exactly. Even as
6:56
you and I are having this
6:58
conversation, Shankar, I'm trying to
7:00
figure out... it is that you want
7:03
to know and how to explain the
7:05
term theory of mind in a way
7:07
that will be accessible and so on.
7:09
And sometimes we take different cues from
7:11
people as we're having that conversation, whether
7:13
they're nodding their heads, whether they're pausing,
7:15
whether they look confused and so on.
7:17
And so we take in all of
7:20
that information to figure out what people
7:22
are thinking and how they're responding to
7:24
the information that we're giving them. Nearly
7:29
all the world's greatest stories
7:31
ask you to exercise theory of
7:33
mind, to inhabit the minds
7:35
of other people. Think
7:37
of books such as Kazuo Ishiguro's The
7:39
Remains of the Day, or
7:41
TV shows such as Breaking Bad, or
7:44
musicals like Hamilton. I
7:47
think it's really important that we're
7:49
able to take the perspective of different
7:51
characters when we're watching movies, watching
7:53
TV shows. reading books and
7:55
often as the reader as the as
7:57
the viewer we have a sort of
7:59
different in some cases omniscient perspective we
8:01
can see the scene unfolding in a
8:03
way that characters within the scene cannot
8:05
and so on one level we understand
8:07
what's going on in a way that
8:09
characters within the story do not and
8:12
we also are able to not just
8:14
get into the minds of characters but
8:16
get into the hearts of characters as
8:18
well so we know how they're feeling
8:20
and how they're reacting and responding in
8:22
ways that maybe other characters in the
8:24
don't. So
8:30
psychologists have found different ways to
8:32
measure this ability and to test
8:34
how it develops in small children.
8:37
What are they finally and is this is this a
8:39
skill we are born with at birth or is
8:41
it something that develops over time? This
8:44
is a little bit controversial
8:46
in the field but I
8:48
think what is generally recognized
8:50
in the field is that
8:52
at least children's capacity for
8:54
explicit theory of mind, being
8:56
able to reason and verbalize
8:58
answers to theory of mind
9:00
tasks, that ability emerges between
9:02
the ages of three and
9:04
five years. Psychologists are
9:07
able to administer batteries of
9:09
theory of mind tasks to young
9:11
children to figure out when
9:13
exactly it is that individual children
9:15
are able to think about
9:17
other agents in the world as
9:19
having minds that are maybe
9:21
separate from the reality of a
9:23
situation. Some of
9:25
these tests create artificial situations
9:27
where one character knows more
9:29
than another. Daniel,
9:31
in Mrs. Doubtfire, understands the
9:33
subdefuge he is perpetrating. Miranda
9:36
does not. The
9:38
tests evaluate whether children can keep track
9:40
of all the different perspectives in the
9:43
minds of different characters, that one person
9:45
has a belief that's true, for example,
9:47
and another has a belief that's
9:50
false. So one example of
9:52
a false belief task would
9:54
be the Sally and task in
9:56
which you have two puppets, Sally
9:58
and Anne. is playing
10:00
with a ball and then she takes
10:02
the ball and puts it away in
10:04
a basket. She leaves the room and
10:07
another puppet comes in and moves the
10:09
ball to a different location. And then
10:11
children are asked when Sally comes back
10:13
into the room, where does she think
10:15
her ball is? Did Sally see Anne
10:17
move the block? Because
10:19
she was outside
10:21
swinging. That's right. She
10:23
didn't see. So when Sally
10:26
comes back in, where
10:28
will she think the block is? and
10:32
there, but it's not, it's in there.
10:34
So she'll think it's in there. And
10:36
three -year -old children will tend to say
10:38
that she thinks the ball is where
10:40
it really is, even though she's not
10:42
supposed to know that Ann came in
10:44
and moved her ball, whereas older children,
10:46
by the time children are five, they
10:49
know that Sally has a false belief
10:51
about where that... is. Right. So once
10:53
Anne moves the ball, small children deduce
10:55
or believe that Sally must somehow intuitively
10:57
also know that the ball has been
10:59
moved to the new location. Whereas older
11:01
children realize no Sally, in fact, does
11:03
not have the same mind as Anne.
11:05
And what Anne knows is not what
11:07
Sally knows. Sally knows only what she
11:09
knows. And as far as she knows,
11:11
the ball is in the old location.
11:14
So when she returns to the room,
11:16
she's going to guess that that's where
11:18
it still is. Why do you think
11:20
she'll think that? She
11:23
put it there. Yes, that's exactly
11:25
right. So younger children, three -year -old children
11:27
don't have a concept that people
11:29
could have beliefs in their heads that
11:31
depart from the reality of the
11:34
world, the facts of the situation. Mm
11:36
-hmm. So we've looked at a couple of
11:38
humorous examples of how theory of mind operates,
11:40
but I want to stress again this capacity
11:42
we have to intuit what's happening in the
11:45
minds of other people. This is a skill
11:47
that we use all the time. Can
11:49
you talk a moment, Leanne, about what would
11:51
happen if we lack the skill? Are there people,
11:53
in fact, who do not have the skill
11:55
as they move through life? Yeah.
11:58
This is in a uniform
12:00
capacity that we see the same
12:02
in all people across all
12:04
situations. dependent on
12:06
the individual, it can
12:08
be dependent on the
12:10
context, even in healthy,
12:13
typical populations. We've
12:15
also looked at specific
12:17
patient populations as well, including
12:19
patients with specific brain
12:21
damage. We've looked at prison
12:23
inmates with a clinical
12:25
diagnosis of psychopathy, and we've
12:27
looked at high -functioning adults
12:29
with autism. And so
12:31
we've seen sort of a
12:33
range of behavioral
12:35
patterns across different populations of
12:37
people in terms of
12:40
how they use and how
12:42
they deploy theory of
12:44
mind capacities for moral judgments
12:46
in particular. Lian
12:48
and others have found that people who
12:50
have a difficult time intuiting what is going
12:52
on in the minds of other people
12:55
find themselves hamstrung as they go through life.
12:57
They can be awkward in interpersonal
12:59
settings. They can fail to
13:01
read the room in a meeting.
13:03
They may even demonstrate reduced empathy
13:05
for others. Moving through
13:08
the world without an understanding that
13:10
other minds are different than your own,
13:12
that they have different intentions, desires,
13:14
and hopes, this is like
13:16
playing music without a sense of rhythm. You
13:18
find yourself constantly out of sync
13:20
with your fellow musicians. I
13:23
mean, we've all been a situation
13:25
where a joke falls flat because
13:27
the person who's telling the joke
13:29
isn't able to... appropriately assess the
13:31
mood in this space or what
13:33
other people know or don't know
13:35
and so on. And
13:37
so certainly there are many cases of
13:39
that. And then there are sort
13:41
of the opposite cases where we really
13:43
admire individuals for having a keen
13:45
sense of what other people are thinking
13:47
and feeling and able to shape
13:49
a conversation or discussion in that way.
13:51
You know, I'm reminded of the
13:53
work of the psychologist E. Tory Higgins,
13:55
who's done some work looking at
13:57
politicians who are very skilled at reading
14:00
a room. He has described
14:02
this phenomenon called audience tuning, where in
14:04
some ways the politicians are changing what
14:06
they say in order to be best
14:08
received by the people in the room.
14:10
They're in some ways manipulating the people
14:12
in the room, but they're also being
14:14
manipulated by the people in the room
14:16
so that what they say aligns with
14:18
the audience in the room. It's interesting.
14:20
Theory of mind is not just, I
14:22
suppose, on an interpersonal level. It
14:24
can also happen at a group setting where
14:26
we intuit how a group of people
14:29
is feeling or feeling toward us. Yeah,
14:31
you're right. complicated
14:34
trying to figure out how theory of
14:36
mind plays out in any given situation.
14:39
You know, in my lab, when I'm
14:41
particularly on Zoom, it can be a
14:43
lot harder to read the room, if
14:46
you will, figure out, you know, as
14:48
a group, how people are doing and
14:50
how to shape that space. Lian
14:55
and other researchers have tried
14:57
to understand how the physical brain
14:59
produces the superpower. Surprisingly,
15:01
they found a specific region of
15:03
the brain plays a crucial role.
15:06
They've even found you can temporarily
15:08
disrupt this brain region and profoundly
15:10
change the ways people think and
15:13
act. That's when we come back.
15:15
You're listening to Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar
15:18
Vedantan. This
15:31
is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedanta.
15:34
To navigate our social worlds,
15:36
we rely on something psychologists
15:38
call theory of mind. It's
15:40
our ability to guess the intentions, desires,
15:43
and motivations of other
15:45
people. When your co -worker
15:47
tells you she's thrilled it's Monday,
15:50
you know that sarcasm because you
15:52
unconsciously pick up the intention behind
15:54
her words. But as amazing
15:56
as our social antenna can be,
15:58
They can also sometimes make mistakes.
16:01
We can misread other people's intentions.
16:04
Maybe your coworker really does like
16:06
Mondays. Psychologists and
16:08
neuroscientists Leah and Yang studies
16:11
how our brains read intention,
16:13
both the intentions of others
16:15
and of ourselves, especially
16:17
when it comes to our
16:19
moral judgments. Lian, you
16:21
run experiments where you test how
16:23
volunteers react to a story about a
16:25
woman who accidentally poisoned her friend.
16:27
Can you tell me the setup of
16:29
the experiment and describe the scenario
16:31
in more detail? Yes,
16:33
absolutely. Usually
16:36
have our subjects read stories
16:38
that we write about other
16:40
people who are performing actions
16:42
that have effects on other
16:44
people in the scenario. So
16:46
in one story, we
16:48
have a person named Grace
16:50
who puts some powder into
16:52
a co -worker's coffee. And
16:55
in one scenario, she thinks
16:57
the powder is sugar, but the
16:59
powder turns out to be
17:01
poison, and she ends up poisoning
17:03
her friend. So that's
17:05
a version of the scenario in
17:07
which someone causes harm to someone
17:09
else by accident because of a
17:11
false belief. In
17:13
another version of the story,
17:15
Grace puts powder into
17:18
her co -worker's coffee. She
17:20
thinks the powder is poison,
17:22
but it turns out to be sugar.
17:24
So that's a situation in which she
17:26
has a harmful intention, but no
17:29
harm is done. So in
17:31
these two cases, there
17:33
is a conflict between the
17:35
intention of the agent
17:37
and the outcome of the
17:39
agent's action. And
17:41
so we can ask
17:43
our volunteer participants for their
17:46
moral judgments of both
17:48
the person, the agent performing
17:50
the action, and also
17:52
the action itself, whether this
17:54
action is morally permissible
17:57
or morally forbidden. And
17:59
using these kinds of scenarios
18:01
and these kinds of moral
18:03
judgment scales, we can get
18:05
a sense for the extent
18:07
to which different people rely
18:09
on information about intentions to
18:11
make. their moral judgments. So
18:14
you and I, for instance,
18:16
could have very different views about
18:18
how bad it is to
18:20
accidentally poison a coworker. And
18:22
sort of depending on the circumstances, there
18:24
could be a situation in which there's just
18:26
no way she could have known maybe
18:28
somebody swapped the sugar and the poison, and
18:31
she had the best of intentions. And
18:33
so those are cases where
18:35
there's a lot of flexibility
18:37
for individual variation in moral
18:39
judgments. We can apply
18:41
that same reasoning to the case of a
18:44
failed attempt to cause harm too. Some
18:46
people might focus more on the neutral
18:48
outcome, the fact that nothing bad happened
18:50
at all, whereas other folks might focus
18:52
a lot more on the fact that
18:54
this person just tried to poison their
18:57
coworker and that's very, very bad. Yeah,
18:59
as I was listening to those scenarios,
19:02
you know, I would have said that the
19:04
person who didn't mean to harm her
19:06
friend but accidentally caused harm is in
19:08
fact innocent. But the person who
19:10
didn't cause harm when she intended to cause
19:12
harm was in fact culpable that this
19:14
was an act of attempted murder. You
19:17
had the insight to study not
19:19
just how people reach different conclusions, but
19:21
how their brains were operating as
19:23
they reach these different moral judgments. Can
19:25
you tell me about those studies
19:27
and what you found, Lian? So
19:29
we've run a number of
19:31
studies now using brain imaging techniques
19:34
to look at how people's
19:36
brains are responding as they're making
19:38
moral judgments of these kinds
19:40
of cases. And so what
19:42
we found in one study
19:44
was that a brain region
19:47
called the right temporal pridal
19:49
junction, which is right above
19:51
and behind your right ear,
19:53
processes information about people's intentions.
19:56
And what we found was
19:58
that the more an individual's
20:00
right -temper -pridal junction responds as
20:02
they are making these moral
20:04
judgments, the more they are
20:06
using information about innocent intentions
20:08
to let the person who
20:10
caused harm by accident off
20:12
the hook. And so we
20:14
see this correlation between brain
20:16
activity in this region that
20:18
tracks intention information and the
20:20
moral judgments that people are
20:22
making of accidental harms. So
20:24
you could, of course, say that
20:26
merely because a brain region appears active,
20:29
you don't necessarily know that it's actually
20:31
connected to the outcome and behavior
20:34
that you're seeing, but you've gone a
20:36
step further to actually test whether
20:38
this brain region is, in fact, implicated
20:40
in understanding the intentions of others. Tell
20:43
me how you've done this, Leanne. In
20:45
addition to using brain imaging, which helps
20:47
us to track what brains are doing
20:49
as people are making moral judgments, we've
20:51
also used a technique called
20:54
Trainscranial magnetic stimulation or TMS
20:56
for short to temporarily disrupt
20:58
activity in this particular brain
21:00
region, the right temporal -pridal
21:02
junction to see what effect
21:04
that has on the moral
21:07
judgments that people make. And
21:09
so when we temporarily disrupt
21:11
activity in this brain region, we
21:13
see that people's moral judgments
21:15
rely less on information about intentions
21:17
in these kinds of cases
21:19
that we've been talking about. So
21:21
to give you an example, if
21:24
you are reading a story about
21:26
somebody who tries to poison their
21:28
friend but fails to do so
21:30
because they mistook the substance for
21:32
poison, but it was in fact
21:34
sugar, if I am
21:36
disrupting activity in your right temporal
21:38
pridal junction, you'll be more likely
21:40
to say that that is more okay
21:43
than if I didn't disrupt activity in
21:45
your right temporal pridal junction. That
21:47
is actually somewhat disturbing, isn't it? The
21:49
idea that you disrupt a small portion
21:51
of my brain and something that I
21:53
think of as core to myself. You
21:56
know, how I think of myself as
21:58
being a moral person can be altered
22:00
by small changes in neurochemistry. I
22:02
think a lot of us share
22:05
the intuition that is confirmed by
22:07
recent empirical work in psychology that
22:09
How we think about moral situations
22:11
or moral beliefs are really central
22:13
to what we consider to be
22:15
our identity. We take our moral
22:18
identity as central to our self
22:20
-concept. And so to think that,
22:22
you know, interventions, scientific
22:24
interventions can alter our moral judgments
22:26
is in some ways upsetting. That
22:29
said, as neuroscientists,
22:31
We've assumed all along that
22:33
our moral judgments have some
22:35
place in in the brain
22:37
and so it stands to
22:40
reason that when you Disrupt
22:42
activity in people's brains that
22:44
you will be disrupting the
22:46
kinds of judgments that we'll
22:48
be making to and including
22:50
moral judgments and There is
22:52
so much work on the
22:54
unconscious influences on behavior.
22:56
And so whether someone is
22:58
in a rush to get
23:00
somewhere can change or impact
23:02
the likelihood of their stopping
23:04
to give money to a
23:06
homeless person. And so I
23:08
think that there are environmental
23:10
influences. There are cultural differences
23:12
in the degree to which
23:14
people rely on intention information.
23:16
And so in many ways,
23:18
I'm not sure that I
23:20
would be more upset by
23:22
the fact that smelling fresh
23:24
cookies is going to impact
23:26
my behavior or somebody applying
23:28
transcranial magnetic stimulation to my
23:30
brain is going to impact
23:32
my behavior or my decision -making.
23:41
So much of our moral reasoning depends
23:43
on our ability to consider the
23:45
intentions of other people. When
23:47
someone makes a mistake but we see they
23:49
didn't mean to do it, We usually are
23:51
less harsh with them. This
23:53
is why kids say, it was an accident.
23:57
But as Leon points out, a number
23:59
of factors can change how and whether
24:01
we are willing to consider the intentions
24:03
of a wrongdoer. When someone steps
24:05
on your toe in the hallway, you
24:07
automatically assume they didn't mean to do it. Your
24:10
mind gravitates to an innocent
24:12
explanation. But other situations
24:14
work the opposite way. They make
24:16
it nearly impossible for us to
24:18
think about the intentions behind an
24:20
outcome. Consider this disturbing new
24:22
story out of Chicago. Now
24:40
when I hear this,
24:42
I find it really difficult
24:44
to think about whether
24:46
the police officer meant to
24:48
do any harm. A
24:50
nine -year -old child is dead. The
24:53
intentions of the driver seem irrelevant.
24:56
And when I hear, as actually happened
24:58
in this case, that the police officer
25:00
was given a traffic citation rather than
25:02
a criminal charge, I feel
25:04
outraged. But here's the thing. If
25:06
the cop had run a stop sign and that
25:08
was the end of it, do I think he
25:10
should be criminally charged? That would
25:12
be absurd. So the
25:14
same actions with the same intentions
25:16
caused my mind to reach
25:18
for very different conclusions. terrible
25:22
tension between the fact
25:24
that nobody meant any harm,
25:26
nobody meant to kill
25:28
anyone, and the fact
25:31
that this nine -year -old
25:33
boy died. And
25:35
to take it a step further, you
25:37
could think of a case in which he
25:39
hadn't run a stop sign. Maybe he
25:41
was just driving and the child came out
25:43
of nowhere. I think we would still
25:45
have the intuition that if you caused that
25:47
event to happen. If you cause that
25:49
bad outcome, then there is a way in
25:52
which you are causally responsible for something
25:54
very bad that you didn't know that you
25:56
would be doing and maybe could not
25:58
have prevented. And so it's
26:00
really tricky to figure out how to
26:02
handle that kind of case. As you
26:04
point out, I think different people have
26:06
different responses to what happened and what
26:08
should be done and how to prevent
26:10
that from happening again. There
26:12
are other situations where our ability
26:15
to think about intentions gets disabled. If
26:17
we hear that someone has knowingly committed
26:20
incest with a sibling, you
26:22
might not stop to think about whether both siblings
26:24
consented or that no one else was affected. The
26:27
violation of the taboo, the
26:29
outcome, is all that matters. And
26:32
often in these
26:34
cases, we downplay
26:37
Intent information it doesn't matter that
26:39
you didn't know the fact
26:41
that you did it is bad
26:43
enough and so that happens
26:45
for again as I mentioned violations
26:47
related to food and sex
26:49
and those are cases in which
26:51
once you are sort of
26:53
defiled there's very little that you
26:55
can do to get clean
26:58
again and you know there's very
27:00
little that you could say
27:02
to sort of justify or mitigate
27:04
the behavior, including that you
27:06
didn't know or that it wasn't
27:08
done on purpose. I
27:11
want to talk a moment, Leanne,
27:13
about how our understanding of events
27:15
changes as our understanding of the
27:17
intentions behind those events changes. On
27:20
September 11, 2001, when
27:22
the first plane hit the World Trade
27:24
Center tower, no one knew what
27:26
was happening. Many news reports, in fact,
27:28
speculated it might have been some kind
27:30
of accident. But when the second plane
27:32
hit, it changed the way people understood
27:34
what was happening. The second plane made
27:36
it clear the attacks were intentional. Yeah,
27:40
exactly. So whether we interpret
27:42
an event as just a
27:44
natural disaster or a, you
27:46
know, technical malfunction or as
27:48
a coordinated planned Attack can
27:50
really affect the way that
27:52
we respond to those events
27:54
and so when we hear
27:56
about something like that I
27:58
think you know first we
28:00
ask ourselves or you know
28:03
read the news to find
28:05
out what happened and then
28:07
we want to know why
28:09
and who If relevant and
28:11
so we ask those kinds
28:13
of questions in that order
28:15
and as you say our
28:17
answers to those questions really
28:19
help shape or understanding of
28:21
an event as either misfortune
28:23
or we are trying to
28:25
figure out who did it
28:27
and why and what we
28:29
can do to prevent it
28:31
from happening in the future.
28:34
So, do you think this is why in some
28:36
ways we have this capacity in our heads in
28:38
the first place? You know, I
28:40
remember on 9 -11 I was working in
28:42
the newsroom of the Washington Post and once
28:44
we knew that that two planes had
28:46
hit the World Trade Center and a third
28:49
plane had hit the Pentagon, it was
28:51
clear that we were under attack. At which
28:53
point, you know, it prompted us to
28:55
say, okay, what should we do? Could we
28:57
be under attack? Is there some danger
28:59
that's facing us? And of course, if our
29:01
reading of the events had been different,
29:03
if we had said, all right, this was
29:05
an isolated accident. It was just a
29:07
plane that basically lost control and happened to
29:09
fly into the World Trade Center building.
29:11
Our response to the incident would be entirely
29:13
different. We would say, okay, we need
29:16
to have better flight security measures, better pilot
29:18
training. So our responses to the events
29:20
are very different as we read the intentions
29:22
behind those events. And I'm wondering, do
29:24
you think this might be partly why our
29:26
brains come with this capacity to read
29:28
intentions? Because as we read intentions, it tells
29:30
us how to respond to the world. Absolutely.
29:33
I think our ability to read intentions
29:36
tells us how to evaluate the events around
29:38
us, how to understand them, how to
29:40
predict what's going to happen in the future,
29:42
and how to interact with people in
29:44
the present. And so
29:46
all of that depends on
29:48
our ability to figure
29:50
out intentions and distinguish intentional
29:52
events from accidental events. This
29:57
happens in a lot of news
29:59
events that we read. When we read
30:01
about a building collapsing, we think,
30:03
you know, what happened and how can
30:05
we prevent that from happening in
30:07
the future? And again, our answers to
30:09
those questions depend on whether that
30:12
happened on purpose, whether someone caused it
30:14
or whether it was an earthquake, for
30:16
instance. And so I
30:18
think your question about why it
30:20
is that we have this capacity
30:23
is a really important one. And
30:25
I think we don't have an
30:27
answer to that question yet as
30:29
psychologists, in part because there's so
30:31
many reasons why that capacity
30:33
for theory of mind could be important. We
30:37
need to think about other people's minds
30:40
in order to figure out whom to
30:42
learn from, who's the right expert in
30:44
a particular domain. We
30:46
need to know about people's intentions
30:48
to figure out who our friends
30:50
are, who to avoid, whom
30:52
to punish, whether to punish. And
30:55
we need to read people's
30:57
intentions. in any ordinary interaction,
30:59
like having a conversation and figuring
31:01
out what to say and how
31:03
to respond. When
31:10
we come back, the ability we have
31:12
to read other people's minds can be a
31:14
superpower, but this superpower
31:16
can fail us, sometimes with
31:18
terrible consequences. You're listening to
31:20
Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedantam. This
31:36
is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar
31:38
Vedantan. Our
31:40
ability to read the minds of other
31:42
people is something of a mental superpower.
31:45
It allows us to effortlessly navigate a
31:47
complex social world and intuit what
31:49
other people want and how they feel.
31:53
This superpower helps us understand when
31:55
bad things happen by accident, when
31:57
they happen by design, and
31:59
it allows us to tell friend from
32:02
foe. Of
32:04
course, the fact that our
32:06
minds read so much into the
32:08
intentions of others also makes
32:10
the superpower ripe for exploitation by
32:12
con artists, marketing gurus, and
32:14
politicians. At Boston
32:16
College, neuroscientist Leanne Young studies the
32:18
psychology of theory of mind,
32:21
our ability to think about the
32:23
mental states of others, including
32:25
their intentions. In her lab,
32:27
she and her colleagues explore the role
32:29
of intention when it comes to making
32:31
moral judgments. Liane, I want
32:33
to talk about some ways in
32:35
which our ability to read other people's
32:38
intentions can sometimes go wrong. And
32:40
I want to start again with television
32:42
and the arts. There's a very
32:44
funny scene in the TV show Seinfeld.
32:46
The character George has just gone
32:48
on a date with a new love
32:50
interest. They drive back to her
32:52
apartment. They're sitting in the car outside
32:55
its midnight. The air
32:57
is crackling with sexual tension. And
32:59
here's what happens next. So,
33:01
uh, thanks for dinner. It was great. We
33:04
should do
33:06
this again. Would
33:10
you like to come upstairs for some coffee? Oh,
33:12
no, thanks. I can't drink coffee late at night. It
33:14
keeps me up. So,
33:19
um, okay. Okay.
33:25
Good night. Yeah, take it
33:27
easy. Leigh Ann,
33:29
I'm not sure if you're a fan of
33:31
Seinfeld, but what makes this clip funny is that
33:33
George is actually not picking up on her
33:35
intentions. I am
33:38
a fan and it's a
33:40
very funny clip because
33:42
it captures this phenomenon that
33:44
we study in psychology
33:46
called indirect speech, which allows
33:49
for a misinterpretation of
33:51
intentions, you know, because she's
33:53
inviting George up for
33:55
quote, coffee, as opposed
33:57
to asking him up more directly,
34:00
it gives her plausible deniability.
34:03
So if he declines
34:05
the invitation, she doesn't
34:07
have to feel bad or
34:09
offended or lose her pride. But
34:11
on the other hand, it also
34:13
leaves room for just misinterpretation and
34:16
miscommunication, which is what happens a
34:18
lot in real life. Such
34:24
miscommunications can be trivial, but they
34:26
can also sometimes have life -and -death
34:28
consequences. A police officer
34:30
might have to make a split -second decision about
34:32
whether a suspect is reaching into a pocket
34:34
to grab a cell phone or to grab
34:36
a gun. The officer has
34:38
to read the other person's intentions
34:40
in order to decide how to
34:42
respond, and how he
34:44
reads those intentions could be shaped by
34:46
all manner of factors, including bias. Again,
34:50
there is this question of what
34:52
cues we are using to
34:54
read people's intentions from their
34:57
actions. And what is really
34:59
tricky about this problem is
35:01
that we can't see into
35:03
people's heads. We can't observe
35:05
their thoughts or their feelings.
35:07
We can only observe what
35:10
people do and, you know,
35:12
in this case, people's body
35:14
movements reaching into a pocket,
35:16
reaching into a glove compartment.
35:18
And so that leaves room
35:21
for misinterpretation and really awful
35:23
consequences. So
35:27
the fact that our ability to
35:29
read intentions happens, you know, unconsciously, that
35:32
most of us are not even
35:34
aware that we are doing it. I'm
35:36
wondering how much of a role
35:38
that plays in our misreading of other
35:40
people's intentions, because presumably that also
35:42
is happening unconsciously. Absolutely.
35:44
And there are many cases
35:47
in which we don't realize
35:49
that we are misreading people's
35:51
intentions. In the Seinfeld clip,
35:53
George realized shortly after the fact
35:55
that he missed the boat on that
35:57
opportunity because he didn't catch what
35:59
the woman was doing, but there are
36:01
many cases in which we don't
36:03
catch our mistakes and we're not able
36:05
to fix them after the fact. I'm
36:08
wondering in your own life, Leanne,
36:10
have you noticed this happening of people failing
36:12
to pick up on things, reading each other wrong?
36:15
You've, I think, described during
36:17
the pandemic wearing a mask as
36:19
you go into some stores or
36:21
other social settings and wondering what
36:23
people must think of you and
36:26
what your intentions are. The
36:28
pandemic is a really
36:30
interesting case of intention reading
36:32
and misunderstanding. So
36:35
there have definitely been
36:37
instances in which I've gone
36:39
into a public indoor
36:41
space wearing a mask. And
36:43
I wonder what people think about
36:45
what I'm doing. Do people think
36:47
that I'm unvaccinated because I'm wearing
36:49
a mask? And then I have
36:51
to sort of stop and think
36:54
about, well, what do I think
36:56
when I see somebody wearing a
36:58
mask indoors? Do I think that
37:00
they're unvaccinated or do I think
37:02
that they're being extra careful? Do
37:04
I, you know, think that they're
37:06
immunocompromised or they have young children
37:08
who are unvaccinated and so on?
37:10
And so it becomes a really
37:12
interesting exercise to think about how
37:14
people are reading my intentions and
37:16
then how to read other people's
37:18
intentions and sort of backtrack from
37:20
that exercise to the other. Not
37:27
only do we assume we can read
37:29
the minds of other people, we often feel
37:31
we can even read their character and
37:33
intuit whether they are good people or bad
37:35
people. It
37:37
turns out we do this a lot
37:39
in politics. We regularly misread
37:42
the intentions behind the choices of
37:44
our political opponents. We
37:46
see them as malevolent. Here's
37:49
a political attack ad from
37:51
the US presidential race in
37:53
1988. As
37:55
Governor Michael Dukakis vetoed mandatory
37:57
sentences for drug dealers, he
38:00
vetoed the death penalty. His
38:02
revolving door prison policy gave
38:04
weekend furloughs to first -degree murderers
38:06
not eligible for parole. While
38:08
out, many committed other crimes like
38:10
kidnapping and rape, and many
38:13
are still at large. Now
38:15
Michael Dukakis says he wants to do
38:17
for America what he's done for Massachusetts.
38:20
America can't afford that risk. So
38:23
what I hear in the ad, Leon,
38:25
is that Michael Dukakis was intentionally allowing
38:27
criminals to go scot -free and commit
38:29
more crimes. And the ad doesn't explicitly
38:31
say that, but I think it leads
38:33
me to that conclusion. That's right.
38:35
There are many cases where...
38:37
Because intentions are not black and
38:39
white, because we can't see
38:41
them, there's no clear evidence for
38:43
intentions. This is a
38:45
case where politicians are able
38:48
to frame or reframe their
38:50
opponents' intentions, however they
38:52
see fit, to be able to
38:54
shape other people's thoughts and
38:56
feelings about others. There's this
38:58
sort of ambiguity in this space.
39:02
Politicians have the opportunity to be able
39:04
to create different narratives,
39:07
particularly about people's
39:09
intentions. I'm
39:14
wondering how much of the daily
39:16
partisan ranker that we hear, not just
39:18
in the United States, but in other
39:20
countries, is shaped by misreading
39:22
the intentions of our opponents, that we're
39:24
not just taking what they say and
39:26
do at face value, but we're reading
39:28
into it what we assume to be
39:31
their intentions. A
39:33
lot of times people do engage
39:35
in this willful misunderstanding or misinterpretation
39:37
of the minds of people on
39:39
the other side. But then in
39:41
a lot of cases, I think
39:43
this happens sort of automatically and
39:45
unconsciously. We give people that we
39:47
know and like the benefit of
39:50
the doubt, and often those are
39:52
the folks who are on our
39:54
team or in our party, and
39:56
we can interpret or understand those
39:58
events very, very... So if
40:00
you imagine that somebody in
40:02
your party is being accused of
40:04
some transgression, you might start
40:06
to seek alternative explanations for why
40:08
they did what they were
40:10
accused of doing. Whereas if you
40:13
heard the same story of
40:15
somebody committing a crime on the
40:17
other side, then you might
40:19
automatically take that story description at
40:21
face value that they're guilty. You've
40:24
conducted studies involving Democrats and
40:26
Republicans or Israelis and Palestinians
40:28
and obviously each of those
40:30
groups is prone to misreading
40:32
the intentions of their opponents.
40:35
What kind of a study was this and what did
40:37
you find Lea? We ran a
40:39
series of studies in which
40:41
we tested American Democrats and
40:43
Republicans and also Palestinians and
40:45
Israelis in the Middle East
40:47
and we gave them examples
40:49
of acts of aggression in
40:51
both of those cases and
40:53
asked our participants to attribute
40:55
motives. And what we
40:57
found, which is maybe not so
41:00
surprising but was very consistent across
41:02
those different groups of people, was
41:04
that people were more likely to
41:06
attribute acts of aggression performed by
41:08
their own group to in -group love.
41:10
People are just trying to defend
41:12
their own values and their own
41:14
people, whereas people would attribute those
41:17
same acts of aggression performed by
41:19
an out -group to out -group hatred.
41:21
They're doing this to retaliate. They're
41:23
doing this to attack us. And
41:25
so it's very interesting that we
41:28
see this asymmetry in how people are
41:30
attributing motives underlying the very same
41:32
actions depending on whether those acts are
41:34
being performed by people on our
41:36
side or people on the other side.
41:42
This tendency to be selective in
41:45
how we read intentions extends well
41:47
beyond the realms of war and
41:49
politics. Lian says we
41:51
often interpret intentions in a way that
41:53
confirms the stories we wish to
41:55
tell about ourselves and others. I
41:58
think we do that all the
42:00
time and we do that in the
42:02
ways that we interpret the intentions
42:04
and actions of our friends as opposed
42:06
to people we don't know or
42:08
people that we know but don't like.
42:10
We give our friends the benefit
42:12
of the doubt. We give ourselves the
42:15
benefit of the doubt. We don't
42:17
want to see ourselves as bad
42:19
people. We don't want to
42:21
see our friends as bad
42:23
people. And so, again, if you
42:25
encounter a friend doing something
42:27
morally ambiguous, you might make
42:30
up an excuse for why
42:32
they did that in order to
42:34
read their behaviors as fitting
42:36
with your narrative of being
42:38
friends. And so it's very
42:40
interesting that we see this asymmetry
42:42
in how people are attributing motives underlying
42:45
the very same actions in very
42:47
different ways depending on whether those acts
42:49
are being presented as performed by
42:51
people on our side or people on
42:53
the other side. You
42:58
know, I'm reminded of a conversation we
43:00
had some time ago with the linguist
43:03
Deborah Tannen. She says it can be
43:05
hard to recognize someone's intentions, and
43:07
so it's worth assuming that their
43:09
intentions are good because it makes for
43:11
smoother conversations. I think
43:13
it's really useful for both relationships
43:15
and also for ourselves to give
43:17
others around us the benefit of
43:20
the doubt. I think it makes
43:22
for smoother social interactions and also
43:24
for happier selves. told
43:27
my students is that if you,
43:29
you know, have a bad interaction
43:31
with someone, chances are they're not
43:33
trying to offend you or insult
43:35
you. Maybe they're having a bad
43:37
day. Maybe they didn't get enough
43:39
sleep. And I tell them to
43:41
sort of think about our one
43:44
-on -one interactions in the same context
43:46
that if we have a bad
43:48
conversation, it's probably because, you
43:50
know, I am feeling bad that
43:52
I yelled at a kid. that morning
43:54
and has nothing to do with
43:56
their paper or their project. And
43:59
so again, we come back
44:01
to this idea of giving people
44:03
the benefit of the doubt
44:05
and taking intentions into consideration. I
44:08
also think about times when I'm on
44:10
the road and I get upset when
44:13
other drivers cut me off. There's
44:16
really nothing that I can do about
44:18
it aside from give them the benefit of
44:20
the doubt because I know that when
44:22
I'm the one who's speeding or cutting other
44:24
people off, usually it's because my three -year
44:26
-old in the backseat says she needs to
44:28
go to the potty or because we're
44:30
rushing to an event and we're late. So
44:33
to be able to extend that to
44:35
other people, both strangers
44:37
and the people that we interact
44:39
with on a regular basis, I
44:41
think just makes for happier interactions
44:43
all around. Isn't it
44:45
really hard to do though, Leanne?
44:47
I feel like even as I
44:49
seek compassion and empathy from other
44:51
people, it's hard for me to give
44:53
them the compassion and empathy that they seek.
44:55
So there's a real paradox here. It's
44:57
really hard. It's really hard
45:00
to take that step back and
45:02
think about what are the
45:04
situational stresses and influences that could
45:06
be leading to other people's
45:08
actions, whereas it's sometimes easier to
45:10
see those external pressures on
45:12
our own selves and lives and
45:14
interactions. And so if we're
45:16
able to pause in the midst
45:18
of a tricky interaction and
45:20
think about what that other person
45:22
is trying to do or
45:24
not trying to do, again, that
45:26
will lead to much smoother, much
45:29
more positive interactions and
45:31
ultimately relationships. Lian
45:38
Yang is a psychologist and
45:40
neuroscientist at Boston College. Lian,
45:42
thank you so much for joining me today
45:44
on Hidden Brain. Thank you so much, Shankar. After
45:50
the break, your questions answered. Our
45:52
segment where we bring back recent guests
45:54
of the show to answer listeners'
45:56
follow -up questions about their work. In
45:58
this edition of Your Questions
46:00
Answered, Fred Luskin responds to listener
46:03
stories of grudges and forgiveness. How
46:05
can I tell whether what I'm
46:07
doing, which is just I no
46:09
longer interact with her, I don't
46:12
speak to her anymore? Whether
46:14
that's truly a grudge
46:16
or is it just
46:18
me keeping myself safe?
46:22
That's when we come back. You're listening
46:24
to Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedanta.
46:41
This is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar
46:43
Vedantan. Think about the
46:45
last time someone really hurt you. Maybe
46:47
it was a friend who betrayed your confidence. A
46:50
colleague who took credit for your work. A
46:53
business partner who cheated you. How
46:57
long did that hurt stay with you? How
46:59
often did you turn it over in
47:01
your mind feeling a fresh wave of pain
47:03
and anger as though it happened just
47:05
yesterday? The
47:08
hurt we experience when someone breaks our
47:10
trust is a natural emotional response. But these
47:12
emotions can start to eat at us
47:14
if we hold on to them for too
47:16
long. We
47:18
recently discussed this with psychologist Fred
47:20
Luskin, who studies forgiveness and
47:23
grudges as the director of the
47:25
Stanford University Forgiveness Project. If
47:28
you missed our initial conversation with Fred,
47:30
you can find it in this podcast
47:32
feed. It's the episode titled, No Heart
47:34
Feelings. Today, Fred
47:36
returns to answer your questions about grudges
47:38
and how we can come to terms
47:41
with the wrongs done to us. Fred
47:43
Luskin, welcome back to Hidden Brain. Thank
47:46
you. Nice to see you
47:48
again. One of the
47:50
things that makes grudges psychologically interesting is
47:52
that they involve a certain amount of
47:54
mental storytelling. We weave our own personal
47:56
narrative of a person and a wrong
47:58
that they've done to us. You've
48:00
said that grudges can be difficult to
48:02
shake because we often start to combine
48:04
different threads of various grievance stories, and
48:06
these stories start to weave together and
48:08
create a sort of meta -narrative. How
48:11
does this happen, Fred? We're
48:13
constantly trying to make meaning
48:15
out of the world, and
48:17
we have different levels of
48:19
meaning -making. We have,
48:22
like, small things, you know,
48:24
that this person said something
48:26
unkind to me. And then
48:28
we have these meta narratives
48:30
that say, you know, this
48:33
isn't safe. And remember when
48:35
your dad did what he
48:37
did to you 20 years
48:39
ago, which set you up
48:42
upon a life which will
48:44
not be safe. It makes
48:46
it hard to just be
48:48
with the experience and deal
48:50
with the actual insult or
48:53
not. as it's happening or
48:55
cognitively or emotionally with the
48:57
best strategies. You know
48:59
I was talking recently
49:01
with a memory researcher and
49:03
she said something really interesting
49:06
which is that our memories
49:08
tend to produce whatever our
49:10
current emotional states are. In
49:12
other words, if we are happy, it
49:14
tends to be easier to pull
49:16
happy memories from our brain. And
49:18
when we are sad, it's easier
49:21
to pull sad memories from our
49:23
brain. And I'm wondering if the
49:25
same thing in some ways happens
49:27
with our grievances. So in other
49:29
words, someone does something wrong to
49:31
me, and then all of a
49:33
sudden it becomes so much easier
49:35
for me to remember all the
49:37
other times this person has also
49:39
done something bad to me or
49:41
said something unkind to me. If
49:43
you're happy, then your brain and
49:45
nervous system make available to you
49:47
all the other lovely things in
49:49
your life so that you get
49:51
to think now wow the last
49:54
time I was happy I went
49:56
and hugged my partner so should
49:58
I do that again from the
50:00
painful side look at how brilliant
50:02
it is to give you access to
50:05
other times that you suffered
50:07
and maybe to look through
50:09
what were the strategies that
50:11
I used that helped us.
50:14
The problem is when
50:17
it crowds out
50:19
other memories, then
50:21
you actually lose access
50:23
to thinking rather
50:25
than gain access to
50:27
thinking. But the
50:30
original kind of
50:32
substratum is really
50:34
smart. I mean,
50:36
you can think about it almost from
50:38
an evolutionary standpoint, which is that if
50:40
someone does something bad to me, if
50:42
another animal does something bad to me,
50:44
it's very useful to remember that this
50:46
animal has harmed me. And it might
50:48
actually be useful to remember the other
50:50
times this other person or this other
50:52
animal has harmed me, because it tells
50:54
me now, steer clear of this person, give
50:57
them a wide berth. But what
50:59
was very useful, perhaps, in our
51:01
evolutionary history when it came to
51:04
dealing with other animals and with
51:06
predators might be less effective in
51:08
the modern workplace, for example, where
51:10
we're constantly interacting with colleagues. And
51:12
now we remember a small slight. Maybe
51:14
it was an accidental slight. But that now
51:16
compounds itself with all the other slights
51:19
that we start to remember. And now we
51:21
form this metanarrative about how a colleague
51:23
of ours doesn't like us or we don't
51:25
get along with them. I'm
51:27
gonna add two things. It's
51:29
not helpful. It's essential. That
51:32
if there is
51:34
pain, difficulty, hostility,
51:38
mistreatment, you
51:40
have to remember it. You have
51:42
to try to process it and
51:44
deal with it. That's
51:46
not just optional. It's
51:48
like essential. The
51:51
second piece, what I think
51:53
you left out of the workplace,
51:55
is the staggering amount
51:57
of distraction that people
52:00
have now. So
52:02
not only are they
52:04
in a workplace
52:06
with maybe more interactions
52:08
and more possible
52:10
difficulties, their minds
52:12
are frazzled even before
52:14
they deal with anybody
52:17
by emails, by texts,
52:19
by relentless checking, by
52:21
social media, by
52:23
all of the just
52:25
demands on their
52:27
attention. So they
52:30
come into work
52:32
already with a higher
52:34
level of arousal
52:36
because they're so used
52:38
to getting stimulated
52:40
by this relentless attention
52:42
and, you know, like,
52:44
I'm at you all day
52:46
long. So that has to
52:48
be factored in there as well.
52:52
So we received a number of listener
52:54
comments and questions from people who
52:56
decided to sever a relationship with someone
52:58
else. Here's a question we
53:00
received from a listener named Lydia. I
53:03
have been living with
53:05
what I imagine can
53:07
be called a grudge
53:09
towards my sister for
53:11
the past almost eight
53:13
years. How can I
53:16
tell whether what I'm doing, which
53:18
is just I no longer interact
53:20
with her, I don't speak to
53:22
her anymore. Whether that's
53:24
truly a grudge or
53:26
is it just me
53:28
keeping myself safe and
53:30
my peace of mind?
53:33
I can't imagine that
53:36
all transgressions need to
53:38
result in forgiveness and
53:40
the ability to continue
53:42
the relationship as though
53:44
nothing happened. Or
53:46
am I wrong about that? So
53:49
what do you think Fred?
53:52
I can see in some
53:54
ways both sides of this
53:56
equation I might distance myself
53:58
from someone because I'm holding
54:00
a grudge But I'm also
54:03
could distance myself from someone
54:05
because I've thought about this
54:07
relationship and I don't want
54:09
to continue it any longer
54:11
I'm gonna add a third
54:14
piece often we Move away
54:16
from somebody because we can't
54:18
handle the disturbance, suffering, stress
54:20
that come up in us
54:22
and we use distancing as
54:25
a self -regulation strategy. So
54:28
if my sister
54:30
causes me every time
54:32
I'm near her
54:34
to feel anxious, angry,
54:37
unsettled, Sometimes
54:39
if I don't have
54:41
sufficient skill at going
54:43
inward and rebalancing or
54:45
whatever, I'll just keep
54:47
them away to try
54:49
to manage my own
54:51
reactivity. That's incredibly
54:53
common. On
54:55
the polarities that the
54:57
listener sent you, it's
55:00
not so simple. One
55:02
of the mistakes that
55:04
she presented was You
55:07
can forgive someone and
55:09
choose not to have contact
55:12
with them. It's not
55:14
either or. She could look
55:16
at her sister and
55:18
say, whatever you
55:20
did, I'm not
55:22
holding anything towards you.
55:24
My work clean. But
55:27
enough is enough. You go
55:29
your way. I go mine.
55:31
But there's no bitterness in
55:33
me. There's just. You
55:35
know, we tried this for
55:38
40 years. It didn't work
55:40
for me and have a
55:42
lovely life for a while
55:44
and thank you. So they're
55:46
not mutually exclusive. What
55:50
I would suggest is
55:52
when you're dealing with
55:54
your sister and like
55:57
you want to see
55:59
if you can rehearse
56:01
even connecting with her,
56:03
like are you able
56:06
to create a bond
56:08
or an outreach even
56:10
just in practice? If
56:14
not that significant information
56:16
that this may go
56:18
really deep. Second,
56:20
on the other side
56:22
of it, if you get
56:25
into a really quiet Clean
56:27
gentle space like you know
56:29
you're lying on the beach
56:32
in Hawaii. It's 92 degrees
56:34
and you just got out
56:36
of the water. If
56:38
you're inside still tell
56:40
you that no this
56:43
person is not safe
56:45
for you. It's probably
56:47
deeply in you that
56:49
that's what you feel.
56:52
But if it's just
56:54
that every time you
56:56
think of joining or
56:58
connecting, you get anxious
57:00
or upset, that may
57:02
not be enough inner
57:04
guidance to follow long term.
57:08
You know, we like to think that
57:10
when we forgive someone, our relationship with
57:12
them is going to come out stronger
57:14
on the other end. Unfortunately, that's not
57:16
always the case. Here's a question we
57:18
received from listener Sue. I'm
57:21
just wondering how
57:23
you actually forgive someone
57:25
if it's a
57:28
person who has pretty
57:30
strong narcissistic tendencies
57:32
would never admit to
57:34
have done anything
57:37
wrong. This is
57:39
my mom. She's since
57:41
passed. And, you
57:43
know, I am doing therapy
57:45
and I've done the radical
57:47
acceptance and all that. But
57:49
I imagine, you know, if she
57:52
were alive and if I were to
57:54
say I forgive you, she would
57:56
probably burst out laughing and say, you
57:58
know, I haven't done anything wrong. And
58:01
so I'm wondering technically
58:03
how you actually forgive someone
58:05
who would never have
58:07
admitted they did anything wrong.
58:10
You know, if what I
58:12
heard from her, if
58:14
the word narcissist
58:16
tendencies, is real and
58:19
not just a becoming
58:21
a cultural way of describing
58:23
people that we don't
58:25
like what they did. But
58:28
if somebody has
58:30
real narcissistic tendencies, you
58:32
can't expect a full
58:34
reciprocal relationship from them.
58:37
You simply can't. So
58:40
within that context, any
58:42
forgiveness is just for
58:44
your own peace of
58:46
mind. So that
58:48
you will calm your
58:50
brain down, you will open
58:52
your heart back up, never
58:54
with the thought that that's
58:56
going to improve them or
58:59
have them see you as
59:01
you are, because a
59:03
real narcissist can't see you
59:05
as they are. They see
59:07
themselves. Again,
59:10
no, there's this confusion.
59:13
between reconciliation
59:15
and forgiveness. You
59:18
can forgive someone
59:20
who's unrepentant simply because
59:22
you don't want
59:24
to carry that in
59:26
you. You make
59:28
no assumption that that
59:30
will change them. Now,
59:34
somebody who has weaker
59:36
narcissistic tendencies and
59:38
you forgive them and
59:40
you show up sometimes
59:43
that does influence them
59:45
to reduce their side
59:47
of it, but there
59:49
is no guarantee. So
59:52
in Sue's case, Fred, her mother
59:54
is now dead. She
59:56
may or may not have been a narcissist, but
59:58
there's no way that she's going to be able to
1:00:00
accept that she did something wrong because she's not
1:00:02
with us anymore. But I think
1:00:04
even there, the point that you're making
1:00:07
still stands, which is from Sue's point of
1:00:09
view, her forgiving her mother might
1:00:11
be good for Sue, regardless of what
1:00:13
her mother may have said or done. We
1:00:16
can't know what will
1:00:18
be good for someone
1:00:20
else, and we
1:00:22
can't know their perception
1:00:24
of what happened. We
1:00:27
can only be as
1:00:29
clear as possible about what
1:00:31
our choices are and
1:00:33
how our responses were. We've
1:00:36
talked on many episodes of Hidden Brain
1:00:38
about political divisions and how to engage
1:00:40
with people in our lives who hold
1:00:42
political beliefs that are different from our
1:00:44
own. Here's a message we received from
1:00:46
a listener named Ezra. For
1:00:49
years now, I've
1:00:51
been holding a
1:00:53
grudge with my
1:00:55
family regarding their
1:00:57
political stance, especially
1:01:00
around queer and
1:01:02
transgender rights. My
1:01:05
parents have voted for
1:01:07
Trump and I cannot
1:01:09
tolerate the emotional cognitive
1:01:11
dissonance of having parents
1:01:13
who on one hand
1:01:16
love me very much
1:01:18
and on the other
1:01:20
hand are literally voting
1:01:22
to take my rights
1:01:24
away as a US
1:01:26
citizen. I
1:01:28
am stuck between wanting
1:01:30
to forgive wanting
1:01:33
to protect myself, feeling
1:01:36
fear and rage
1:01:38
and love altogether.
1:01:41
I would love some advice
1:01:43
and help for how to
1:01:45
move forward in a way
1:01:47
that feels grounded, loving,
1:01:51
protective, respectful.
1:01:55
What do you think, Fred? What advice would you give
1:01:57
to Ezra? What
1:01:59
a lovely description
1:02:01
of the poles of
1:02:03
a mind that
1:02:05
is trying to integrate
1:02:07
the heart but
1:02:09
knows this is tough
1:02:11
sledding. The
1:02:13
real question is not
1:02:15
should we forgive, but
1:02:19
what's our most skillful
1:02:21
action? And what's
1:02:23
the best mindset
1:02:25
for moving ahead?
1:02:28
Like, forgiveness is a
1:02:30
pathway to the
1:02:32
best mindset. It's not
1:02:34
the mindset in
1:02:36
and of itself. So
1:02:39
when you're in
1:02:41
a situation where
1:02:43
you're vulnerable, like,
1:02:45
you know, that, you know,
1:02:48
listener just called in
1:02:50
and legitimately afraid. That's
1:02:53
not trivial, and
1:02:56
that can't be, you know, swept
1:02:58
under the rug by saying,
1:03:00
well, I forgive them. It's not
1:03:02
that simple. But
1:03:04
at the same
1:03:06
time, we recognize that
1:03:08
simply hating or
1:03:11
demonizing whatever it is
1:03:13
we think caused
1:03:15
our vulnerability, after
1:03:17
a while, will diminish
1:03:19
our own ability to
1:03:21
take skillful action because
1:03:23
we're tired from our
1:03:26
anger and our resentment
1:03:28
and clouds our judgment. So
1:03:31
part of it
1:03:33
is grieving and admitting
1:03:36
our vulnerability and
1:03:38
loss. Secondly,
1:03:41
joining together with other
1:03:43
people who have similar experiences,
1:03:47
so there's some
1:03:49
strength in numbers. And
1:03:52
then really focusing
1:03:54
on within the
1:03:56
legitimate experience of
1:03:59
our life, what's
1:04:01
the most skillful
1:04:03
action? How do we
1:04:05
behave in a way that
1:04:07
will get us closer to
1:04:09
what our real goals are? and
1:04:12
not just have us
1:04:14
dominated by fear or resentment.
1:04:17
That's the piece that
1:04:19
forgiveness clears out. Say
1:04:23
more about this idea when you say
1:04:25
that the goal is to come up with
1:04:27
skillful action. How do we
1:04:29
know what skillful action is for Ezra in
1:04:31
this case? Well,
1:04:33
it can be a couple
1:04:35
of things. One, that
1:04:38
the goal or
1:04:40
motivation you align
1:04:42
with even when you're
1:04:44
not upset or even
1:04:46
when you're not meeting
1:04:49
with other people in
1:04:51
like shared outrage. But
1:04:53
yeah, that's really what
1:04:55
I believe in. Two,
1:04:59
is it experimentally
1:05:01
kind of
1:05:04
verifiable? Like, I try
1:05:06
this. Does it help? If
1:05:09
it doesn't help, Do I
1:05:11
admit that I was not doing
1:05:13
the best thing and go
1:05:15
back and try something else? Do
1:05:18
I maybe ask for
1:05:20
advice as to other
1:05:23
people's experience? Do I
1:05:25
read up about past
1:05:27
skillful action? Is my
1:05:29
mind open to problem
1:05:32
-solving or is it
1:05:34
motivated by resentment and
1:05:36
revenge? Those are
1:05:39
very different motivations, and
1:05:41
they give us different
1:05:43
minds to evaluate the outcome.
1:05:47
I'm wondering whether any research has
1:05:49
been done about the utility
1:05:51
of actually asking the person who
1:05:53
we feel has harmed us
1:05:55
their advice on what we should
1:05:57
do. So in Ezra's case,
1:05:59
for example, I'm wondering what
1:06:01
if he went to his parents and said, Here's
1:06:03
what I'm feeling. I know that you love
1:06:05
me very much. I also feel like you have
1:06:07
betrayed me. I'm struggling between my
1:06:09
feelings of my love for you and
1:06:11
my admiration for you and my resentment
1:06:14
for you. Help me figure out what
1:06:16
I should do with these feelings. You'd
1:06:19
have to be
1:06:21
incredibly emotionally competent to
1:06:23
say that. And
1:06:26
you would have
1:06:28
to have really emotionally
1:06:30
competent parents to
1:06:32
hear that but it
1:06:34
is a phenomenal
1:06:37
strategy and you could
1:06:39
extrapolate that strategy
1:06:41
to so many interpersonal
1:06:43
difficulties just so
1:06:45
many you could imagine
1:06:47
how many intimate
1:06:50
partners. Could bring
1:06:52
that to each other,
1:06:54
you know, you
1:06:56
said this it hurt
1:06:58
me part of
1:07:00
me wants to strike
1:07:02
back at you
1:07:04
part of me wants
1:07:06
to understand you
1:07:08
but underneath what you
1:07:10
said are some
1:07:12
incredibly skillful means of
1:07:15
I'm responsible for
1:07:17
my emotional state I
1:07:19
need to put what I'm
1:07:21
feeling into words, and
1:07:23
I trust people
1:07:26
enough to share my
1:07:28
vulnerability with them. None
1:07:31
of those are easy for
1:07:33
people to access. I'm
1:07:49
Shankar Vedanta. This
1:08:02
is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedanta.
1:08:05
This is Your Question's Answer, our segment
1:08:07
in which we bring back past guests
1:08:09
of the show to respond to listeners'
1:08:11
follow -up thoughts and questions about their work.
1:08:14
Today we're talking with Fred Luskin. He's
1:08:16
a psychologist and director of the
1:08:18
Stanford University Forgiveness Project. Fred
1:08:20
is also the author of the books,
1:08:22
Forgive for Good, a proven prescription for
1:08:24
health and happiness, and Forgive
1:08:27
for Love, the missing ingredient for a
1:08:29
healthy and lasting relationship. Let's
1:08:33
turn again to listener questions. Our next one
1:08:35
is from a listener named Laura, and
1:08:37
it's a version of a question that we have
1:08:39
received from a number of listeners. What
1:08:41
if the person that you
1:08:44
need to forgive is actually
1:08:46
yourself? Could you
1:08:48
apply the same concepts? What
1:08:52
do you think Fred? Is it possible
1:08:54
to essentially hold a grudge against yourself? The
1:08:57
answer to that is yes.
1:09:00
There's a couple of steps
1:09:02
that you know people need
1:09:04
to look at. One
1:09:06
of them is to
1:09:08
legitimately acknowledge what
1:09:11
you have done. That
1:09:13
there really is
1:09:16
no self -forgiveness
1:09:18
without some acknowledgement, some
1:09:21
remorse. You
1:09:23
know, if you look
1:09:25
at Truth and Reconciliation in
1:09:27
South Africa, it's just
1:09:30
a general model. You
1:09:32
know, there was a
1:09:34
pretty strong public forum for
1:09:37
a boy did
1:09:39
we do bad and
1:09:41
this can't be
1:09:43
hidden so the first
1:09:45
step is to
1:09:47
whatever degree possible admit
1:09:49
it to yourself
1:09:51
if safe admit it
1:09:53
to anybody you've
1:09:55
harmed and allow oneself
1:09:58
to feel remorse There
1:10:00
is literature, you know,
1:10:03
research on the value
1:10:05
of a sincere apology,
1:10:08
which is, I did
1:10:10
it. My bad hurts
1:10:12
you. I make that
1:10:14
link. I'm sorry. And
1:10:16
if I can, I won't do it
1:10:19
again. It is
1:10:21
hard for many of
1:10:23
us to have the humility
1:10:25
of a sincere apology. The
1:10:28
last step is taken
1:10:30
from the wisdom of
1:10:32
the 12 -step programs,
1:10:34
which is whenever you
1:10:36
can make it right, make
1:10:39
amends. Now, if
1:10:41
the person that you've
1:10:43
harmed is just you, like
1:10:46
you didn't harm anybody
1:10:48
else, but you got drunk
1:10:50
or you had a
1:10:52
ridiculous sexual escapade that harmed
1:10:54
your life, It's
1:10:57
helpful to share with
1:10:59
one or two trusted
1:11:01
people as an offering.
1:11:04
You do make a kind
1:11:06
of inner mea culpa, but
1:11:09
the amends is you
1:11:11
make sure it
1:11:13
doesn't happen again. You
1:11:16
go to therapy, you
1:11:18
go to 12 -step programs, you
1:11:21
take classes in anger
1:11:23
management, whatever it is. but
1:11:26
you need to do something
1:11:28
positive. When
1:11:30
those preconditions
1:11:32
are met, you
1:11:35
have absolutely no
1:11:37
need for negative self
1:11:39
-talk or bad feelings.
1:11:41
You have done
1:11:44
the basic requirements of
1:11:46
self -forgiveness. If you
1:11:48
have to, you go see a therapist for
1:11:50
a couple sessions. But you
1:11:52
do not need at all
1:11:54
that negative emotion once you've
1:11:56
done those steps. In
1:11:59
our initial conversation, Fred, we mostly focused on
1:12:01
how we can let go of grudges against
1:12:03
people who have wronged us. But
1:12:05
we've all probably been in situations where
1:12:07
we are the wrong doer, where
1:12:09
we have betrayed someone in our life.
1:12:11
Here's a question we received about
1:12:13
that from listener Booma. I
1:12:15
want to know how you
1:12:17
helped a loved one. get over
1:12:19
or let go of their
1:12:21
grudge or heart feelings or bitter
1:12:23
feelings against you. Let's
1:12:26
say you betrayed them.
1:12:29
A terrible incident happened in their lives
1:12:31
and you were not there for them. You
1:12:34
realized it. It's a mistake. You
1:12:37
regret it. You offered them
1:12:39
an apology and later you
1:12:41
have tried to be as best
1:12:43
as you can for them
1:12:45
and been supportive in their life.
1:12:48
But it looks like, you know,
1:12:51
we both are still reading from the
1:12:53
aftermath of the betrayal. I
1:12:55
want to help this loved one
1:12:57
and I want to help myself get
1:12:59
better. Do you have any advice? I
1:13:03
mean, that is a
1:13:05
very tough situation when
1:13:08
somebody feels genuine remorse,
1:13:10
wants to make it
1:13:12
right and is stymied.
1:13:14
I have heard that
1:13:16
multiple times. There's
1:13:20
two things that come to
1:13:22
my mind immediately. One
1:13:25
is some degree
1:13:27
of self -examination. What
1:13:30
was it that led me
1:13:32
to not be there when they
1:13:34
needed me? Is
1:13:36
there something characterologic
1:13:38
in me? Was
1:13:41
I frightened? Was
1:13:43
I preoccupied? What
1:13:45
was the flaw or
1:13:47
weakness in me? So
1:13:50
that I can
1:13:52
address it or I
1:13:54
can show this
1:13:56
person deep vulnerability and
1:13:59
reflection when I
1:14:01
talk to them. Not
1:14:03
just that I'm
1:14:05
sorry. but it
1:14:07
caused me to recognize
1:14:09
how hard something like
1:14:12
this is, or, wow,
1:14:14
I really struggle when
1:14:16
somebody's needs are way
1:14:18
bigger than mine, or
1:14:20
I may not always
1:14:23
hear at the moment
1:14:25
the depth of somebody's
1:14:27
pain. That would be
1:14:29
one, you know, first of all,
1:14:31
it helps her. but
1:14:34
secondly it creates
1:14:36
a shared pain. The
1:14:39
second thing is
1:14:41
sometimes we can't turn
1:14:43
back time and
1:14:45
there is a forgiveness
1:14:48
element there that
1:14:50
you know it's up
1:14:52
to you and
1:14:54
I respect as best
1:14:56
I can how
1:14:59
you deal with your
1:15:01
suffering. The
1:15:03
third piece
1:15:05
is now that
1:15:07
I have
1:15:09
learned this about
1:15:11
the dangers, or
1:15:14
even if I didn't
1:15:16
do anything deliberate, even
1:15:18
if it was just
1:15:20
a bad confluence of
1:15:22
events where absolutely nobody
1:15:24
is at fault, one
1:15:27
of the things that
1:15:29
she can do is move
1:15:31
ahead with a
1:15:33
renewed attention to
1:15:35
make sure that this
1:15:37
never happens again
1:15:40
so that her radar
1:15:42
are more finely
1:15:44
attuned to other people's
1:15:46
pain and she
1:15:49
can even use this
1:15:51
as a teaching
1:15:53
to tell other people
1:15:56
never, you know,
1:15:58
never underestimate when
1:16:00
people are suffering
1:16:02
how vulnerable or how
1:16:04
reactive they might
1:16:06
be. So she can
1:16:08
use her loss
1:16:10
to help others. That's
1:16:13
sometimes the best we can
1:16:15
do. We
1:16:17
received an interesting question from listener
1:16:19
Richard. He wanted to know
1:16:21
about the factors that might affect our ability
1:16:23
to forgive someone. Here he is.
1:16:26
Do you think someone having
1:16:28
more positive emotional states like
1:16:30
the joy of their first
1:16:32
child may increase their likelihood
1:16:34
of being more able and
1:16:36
willing to forgive. I
1:16:38
mean, if you ask
1:16:41
me about the factors that
1:16:43
influence the ability to
1:16:45
forgive, research has
1:16:47
picked up a few,
1:16:49
not that many. One
1:16:53
of them is
1:16:55
gratitude. and it may be
1:16:57
a surprise to you, but
1:16:59
people who see the good in
1:17:01
their lives are more capable of
1:17:03
letting go of the bad. And
1:17:06
that is a reasonably
1:17:09
robust finding that, you
1:17:11
know, if you wake up in
1:17:13
the morning and it's a beautiful day
1:17:15
out and you're delighted that you
1:17:17
had breakfast and you look at a
1:17:19
picture of your kid and you
1:17:21
go, I got a good life, then
1:17:24
when somebody calls you in
1:17:26
his rude, you're going to be
1:17:28
more likely to hold it
1:17:30
better. Now, let
1:17:32
me add one or two
1:17:34
other things to that. A
1:17:37
home where you
1:17:40
were raised, where
1:17:42
forgiveness is modeled,
1:17:45
is more likely to imprint it
1:17:47
on your brain than not. Did
1:17:50
you see your parents
1:17:53
forgive each other? Did
1:17:55
you see your parents
1:17:57
forgive you? That
1:17:59
is a huge modeling
1:18:01
that even can be
1:18:03
pre -verbal, you know,
1:18:05
like, oh my gosh,
1:18:07
I saw this. Second, our
1:18:11
nervous systems have something to
1:18:13
do with our ability to
1:18:15
forgive. There's a
1:18:17
quality of human
1:18:20
nervous system arousal where
1:18:22
people are called
1:18:24
hot reactors. They they
1:18:26
just you know, you've
1:18:28
met them they get aroused
1:18:30
very quickly. Those
1:18:32
people struggle like
1:18:34
crazy to forgive
1:18:36
because the adrenaline
1:18:38
just comes so
1:18:40
quickly and so
1:18:42
strongly and pulsates
1:18:44
into anger. that
1:18:47
that is a real
1:18:49
challenge. The last thing
1:18:51
that I will say is, it
1:18:54
also depends on the amount
1:18:56
of practice you put in.
1:18:58
You know, if you
1:19:00
practice just normally, when
1:19:03
you're cut off in
1:19:05
traffic, when somebody cuts
1:19:07
in front of you in
1:19:09
the supermarket line, when the
1:19:11
airplane is late and you're
1:19:13
gonna miss your connecting flight,
1:19:17
You're practicing what you might need
1:19:19
a year down the road when
1:19:21
somebody doesn't behave right Hmm. I
1:19:23
think one interesting theme that's come
1:19:25
up in many of these questions
1:19:27
and also came up in our
1:19:29
initial conversation Fred is that I
1:19:32
think when people think about grudges
1:19:34
They sometimes imagine themselves to be
1:19:36
in the position of judges. They're
1:19:38
asking themselves I'm a judge. Here
1:19:40
are the facts before me. Do
1:19:42
these facts justify forgiveness? Should I
1:19:45
hold on to the grudge? Should
1:19:47
I grant forgiveness? Should I not
1:19:49
grant forgiveness? And that is
1:19:51
one way of thinking about it. But
1:19:53
I think the point that you're making
1:19:55
is that a judge is making a
1:19:57
decision that really is in the public
1:19:59
good. They're deciding, should this person be
1:20:01
sentenced, should this person be let free?
1:20:03
And you're trying to make a judgment
1:20:05
that's in the public good. When it
1:20:08
comes to the forgiveness that you're talking
1:20:10
about, Fred, you're already talking about what's
1:20:12
in people's own good. And you're basically
1:20:14
saying, is it in my interest to
1:20:16
actually let this go, regardless of whether
1:20:18
it's in the other person's interest or
1:20:20
anyone else's interest? I'm gonna
1:20:22
add two things to that. One,
1:20:25
there's also the statute of limitations.
1:20:30
which most people forget
1:20:32
about, you know, even
1:20:34
if they're bringing an argument before
1:20:36
the judge, most
1:20:38
cases have five,
1:20:40
seven -year statute of
1:20:42
limitations. And
1:20:44
so that's the water
1:20:46
dampening on that
1:20:48
analogy, even though it's
1:20:50
true. Secondly,
1:20:53
forgiveness is not just
1:20:55
for the self. but
1:20:59
it's for the current
1:21:01
important relationships in one's
1:21:03
life that one wants
1:21:05
to maintain and grow.
1:21:09
And again, let's just use
1:21:11
the judge analogy. So
1:21:13
let's say that somebody
1:21:16
does something that's wrong. There's
1:21:18
nothing wrong with imposing a
1:21:21
one week sentence on them,
1:21:23
but then you also want
1:21:25
to be merciful. And
1:21:27
that's missing often
1:21:29
in the judge analogy.
1:21:33
Sometimes you let people out
1:21:35
for good behavior. Sometimes
1:21:37
you commute their sentence. So
1:21:40
we're looking at
1:21:42
a complex life and
1:21:44
we want to
1:21:47
be careful not to
1:21:49
lose the complex
1:21:51
life on our over
1:21:53
fixation with a
1:21:55
negative experience. Fred
1:22:02
Luskin is a psychologist and director
1:22:04
of the Stanford University Forgiveness Project. He
1:22:06
is the author of Forgive for
1:22:08
Good, A Proven Prescription for Health and
1:22:10
Happiness, and Forgive for Love,
1:22:12
the missing ingredient for a healthy
1:22:14
and lasting relationship. Fred Luskin,
1:22:16
thank you so much for joining me
1:22:18
today on Hidden Brain. I thank you
1:22:20
very much. Hidden
1:22:26
Brain is produced by Hidden
1:22:28
Brain Media. Our audio production
1:22:30
team includes Annie Murphy -Paul,
1:22:32
Kristen Wong, Laura Quirelle, Ryan
1:22:34
Katz, Autumn Barnes, Andrew Chadwick,
1:22:37
and Nick Woodbury. Tara
1:22:39
Boyle is our executive producer. I'm
1:22:41
Hidden Brain's executive editor. We
1:22:43
had voice acting in today's episode
1:22:45
from Clara and Rose Dubois and
1:22:47
Scarlett McNally. We
1:22:52
end today with a story from
1:22:54
our sister podcast, My Unsung Hero.
1:22:56
This My Unsung Hero segment is
1:22:58
brought to you by Discover. In
1:23:02
2022, David Jefferson's
1:23:04
wife Jill was diagnosed with
1:23:06
cancer. Jill didn't have
1:23:08
many close friends, but one of
1:23:11
her former coworkers, a woman named
1:23:13
Nicole Kyle, started coming by to
1:23:15
try and cheer her up. Soon,
1:23:17
Nicole became a consistent comfort to
1:23:19
the couple. So in
1:23:21
2024, it didn't surprise David
1:23:23
when Nicole offered to help him
1:23:25
get Jill home after a hospital
1:23:27
stay. What did surprise him was
1:23:29
the lengths she went to once they
1:23:31
got Jill home. David
1:23:33
recorded his story in a park where he
1:23:35
and Jill hoped to one day hike together,
1:23:37
so you'll hear some wind in the background. Nicole
1:23:41
and I got Jill home. It
1:23:43
was during a winter snowstorm. The
1:23:45
pipes had frozen in the house,
1:23:47
but we got them thawed out.
1:23:50
Nicole and I gave Jill a
1:23:52
shower and put her to
1:23:54
bed. Some
1:23:56
hours later, Nicole showed
1:23:58
up with a bag. Show
1:24:01
me which room you want me
1:24:03
to sleep in and I'll stay until
1:24:06
the end with Jill. Nicole,
1:24:09
in a very unselfish
1:24:11
way, took care
1:24:13
of the laundry, food, house
1:24:16
cleaning. She invited
1:24:18
her friends to bring over food
1:24:20
along with some of the food
1:24:22
my friends were bringing. Nicole
1:24:25
would get up in
1:24:27
the middle the night
1:24:29
with me and help
1:24:31
roll Jill over and
1:24:34
attend to her private
1:24:36
needs as only another
1:24:38
woman could. Nicole
1:24:40
was a steady presence.
1:24:43
She never panicked. She
1:24:46
always had a smile and
1:24:48
willing to do whatever what was
1:24:50
next. Nicole
1:24:52
helped me. See
1:24:54
Jill to the
1:24:56
end of her
1:24:58
life, which was
1:25:00
February 3rd 2024
1:25:03
Nicole was an
1:25:05
angel and the
1:25:07
time of need
1:25:09
Now forever be
1:25:11
in her debt
1:25:13
This situation called
1:25:15
for a hero
1:25:17
and Nicole Kyle
1:25:19
is my Hero
1:25:21
for life Listener
1:25:36
David Jefferson. Jill
1:25:38
and David were together for 36 years. This
1:25:44
segment of My Unsung Hero was
1:25:46
brought to you by Discover. Discover
1:25:48
believes everyone deserves to feel
1:25:50
special, and celebrates those who exhibit
1:25:52
the spirit in their communities. I'm
1:25:55
a long -standing card member myself. Learn
1:25:58
more at discover.com slash
1:26:00
credit card. I'm
1:26:08
Vedantam. see you
1:26:10
soon.
Podchaser is the ultimate destination for podcast data, search, and discovery. Learn More