Relationships 2.0: Why Did You Do That? + Your Questions Answered: Fred Luskin on Grudges

Relationships 2.0: Why Did You Do That? + Your Questions Answered: Fred Luskin on Grudges

Released Monday, 21st April 2025
Good episode? Give it some love!
Relationships 2.0: Why Did You Do That? + Your Questions Answered: Fred Luskin on Grudges

Relationships 2.0: Why Did You Do That? + Your Questions Answered: Fred Luskin on Grudges

Relationships 2.0: Why Did You Do That? + Your Questions Answered: Fred Luskin on Grudges

Relationships 2.0: Why Did You Do That? + Your Questions Answered: Fred Luskin on Grudges

Monday, 21st April 2025
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

This is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar

0:02

Vedantan. All parents have

0:04

moments when their kids test their patience.

0:08

Lian Yang is no exception.

0:11

I often yell at my kids

0:13

for things that they did by

0:15

accident like spilling a smoothie

0:18

or leaving a cap off of

0:20

a permanent marker and you

0:22

know making black permanent stains all

0:24

over the sofa. When

0:26

this happens and the couch is covered

0:28

in black splotches or they're smoothie

0:30

on the floor, the

0:33

perpetrators inevitably offer this

0:35

defense. It was an

0:37

accident. It's not my

0:39

fault. I didn't mean to

0:41

do it. I shouldn't say this,

0:43

but I tell them it doesn't matter that you didn't

0:45

mean to do it. What matters is that you won't

0:47

do it again. Lian's

0:50

reaction, while understandable, is

0:52

deeply ironic. She's a

0:55

psychologist who studies how we

0:57

read other people's intentions. We

0:59

need to think about other people's minds in

1:01

order to figure out who our friends are,

1:03

who to avoid, whom to

1:05

punish, whether to punish. And

1:07

we need to read people's intentions

1:09

in any ordinary interaction, like having

1:11

a conversation and figuring out what

1:13

to say and how to respond.

1:19

As we go through life, we

1:21

are constantly making sense of

1:23

people's actions by interpreting their intentions.

1:27

Our ability to read what is

1:29

happening in other people's minds is like

1:32

an invisible compass guiding us through

1:34

life. But sometimes it

1:36

leads us astray. We

1:39

misread other people's intentions,

1:41

especially when we're angry or

1:44

hurt. In

1:46

the last few weeks in our Relationships

1:48

2 .0 series, we've looked at the importance

1:50

of human connection and how we can

1:53

become better negotiators. If

1:55

you missed those episodes, do check them out.

1:59

This week on Hidden Brain, how

2:01

our powers of observation allow us

2:03

to navigate our social worlds

2:05

until they don't. We

2:17

are constantly trying to read other

2:19

people's minds. When we

2:21

interact with friends, relatives, and coworkers,

2:24

we ask ourselves, what is going

2:26

on in this other person's head? What

2:28

does she want? What are his intentions?

2:32

Our ability to read other minds involves

2:34

an extraordinary feat of

2:36

cognition. Yet, it mostly

2:38

unfolds in our heads without us being

2:40

aware of it. Minus the

2:42

skill, the simplest of interactions would

2:44

be mired in confusion, and

2:46

misunderstanding. Lian

2:49

Yang is a psychologist and neuroscientist

2:51

at Boston College. She

2:53

has spent years studying this mental ability

2:55

and the profound effects it has

2:57

on our lives. Lian Yang,

3:00

welcome to Hidden Brain. Thanks

3:02

so much. It's good to be here, Shankar. I

3:04

want to start with a very simple example that

3:06

shows how important it is for us to

3:08

read what's happening in the minds of other people.

3:11

In the 1993 movie Mrs.

3:13

Doubtfire starring Robin Williams, the

3:16

characters Daniel and Miranda have split

3:18

up, and Daniel comes up with

3:20

this unconventional way to win Miranda

3:22

back. He returns to the house

3:24

in disguise as Mrs. Doubtfire, an

3:26

elderly widow who seeks the role

3:28

of nanny and housekeeper. Now, he

3:30

quickly wins the trust of the

3:32

family. Miranda

3:34

is asking Mrs. Doubtfire for life advice,

3:36

including whether to go on a

3:38

date with a man she's just met.

3:41

Mrs. Doubtfire, may I ask

3:43

your question? Oh, sir. How

3:45

long after Mr. Doubtfire

3:48

passed away, Winston, did

3:50

you feel any desire? Never.

3:54

Never? Never again. Never again? Once

3:56

the father of your children is

3:58

out of the picture. The

4:00

only solution is total

4:02

and life belongs celibacy. Celibacy?

4:05

Yes. So

4:07

Leanne, if we lack the capacity to

4:09

read what was happening inside the minds

4:11

of Daniel and Miranda, how would

4:13

that change how we understood this scene? Well,

4:16

I think we wouldn't be

4:18

able to appreciate the humor

4:20

and the irony in that

4:22

scene where Daniel is essentially,

4:24

he knows what is going

4:26

on with his wife and

4:28

he is trying to get

4:30

his wife to not date

4:32

this other man and of

4:34

course we know that the

4:36

wife doesn't know Daniel's true

4:38

identity as Daniel. She thinks

4:40

that he is this housekeeper

4:42

and we know that she

4:44

doesn't know and so there's

4:46

this very sort of

4:49

layered understanding that we need to have

4:51

as the audience to find the

4:53

scene funny. We can't find it funny

4:55

without realizing that she doesn't know

4:57

what he knows and who he is.

4:59

Right. So we're able to read

5:01

in some ways that he has an

5:03

agenda here because he wants to

5:05

keep his wife from dating other men.

5:08

And we also understand that she doesn't know what's

5:10

going on. But what's interesting to me, Leanne,

5:12

is that we intuit all of this effortlessly. No

5:14

one sits down as they're watching the movie

5:16

and actually says to themselves, all right,

5:18

this is who what's going through his head. This

5:20

is what's going through her head. It's the fact we're

5:22

able to take it in so effortlessly that allows

5:24

us to understand the scene. Yeah, so

5:26

we're able to and I remember

5:28

watching this movie as a as a

5:30

child Who of course hadn't had

5:32

the benefit of studying how theory of

5:35

mind works in the brains of

5:37

children and adults and I still found

5:39

it very funny I knew exactly

5:41

what was happening who was misunderstanding what

5:43

who knew what other people didn't

5:45

know and so on in order to

5:47

be able to Enjoy the scene

5:49

and really the entire movie So

5:52

you used the term just now, theory

5:54

of mind. It's a term that you

5:56

and other researchers have to describe our

5:58

capacity to understand what is happening in

6:00

the minds of other people. Can you

6:02

explain what that term means to me?

6:05

Yes. So I should say that

6:07

many psychologists and neuroscientists use a

6:09

number of different terms. Theory

6:11

of mind is one of

6:13

those terms and that describes the

6:15

theory that we all have,

6:17

ordinary people have, about other people's

6:20

minds. And what I mean

6:22

by that is how we understand

6:24

that other people have thoughts,

6:26

beliefs, desires, and intentions, mental

6:28

states in general. And so

6:30

other terms that have been used

6:32

for this general cognitive capacity include

6:34

mental state reasoning, mentalizing,

6:38

reasoning about intentions, and so on. And

6:40

again, the fact that we do it

6:42

so effortlessly, many of us don't even

6:44

realize that we are doing it. Many

6:46

of us don't realize that if we're

6:48

having a conversation and we were not

6:50

able to intuit what was happening in

6:52

someone else's mind, really difficult to have

6:54

a conversation. Exactly. Even as

6:56

you and I are having this

6:58

conversation, Shankar, I'm trying to

7:00

figure out... it is that you want

7:03

to know and how to explain the

7:05

term theory of mind in a way

7:07

that will be accessible and so on.

7:09

And sometimes we take different cues from

7:11

people as we're having that conversation, whether

7:13

they're nodding their heads, whether they're pausing,

7:15

whether they look confused and so on.

7:17

And so we take in all of

7:20

that information to figure out what people

7:22

are thinking and how they're responding to

7:24

the information that we're giving them. Nearly

7:29

all the world's greatest stories

7:31

ask you to exercise theory of

7:33

mind, to inhabit the minds

7:35

of other people. Think

7:37

of books such as Kazuo Ishiguro's The

7:39

Remains of the Day, or

7:41

TV shows such as Breaking Bad, or

7:44

musicals like Hamilton. I

7:47

think it's really important that we're

7:49

able to take the perspective of different

7:51

characters when we're watching movies, watching

7:53

TV shows. reading books and

7:55

often as the reader as the as

7:57

the viewer we have a sort of

7:59

different in some cases omniscient perspective we

8:01

can see the scene unfolding in a

8:03

way that characters within the scene cannot

8:05

and so on one level we understand

8:07

what's going on in a way that

8:09

characters within the story do not and

8:12

we also are able to not just

8:14

get into the minds of characters but

8:16

get into the hearts of characters as

8:18

well so we know how they're feeling

8:20

and how they're reacting and responding in

8:22

ways that maybe other characters in the

8:24

don't. So

8:30

psychologists have found different ways to

8:32

measure this ability and to test

8:34

how it develops in small children.

8:37

What are they finally and is this is this a

8:39

skill we are born with at birth or is

8:41

it something that develops over time? This

8:44

is a little bit controversial

8:46

in the field but I

8:48

think what is generally recognized

8:50

in the field is that

8:52

at least children's capacity for

8:54

explicit theory of mind, being

8:56

able to reason and verbalize

8:58

answers to theory of mind

9:00

tasks, that ability emerges between

9:02

the ages of three and

9:04

five years. Psychologists are

9:07

able to administer batteries of

9:09

theory of mind tasks to young

9:11

children to figure out when

9:13

exactly it is that individual children

9:15

are able to think about

9:17

other agents in the world as

9:19

having minds that are maybe

9:21

separate from the reality of a

9:23

situation. Some of

9:25

these tests create artificial situations

9:27

where one character knows more

9:29

than another. Daniel,

9:31

in Mrs. Doubtfire, understands the

9:33

subdefuge he is perpetrating. Miranda

9:36

does not. The

9:38

tests evaluate whether children can keep track

9:40

of all the different perspectives in the

9:43

minds of different characters, that one person

9:45

has a belief that's true, for example,

9:47

and another has a belief that's

9:50

false. So one example of

9:52

a false belief task would

9:54

be the Sally and task in

9:56

which you have two puppets, Sally

9:58

and Anne. is playing

10:00

with a ball and then she takes

10:02

the ball and puts it away in

10:04

a basket. She leaves the room and

10:07

another puppet comes in and moves the

10:09

ball to a different location. And then

10:11

children are asked when Sally comes back

10:13

into the room, where does she think

10:15

her ball is? Did Sally see Anne

10:17

move the block? Because

10:19

she was outside

10:21

swinging. That's right. She

10:23

didn't see. So when Sally

10:26

comes back in, where

10:28

will she think the block is? and

10:32

there, but it's not, it's in there.

10:34

So she'll think it's in there. And

10:36

three -year -old children will tend to say

10:38

that she thinks the ball is where

10:40

it really is, even though she's not

10:42

supposed to know that Ann came in

10:44

and moved her ball, whereas older children,

10:46

by the time children are five, they

10:49

know that Sally has a false belief

10:51

about where that... is. Right. So once

10:53

Anne moves the ball, small children deduce

10:55

or believe that Sally must somehow intuitively

10:57

also know that the ball has been

10:59

moved to the new location. Whereas older

11:01

children realize no Sally, in fact, does

11:03

not have the same mind as Anne.

11:05

And what Anne knows is not what

11:07

Sally knows. Sally knows only what she

11:09

knows. And as far as she knows,

11:11

the ball is in the old location.

11:14

So when she returns to the room,

11:16

she's going to guess that that's where

11:18

it still is. Why do you think

11:20

she'll think that? She

11:23

put it there. Yes, that's exactly

11:25

right. So younger children, three -year -old children

11:27

don't have a concept that people

11:29

could have beliefs in their heads that

11:31

depart from the reality of the

11:34

world, the facts of the situation. Mm

11:36

-hmm. So we've looked at a couple of

11:38

humorous examples of how theory of mind operates,

11:40

but I want to stress again this capacity

11:42

we have to intuit what's happening in the

11:45

minds of other people. This is a skill

11:47

that we use all the time. Can

11:49

you talk a moment, Leanne, about what would

11:51

happen if we lack the skill? Are there people,

11:53

in fact, who do not have the skill

11:55

as they move through life? Yeah.

11:58

This is in a uniform

12:00

capacity that we see the same

12:02

in all people across all

12:04

situations. dependent on

12:06

the individual, it can

12:08

be dependent on the

12:10

context, even in healthy,

12:13

typical populations. We've

12:15

also looked at specific

12:17

patient populations as well, including

12:19

patients with specific brain

12:21

damage. We've looked at prison

12:23

inmates with a clinical

12:25

diagnosis of psychopathy, and we've

12:27

looked at high -functioning adults

12:29

with autism. And so

12:31

we've seen sort of a

12:33

range of behavioral

12:35

patterns across different populations of

12:37

people in terms of

12:40

how they use and how

12:42

they deploy theory of

12:44

mind capacities for moral judgments

12:46

in particular. Lian

12:48

and others have found that people who

12:50

have a difficult time intuiting what is going

12:52

on in the minds of other people

12:55

find themselves hamstrung as they go through life.

12:57

They can be awkward in interpersonal

12:59

settings. They can fail to

13:01

read the room in a meeting.

13:03

They may even demonstrate reduced empathy

13:05

for others. Moving through

13:08

the world without an understanding that

13:10

other minds are different than your own,

13:12

that they have different intentions, desires,

13:14

and hopes, this is like

13:16

playing music without a sense of rhythm. You

13:18

find yourself constantly out of sync

13:20

with your fellow musicians. I

13:23

mean, we've all been a situation

13:25

where a joke falls flat because

13:27

the person who's telling the joke

13:29

isn't able to... appropriately assess the

13:31

mood in this space or what

13:33

other people know or don't know

13:35

and so on. And

13:37

so certainly there are many cases of

13:39

that. And then there are sort

13:41

of the opposite cases where we really

13:43

admire individuals for having a keen

13:45

sense of what other people are thinking

13:47

and feeling and able to shape

13:49

a conversation or discussion in that way.

13:51

You know, I'm reminded of the

13:53

work of the psychologist E. Tory Higgins,

13:55

who's done some work looking at

13:57

politicians who are very skilled at reading

14:00

a room. He has described

14:02

this phenomenon called audience tuning, where in

14:04

some ways the politicians are changing what

14:06

they say in order to be best

14:08

received by the people in the room.

14:10

They're in some ways manipulating the people

14:12

in the room, but they're also being

14:14

manipulated by the people in the room

14:16

so that what they say aligns with

14:18

the audience in the room. It's interesting.

14:20

Theory of mind is not just, I

14:22

suppose, on an interpersonal level. It

14:24

can also happen at a group setting where

14:26

we intuit how a group of people

14:29

is feeling or feeling toward us. Yeah,

14:31

you're right. complicated

14:34

trying to figure out how theory of

14:36

mind plays out in any given situation.

14:39

You know, in my lab, when I'm

14:41

particularly on Zoom, it can be a

14:43

lot harder to read the room, if

14:46

you will, figure out, you know, as

14:48

a group, how people are doing and

14:50

how to shape that space. Lian

14:55

and other researchers have tried

14:57

to understand how the physical brain

14:59

produces the superpower. Surprisingly,

15:01

they found a specific region of

15:03

the brain plays a crucial role.

15:06

They've even found you can temporarily

15:08

disrupt this brain region and profoundly

15:10

change the ways people think and

15:13

act. That's when we come back.

15:15

You're listening to Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar

15:18

Vedantan. This

15:31

is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedanta.

15:34

To navigate our social worlds,

15:36

we rely on something psychologists

15:38

call theory of mind. It's

15:40

our ability to guess the intentions, desires,

15:43

and motivations of other

15:45

people. When your co -worker

15:47

tells you she's thrilled it's Monday,

15:50

you know that sarcasm because you

15:52

unconsciously pick up the intention behind

15:54

her words. But as amazing

15:56

as our social antenna can be,

15:58

They can also sometimes make mistakes.

16:01

We can misread other people's intentions.

16:04

Maybe your coworker really does like

16:06

Mondays. Psychologists and

16:08

neuroscientists Leah and Yang studies

16:11

how our brains read intention,

16:13

both the intentions of others

16:15

and of ourselves, especially

16:17

when it comes to our

16:19

moral judgments. Lian, you

16:21

run experiments where you test how

16:23

volunteers react to a story about a

16:25

woman who accidentally poisoned her friend.

16:27

Can you tell me the setup of

16:29

the experiment and describe the scenario

16:31

in more detail? Yes,

16:33

absolutely. Usually

16:36

have our subjects read stories

16:38

that we write about other

16:40

people who are performing actions

16:42

that have effects on other

16:44

people in the scenario. So

16:46

in one story, we

16:48

have a person named Grace

16:50

who puts some powder into

16:52

a co -worker's coffee. And

16:55

in one scenario, she thinks

16:57

the powder is sugar, but the

16:59

powder turns out to be

17:01

poison, and she ends up poisoning

17:03

her friend. So that's

17:05

a version of the scenario in

17:07

which someone causes harm to someone

17:09

else by accident because of a

17:11

false belief. In

17:13

another version of the story,

17:15

Grace puts powder into

17:18

her co -worker's coffee. She

17:20

thinks the powder is poison,

17:22

but it turns out to be sugar.

17:24

So that's a situation in which she

17:26

has a harmful intention, but no

17:29

harm is done. So in

17:31

these two cases, there

17:33

is a conflict between the

17:35

intention of the agent

17:37

and the outcome of the

17:39

agent's action. And

17:41

so we can ask

17:43

our volunteer participants for their

17:46

moral judgments of both

17:48

the person, the agent performing

17:50

the action, and also

17:52

the action itself, whether this

17:54

action is morally permissible

17:57

or morally forbidden. And

17:59

using these kinds of scenarios

18:01

and these kinds of moral

18:03

judgment scales, we can get

18:05

a sense for the extent

18:07

to which different people rely

18:09

on information about intentions to

18:11

make. their moral judgments. So

18:14

you and I, for instance,

18:16

could have very different views about

18:18

how bad it is to

18:20

accidentally poison a coworker. And

18:22

sort of depending on the circumstances, there

18:24

could be a situation in which there's just

18:26

no way she could have known maybe

18:28

somebody swapped the sugar and the poison, and

18:31

she had the best of intentions. And

18:33

so those are cases where

18:35

there's a lot of flexibility

18:37

for individual variation in moral

18:39

judgments. We can apply

18:41

that same reasoning to the case of a

18:44

failed attempt to cause harm too. Some

18:46

people might focus more on the neutral

18:48

outcome, the fact that nothing bad happened

18:50

at all, whereas other folks might focus

18:52

a lot more on the fact that

18:54

this person just tried to poison their

18:57

coworker and that's very, very bad. Yeah,

18:59

as I was listening to those scenarios,

19:02

you know, I would have said that the

19:04

person who didn't mean to harm her

19:06

friend but accidentally caused harm is in

19:08

fact innocent. But the person who

19:10

didn't cause harm when she intended to cause

19:12

harm was in fact culpable that this

19:14

was an act of attempted murder. You

19:17

had the insight to study not

19:19

just how people reach different conclusions, but

19:21

how their brains were operating as

19:23

they reach these different moral judgments. Can

19:25

you tell me about those studies

19:27

and what you found, Lian? So

19:29

we've run a number of

19:31

studies now using brain imaging techniques

19:34

to look at how people's

19:36

brains are responding as they're making

19:38

moral judgments of these kinds

19:40

of cases. And so what

19:42

we found in one study

19:44

was that a brain region

19:47

called the right temporal pridal

19:49

junction, which is right above

19:51

and behind your right ear,

19:53

processes information about people's intentions.

19:56

And what we found was

19:58

that the more an individual's

20:00

right -temper -pridal junction responds as

20:02

they are making these moral

20:04

judgments, the more they are

20:06

using information about innocent intentions

20:08

to let the person who

20:10

caused harm by accident off

20:12

the hook. And so we

20:14

see this correlation between brain

20:16

activity in this region that

20:18

tracks intention information and the

20:20

moral judgments that people are

20:22

making of accidental harms. So

20:24

you could, of course, say that

20:26

merely because a brain region appears active,

20:29

you don't necessarily know that it's actually

20:31

connected to the outcome and behavior

20:34

that you're seeing, but you've gone a

20:36

step further to actually test whether

20:38

this brain region is, in fact, implicated

20:40

in understanding the intentions of others. Tell

20:43

me how you've done this, Leanne. In

20:45

addition to using brain imaging, which helps

20:47

us to track what brains are doing

20:49

as people are making moral judgments, we've

20:51

also used a technique called

20:54

Trainscranial magnetic stimulation or TMS

20:56

for short to temporarily disrupt

20:58

activity in this particular brain

21:00

region, the right temporal -pridal

21:02

junction to see what effect

21:04

that has on the moral

21:07

judgments that people make. And

21:09

so when we temporarily disrupt

21:11

activity in this brain region, we

21:13

see that people's moral judgments

21:15

rely less on information about intentions

21:17

in these kinds of cases

21:19

that we've been talking about. So

21:21

to give you an example, if

21:24

you are reading a story about

21:26

somebody who tries to poison their

21:28

friend but fails to do so

21:30

because they mistook the substance for

21:32

poison, but it was in fact

21:34

sugar, if I am

21:36

disrupting activity in your right temporal

21:38

pridal junction, you'll be more likely

21:40

to say that that is more okay

21:43

than if I didn't disrupt activity in

21:45

your right temporal pridal junction. That

21:47

is actually somewhat disturbing, isn't it? The

21:49

idea that you disrupt a small portion

21:51

of my brain and something that I

21:53

think of as core to myself. You

21:56

know, how I think of myself as

21:58

being a moral person can be altered

22:00

by small changes in neurochemistry. I

22:02

think a lot of us share

22:05

the intuition that is confirmed by

22:07

recent empirical work in psychology that

22:09

How we think about moral situations

22:11

or moral beliefs are really central

22:13

to what we consider to be

22:15

our identity. We take our moral

22:18

identity as central to our self

22:20

-concept. And so to think that,

22:22

you know, interventions, scientific

22:24

interventions can alter our moral judgments

22:26

is in some ways upsetting. That

22:29

said, as neuroscientists,

22:31

We've assumed all along that

22:33

our moral judgments have some

22:35

place in in the brain

22:37

and so it stands to

22:40

reason that when you Disrupt

22:42

activity in people's brains that

22:44

you will be disrupting the

22:46

kinds of judgments that we'll

22:48

be making to and including

22:50

moral judgments and There is

22:52

so much work on the

22:54

unconscious influences on behavior.

22:56

And so whether someone is

22:58

in a rush to get

23:00

somewhere can change or impact

23:02

the likelihood of their stopping

23:04

to give money to a

23:06

homeless person. And so I

23:08

think that there are environmental

23:10

influences. There are cultural differences

23:12

in the degree to which

23:14

people rely on intention information.

23:16

And so in many ways,

23:18

I'm not sure that I

23:20

would be more upset by

23:22

the fact that smelling fresh

23:24

cookies is going to impact

23:26

my behavior or somebody applying

23:28

transcranial magnetic stimulation to my

23:30

brain is going to impact

23:32

my behavior or my decision -making.

23:41

So much of our moral reasoning depends

23:43

on our ability to consider the

23:45

intentions of other people. When

23:47

someone makes a mistake but we see they

23:49

didn't mean to do it, We usually are

23:51

less harsh with them. This

23:53

is why kids say, it was an accident.

23:57

But as Leon points out, a number

23:59

of factors can change how and whether

24:01

we are willing to consider the intentions

24:03

of a wrongdoer. When someone steps

24:05

on your toe in the hallway, you

24:07

automatically assume they didn't mean to do it. Your

24:10

mind gravitates to an innocent

24:12

explanation. But other situations

24:14

work the opposite way. They make

24:16

it nearly impossible for us to

24:18

think about the intentions behind an

24:20

outcome. Consider this disturbing new

24:22

story out of Chicago. Now

24:40

when I hear this,

24:42

I find it really difficult

24:44

to think about whether

24:46

the police officer meant to

24:48

do any harm. A

24:50

nine -year -old child is dead. The

24:53

intentions of the driver seem irrelevant.

24:56

And when I hear, as actually happened

24:58

in this case, that the police officer

25:00

was given a traffic citation rather than

25:02

a criminal charge, I feel

25:04

outraged. But here's the thing. If

25:06

the cop had run a stop sign and that

25:08

was the end of it, do I think he

25:10

should be criminally charged? That would

25:12

be absurd. So the

25:14

same actions with the same intentions

25:16

caused my mind to reach

25:18

for very different conclusions. terrible

25:22

tension between the fact

25:24

that nobody meant any harm,

25:26

nobody meant to kill

25:28

anyone, and the fact

25:31

that this nine -year -old

25:33

boy died. And

25:35

to take it a step further, you

25:37

could think of a case in which he

25:39

hadn't run a stop sign. Maybe he

25:41

was just driving and the child came out

25:43

of nowhere. I think we would still

25:45

have the intuition that if you caused that

25:47

event to happen. If you cause that

25:49

bad outcome, then there is a way in

25:52

which you are causally responsible for something

25:54

very bad that you didn't know that you

25:56

would be doing and maybe could not

25:58

have prevented. And so it's

26:00

really tricky to figure out how to

26:02

handle that kind of case. As you

26:04

point out, I think different people have

26:06

different responses to what happened and what

26:08

should be done and how to prevent

26:10

that from happening again. There

26:12

are other situations where our ability

26:15

to think about intentions gets disabled. If

26:17

we hear that someone has knowingly committed

26:20

incest with a sibling, you

26:22

might not stop to think about whether both siblings

26:24

consented or that no one else was affected. The

26:27

violation of the taboo, the

26:29

outcome, is all that matters. And

26:32

often in these

26:34

cases, we downplay

26:37

Intent information it doesn't matter that

26:39

you didn't know the fact

26:41

that you did it is bad

26:43

enough and so that happens

26:45

for again as I mentioned violations

26:47

related to food and sex

26:49

and those are cases in which

26:51

once you are sort of

26:53

defiled there's very little that you

26:55

can do to get clean

26:58

again and you know there's very

27:00

little that you could say

27:02

to sort of justify or mitigate

27:04

the behavior, including that you

27:06

didn't know or that it wasn't

27:08

done on purpose. I

27:11

want to talk a moment, Leanne,

27:13

about how our understanding of events

27:15

changes as our understanding of the

27:17

intentions behind those events changes. On

27:20

September 11, 2001, when

27:22

the first plane hit the World Trade

27:24

Center tower, no one knew what

27:26

was happening. Many news reports, in fact,

27:28

speculated it might have been some kind

27:30

of accident. But when the second plane

27:32

hit, it changed the way people understood

27:34

what was happening. The second plane made

27:36

it clear the attacks were intentional. Yeah,

27:40

exactly. So whether we interpret

27:42

an event as just a

27:44

natural disaster or a, you

27:46

know, technical malfunction or as

27:48

a coordinated planned Attack can

27:50

really affect the way that

27:52

we respond to those events

27:54

and so when we hear

27:56

about something like that I

27:58

think you know first we

28:00

ask ourselves or you know

28:03

read the news to find

28:05

out what happened and then

28:07

we want to know why

28:09

and who If relevant and

28:11

so we ask those kinds

28:13

of questions in that order

28:15

and as you say our

28:17

answers to those questions really

28:19

help shape or understanding of

28:21

an event as either misfortune

28:23

or we are trying to

28:25

figure out who did it

28:27

and why and what we

28:29

can do to prevent it

28:31

from happening in the future.

28:34

So, do you think this is why in some

28:36

ways we have this capacity in our heads in

28:38

the first place? You know, I

28:40

remember on 9 -11 I was working in

28:42

the newsroom of the Washington Post and once

28:44

we knew that that two planes had

28:46

hit the World Trade Center and a third

28:49

plane had hit the Pentagon, it was

28:51

clear that we were under attack. At which

28:53

point, you know, it prompted us to

28:55

say, okay, what should we do? Could we

28:57

be under attack? Is there some danger

28:59

that's facing us? And of course, if our

29:01

reading of the events had been different,

29:03

if we had said, all right, this was

29:05

an isolated accident. It was just a

29:07

plane that basically lost control and happened to

29:09

fly into the World Trade Center building.

29:11

Our response to the incident would be entirely

29:13

different. We would say, okay, we need

29:16

to have better flight security measures, better pilot

29:18

training. So our responses to the events

29:20

are very different as we read the intentions

29:22

behind those events. And I'm wondering, do

29:24

you think this might be partly why our

29:26

brains come with this capacity to read

29:28

intentions? Because as we read intentions, it tells

29:30

us how to respond to the world. Absolutely.

29:33

I think our ability to read intentions

29:36

tells us how to evaluate the events around

29:38

us, how to understand them, how to

29:40

predict what's going to happen in the future,

29:42

and how to interact with people in

29:44

the present. And so

29:46

all of that depends on

29:48

our ability to figure

29:50

out intentions and distinguish intentional

29:52

events from accidental events. This

29:57

happens in a lot of news

29:59

events that we read. When we read

30:01

about a building collapsing, we think,

30:03

you know, what happened and how can

30:05

we prevent that from happening in

30:07

the future? And again, our answers to

30:09

those questions depend on whether that

30:12

happened on purpose, whether someone caused it

30:14

or whether it was an earthquake, for

30:16

instance. And so I

30:18

think your question about why it

30:20

is that we have this capacity

30:23

is a really important one. And

30:25

I think we don't have an

30:27

answer to that question yet as

30:29

psychologists, in part because there's so

30:31

many reasons why that capacity

30:33

for theory of mind could be important. We

30:37

need to think about other people's minds

30:40

in order to figure out whom to

30:42

learn from, who's the right expert in

30:44

a particular domain. We

30:46

need to know about people's intentions

30:48

to figure out who our friends

30:50

are, who to avoid, whom

30:52

to punish, whether to punish. And

30:55

we need to read people's

30:57

intentions. in any ordinary interaction,

30:59

like having a conversation and figuring

31:01

out what to say and how

31:03

to respond. When

31:10

we come back, the ability we have

31:12

to read other people's minds can be a

31:14

superpower, but this superpower

31:16

can fail us, sometimes with

31:18

terrible consequences. You're listening to

31:20

Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedantam. This

31:36

is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar

31:38

Vedantan. Our

31:40

ability to read the minds of other

31:42

people is something of a mental superpower.

31:45

It allows us to effortlessly navigate a

31:47

complex social world and intuit what

31:49

other people want and how they feel.

31:53

This superpower helps us understand when

31:55

bad things happen by accident, when

31:57

they happen by design, and

31:59

it allows us to tell friend from

32:02

foe. Of

32:04

course, the fact that our

32:06

minds read so much into the

32:08

intentions of others also makes

32:10

the superpower ripe for exploitation by

32:12

con artists, marketing gurus, and

32:14

politicians. At Boston

32:16

College, neuroscientist Leanne Young studies the

32:18

psychology of theory of mind,

32:21

our ability to think about the

32:23

mental states of others, including

32:25

their intentions. In her lab,

32:27

she and her colleagues explore the role

32:29

of intention when it comes to making

32:31

moral judgments. Liane, I want

32:33

to talk about some ways in

32:35

which our ability to read other people's

32:38

intentions can sometimes go wrong. And

32:40

I want to start again with television

32:42

and the arts. There's a very

32:44

funny scene in the TV show Seinfeld.

32:46

The character George has just gone

32:48

on a date with a new love

32:50

interest. They drive back to her

32:52

apartment. They're sitting in the car outside

32:55

its midnight. The air

32:57

is crackling with sexual tension. And

32:59

here's what happens next. So,

33:01

uh, thanks for dinner. It was great. We

33:04

should do

33:06

this again. Would

33:10

you like to come upstairs for some coffee? Oh,

33:12

no, thanks. I can't drink coffee late at night. It

33:14

keeps me up. So,

33:19

um, okay. Okay.

33:25

Good night. Yeah, take it

33:27

easy. Leigh Ann,

33:29

I'm not sure if you're a fan of

33:31

Seinfeld, but what makes this clip funny is that

33:33

George is actually not picking up on her

33:35

intentions. I am

33:38

a fan and it's a

33:40

very funny clip because

33:42

it captures this phenomenon that

33:44

we study in psychology

33:46

called indirect speech, which allows

33:49

for a misinterpretation of

33:51

intentions, you know, because she's

33:53

inviting George up for

33:55

quote, coffee, as opposed

33:57

to asking him up more directly,

34:00

it gives her plausible deniability.

34:03

So if he declines

34:05

the invitation, she doesn't

34:07

have to feel bad or

34:09

offended or lose her pride. But

34:11

on the other hand, it also

34:13

leaves room for just misinterpretation and

34:16

miscommunication, which is what happens a

34:18

lot in real life. Such

34:24

miscommunications can be trivial, but they

34:26

can also sometimes have life -and -death

34:28

consequences. A police officer

34:30

might have to make a split -second decision about

34:32

whether a suspect is reaching into a pocket

34:34

to grab a cell phone or to grab

34:36

a gun. The officer has

34:38

to read the other person's intentions

34:40

in order to decide how to

34:42

respond, and how he

34:44

reads those intentions could be shaped by

34:46

all manner of factors, including bias. Again,

34:50

there is this question of what

34:52

cues we are using to

34:54

read people's intentions from their

34:57

actions. And what is really

34:59

tricky about this problem is

35:01

that we can't see into

35:03

people's heads. We can't observe

35:05

their thoughts or their feelings.

35:07

We can only observe what

35:10

people do and, you know,

35:12

in this case, people's body

35:14

movements reaching into a pocket,

35:16

reaching into a glove compartment.

35:18

And so that leaves room

35:21

for misinterpretation and really awful

35:23

consequences. So

35:27

the fact that our ability to

35:29

read intentions happens, you know, unconsciously, that

35:32

most of us are not even

35:34

aware that we are doing it. I'm

35:36

wondering how much of a role

35:38

that plays in our misreading of other

35:40

people's intentions, because presumably that also

35:42

is happening unconsciously. Absolutely.

35:44

And there are many cases

35:47

in which we don't realize

35:49

that we are misreading people's

35:51

intentions. In the Seinfeld clip,

35:53

George realized shortly after the fact

35:55

that he missed the boat on that

35:57

opportunity because he didn't catch what

35:59

the woman was doing, but there are

36:01

many cases in which we don't

36:03

catch our mistakes and we're not able

36:05

to fix them after the fact. I'm

36:08

wondering in your own life, Leanne,

36:10

have you noticed this happening of people failing

36:12

to pick up on things, reading each other wrong?

36:15

You've, I think, described during

36:17

the pandemic wearing a mask as

36:19

you go into some stores or

36:21

other social settings and wondering what

36:23

people must think of you and

36:26

what your intentions are. The

36:28

pandemic is a really

36:30

interesting case of intention reading

36:32

and misunderstanding. So

36:35

there have definitely been

36:37

instances in which I've gone

36:39

into a public indoor

36:41

space wearing a mask. And

36:43

I wonder what people think about

36:45

what I'm doing. Do people think

36:47

that I'm unvaccinated because I'm wearing

36:49

a mask? And then I have

36:51

to sort of stop and think

36:54

about, well, what do I think

36:56

when I see somebody wearing a

36:58

mask indoors? Do I think that

37:00

they're unvaccinated or do I think

37:02

that they're being extra careful? Do

37:04

I, you know, think that they're

37:06

immunocompromised or they have young children

37:08

who are unvaccinated and so on?

37:10

And so it becomes a really

37:12

interesting exercise to think about how

37:14

people are reading my intentions and

37:16

then how to read other people's

37:18

intentions and sort of backtrack from

37:20

that exercise to the other. Not

37:27

only do we assume we can read

37:29

the minds of other people, we often feel

37:31

we can even read their character and

37:33

intuit whether they are good people or bad

37:35

people. It

37:37

turns out we do this a lot

37:39

in politics. We regularly misread

37:42

the intentions behind the choices of

37:44

our political opponents. We

37:46

see them as malevolent. Here's

37:49

a political attack ad from

37:51

the US presidential race in

37:53

1988. As

37:55

Governor Michael Dukakis vetoed mandatory

37:57

sentences for drug dealers, he

38:00

vetoed the death penalty. His

38:02

revolving door prison policy gave

38:04

weekend furloughs to first -degree murderers

38:06

not eligible for parole. While

38:08

out, many committed other crimes like

38:10

kidnapping and rape, and many

38:13

are still at large. Now

38:15

Michael Dukakis says he wants to do

38:17

for America what he's done for Massachusetts.

38:20

America can't afford that risk. So

38:23

what I hear in the ad, Leon,

38:25

is that Michael Dukakis was intentionally allowing

38:27

criminals to go scot -free and commit

38:29

more crimes. And the ad doesn't explicitly

38:31

say that, but I think it leads

38:33

me to that conclusion. That's right.

38:35

There are many cases where...

38:37

Because intentions are not black and

38:39

white, because we can't see

38:41

them, there's no clear evidence for

38:43

intentions. This is a

38:45

case where politicians are able

38:48

to frame or reframe their

38:50

opponents' intentions, however they

38:52

see fit, to be able to

38:54

shape other people's thoughts and

38:56

feelings about others. There's this

38:58

sort of ambiguity in this space.

39:02

Politicians have the opportunity to be able

39:04

to create different narratives,

39:07

particularly about people's

39:09

intentions. I'm

39:14

wondering how much of the daily

39:16

partisan ranker that we hear, not just

39:18

in the United States, but in other

39:20

countries, is shaped by misreading

39:22

the intentions of our opponents, that we're

39:24

not just taking what they say and

39:26

do at face value, but we're reading

39:28

into it what we assume to be

39:31

their intentions. A

39:33

lot of times people do engage

39:35

in this willful misunderstanding or misinterpretation

39:37

of the minds of people on

39:39

the other side. But then in

39:41

a lot of cases, I think

39:43

this happens sort of automatically and

39:45

unconsciously. We give people that we

39:47

know and like the benefit of

39:50

the doubt, and often those are

39:52

the folks who are on our

39:54

team or in our party, and

39:56

we can interpret or understand those

39:58

events very, very... So if

40:00

you imagine that somebody in

40:02

your party is being accused of

40:04

some transgression, you might start

40:06

to seek alternative explanations for why

40:08

they did what they were

40:10

accused of doing. Whereas if you

40:13

heard the same story of

40:15

somebody committing a crime on the

40:17

other side, then you might

40:19

automatically take that story description at

40:21

face value that they're guilty. You've

40:24

conducted studies involving Democrats and

40:26

Republicans or Israelis and Palestinians

40:28

and obviously each of those

40:30

groups is prone to misreading

40:32

the intentions of their opponents.

40:35

What kind of a study was this and what did

40:37

you find Lea? We ran a

40:39

series of studies in which

40:41

we tested American Democrats and

40:43

Republicans and also Palestinians and

40:45

Israelis in the Middle East

40:47

and we gave them examples

40:49

of acts of aggression in

40:51

both of those cases and

40:53

asked our participants to attribute

40:55

motives. And what we

40:57

found, which is maybe not so

41:00

surprising but was very consistent across

41:02

those different groups of people, was

41:04

that people were more likely to

41:06

attribute acts of aggression performed by

41:08

their own group to in -group love.

41:10

People are just trying to defend

41:12

their own values and their own

41:14

people, whereas people would attribute those

41:17

same acts of aggression performed by

41:19

an out -group to out -group hatred.

41:21

They're doing this to retaliate. They're

41:23

doing this to attack us. And

41:25

so it's very interesting that we

41:28

see this asymmetry in how people are

41:30

attributing motives underlying the very same

41:32

actions depending on whether those acts are

41:34

being performed by people on our

41:36

side or people on the other side.

41:42

This tendency to be selective in

41:45

how we read intentions extends well

41:47

beyond the realms of war and

41:49

politics. Lian says we

41:51

often interpret intentions in a way that

41:53

confirms the stories we wish to

41:55

tell about ourselves and others. I

41:58

think we do that all the

42:00

time and we do that in the

42:02

ways that we interpret the intentions

42:04

and actions of our friends as opposed

42:06

to people we don't know or

42:08

people that we know but don't like.

42:10

We give our friends the benefit

42:12

of the doubt. We give ourselves the

42:15

benefit of the doubt. We don't

42:17

want to see ourselves as bad

42:19

people. We don't want to

42:21

see our friends as bad

42:23

people. And so, again, if you

42:25

encounter a friend doing something

42:27

morally ambiguous, you might make

42:30

up an excuse for why

42:32

they did that in order to

42:34

read their behaviors as fitting

42:36

with your narrative of being

42:38

friends. And so it's very

42:40

interesting that we see this asymmetry

42:42

in how people are attributing motives underlying

42:45

the very same actions in very

42:47

different ways depending on whether those acts

42:49

are being presented as performed by

42:51

people on our side or people on

42:53

the other side. You

42:58

know, I'm reminded of a conversation we

43:00

had some time ago with the linguist

43:03

Deborah Tannen. She says it can be

43:05

hard to recognize someone's intentions, and

43:07

so it's worth assuming that their

43:09

intentions are good because it makes for

43:11

smoother conversations. I think

43:13

it's really useful for both relationships

43:15

and also for ourselves to give

43:17

others around us the benefit of

43:20

the doubt. I think it makes

43:22

for smoother social interactions and also

43:24

for happier selves. told

43:27

my students is that if you,

43:29

you know, have a bad interaction

43:31

with someone, chances are they're not

43:33

trying to offend you or insult

43:35

you. Maybe they're having a bad

43:37

day. Maybe they didn't get enough

43:39

sleep. And I tell them to

43:41

sort of think about our one

43:44

-on -one interactions in the same context

43:46

that if we have a bad

43:48

conversation, it's probably because, you

43:50

know, I am feeling bad that

43:52

I yelled at a kid. that morning

43:54

and has nothing to do with

43:56

their paper or their project. And

43:59

so again, we come back

44:01

to this idea of giving people

44:03

the benefit of the doubt

44:05

and taking intentions into consideration. I

44:08

also think about times when I'm on

44:10

the road and I get upset when

44:13

other drivers cut me off. There's

44:16

really nothing that I can do about

44:18

it aside from give them the benefit of

44:20

the doubt because I know that when

44:22

I'm the one who's speeding or cutting other

44:24

people off, usually it's because my three -year

44:26

-old in the backseat says she needs to

44:28

go to the potty or because we're

44:30

rushing to an event and we're late. So

44:33

to be able to extend that to

44:35

other people, both strangers

44:37

and the people that we interact

44:39

with on a regular basis, I

44:41

think just makes for happier interactions

44:43

all around. Isn't it

44:45

really hard to do though, Leanne?

44:47

I feel like even as I

44:49

seek compassion and empathy from other

44:51

people, it's hard for me to give

44:53

them the compassion and empathy that they seek.

44:55

So there's a real paradox here. It's

44:57

really hard. It's really hard

45:00

to take that step back and

45:02

think about what are the

45:04

situational stresses and influences that could

45:06

be leading to other people's

45:08

actions, whereas it's sometimes easier to

45:10

see those external pressures on

45:12

our own selves and lives and

45:14

interactions. And so if we're

45:16

able to pause in the midst

45:18

of a tricky interaction and

45:20

think about what that other person

45:22

is trying to do or

45:24

not trying to do, again, that

45:26

will lead to much smoother, much

45:29

more positive interactions and

45:31

ultimately relationships. Lian

45:38

Yang is a psychologist and

45:40

neuroscientist at Boston College. Lian,

45:42

thank you so much for joining me today

45:44

on Hidden Brain. Thank you so much, Shankar. After

45:50

the break, your questions answered. Our

45:52

segment where we bring back recent guests

45:54

of the show to answer listeners'

45:56

follow -up questions about their work. In

45:58

this edition of Your Questions

46:00

Answered, Fred Luskin responds to listener

46:03

stories of grudges and forgiveness. How

46:05

can I tell whether what I'm

46:07

doing, which is just I no

46:09

longer interact with her, I don't

46:12

speak to her anymore? Whether

46:14

that's truly a grudge

46:16

or is it just

46:18

me keeping myself safe?

46:22

That's when we come back. You're listening

46:24

to Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedanta.

46:41

This is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar

46:43

Vedantan. Think about the

46:45

last time someone really hurt you. Maybe

46:47

it was a friend who betrayed your confidence. A

46:50

colleague who took credit for your work. A

46:53

business partner who cheated you. How

46:57

long did that hurt stay with you? How

46:59

often did you turn it over in

47:01

your mind feeling a fresh wave of pain

47:03

and anger as though it happened just

47:05

yesterday? The

47:08

hurt we experience when someone breaks our

47:10

trust is a natural emotional response. But these

47:12

emotions can start to eat at us

47:14

if we hold on to them for too

47:16

long. We

47:18

recently discussed this with psychologist Fred

47:20

Luskin, who studies forgiveness and

47:23

grudges as the director of the

47:25

Stanford University Forgiveness Project. If

47:28

you missed our initial conversation with Fred,

47:30

you can find it in this podcast

47:32

feed. It's the episode titled, No Heart

47:34

Feelings. Today, Fred

47:36

returns to answer your questions about grudges

47:38

and how we can come to terms

47:41

with the wrongs done to us. Fred

47:43

Luskin, welcome back to Hidden Brain. Thank

47:46

you. Nice to see you

47:48

again. One of the

47:50

things that makes grudges psychologically interesting is

47:52

that they involve a certain amount of

47:54

mental storytelling. We weave our own personal

47:56

narrative of a person and a wrong

47:58

that they've done to us. You've

48:00

said that grudges can be difficult to

48:02

shake because we often start to combine

48:04

different threads of various grievance stories, and

48:06

these stories start to weave together and

48:08

create a sort of meta -narrative. How

48:11

does this happen, Fred? We're

48:13

constantly trying to make meaning

48:15

out of the world, and

48:17

we have different levels of

48:19

meaning -making. We have,

48:22

like, small things, you know,

48:24

that this person said something

48:26

unkind to me. And then

48:28

we have these meta narratives

48:30

that say, you know, this

48:33

isn't safe. And remember when

48:35

your dad did what he

48:37

did to you 20 years

48:39

ago, which set you up

48:42

upon a life which will

48:44

not be safe. It makes

48:46

it hard to just be

48:48

with the experience and deal

48:50

with the actual insult or

48:53

not. as it's happening or

48:55

cognitively or emotionally with the

48:57

best strategies. You know

48:59

I was talking recently

49:01

with a memory researcher and

49:03

she said something really interesting

49:06

which is that our memories

49:08

tend to produce whatever our

49:10

current emotional states are. In

49:12

other words, if we are happy, it

49:14

tends to be easier to pull

49:16

happy memories from our brain. And

49:18

when we are sad, it's easier

49:21

to pull sad memories from our

49:23

brain. And I'm wondering if the

49:25

same thing in some ways happens

49:27

with our grievances. So in other

49:29

words, someone does something wrong to

49:31

me, and then all of a

49:33

sudden it becomes so much easier

49:35

for me to remember all the

49:37

other times this person has also

49:39

done something bad to me or

49:41

said something unkind to me. If

49:43

you're happy, then your brain and

49:45

nervous system make available to you

49:47

all the other lovely things in

49:49

your life so that you get

49:51

to think now wow the last

49:54

time I was happy I went

49:56

and hugged my partner so should

49:58

I do that again from the

50:00

painful side look at how brilliant

50:02

it is to give you access to

50:05

other times that you suffered

50:07

and maybe to look through

50:09

what were the strategies that

50:11

I used that helped us.

50:14

The problem is when

50:17

it crowds out

50:19

other memories, then

50:21

you actually lose access

50:23

to thinking rather

50:25

than gain access to

50:27

thinking. But the

50:30

original kind of

50:32

substratum is really

50:34

smart. I mean,

50:36

you can think about it almost from

50:38

an evolutionary standpoint, which is that if

50:40

someone does something bad to me, if

50:42

another animal does something bad to me,

50:44

it's very useful to remember that this

50:46

animal has harmed me. And it might

50:48

actually be useful to remember the other

50:50

times this other person or this other

50:52

animal has harmed me, because it tells

50:54

me now, steer clear of this person, give

50:57

them a wide berth. But what

50:59

was very useful, perhaps, in our

51:01

evolutionary history when it came to

51:04

dealing with other animals and with

51:06

predators might be less effective in

51:08

the modern workplace, for example, where

51:10

we're constantly interacting with colleagues. And

51:12

now we remember a small slight. Maybe

51:14

it was an accidental slight. But that now

51:16

compounds itself with all the other slights

51:19

that we start to remember. And now we

51:21

form this metanarrative about how a colleague

51:23

of ours doesn't like us or we don't

51:25

get along with them. I'm

51:27

gonna add two things. It's

51:29

not helpful. It's essential. That

51:32

if there is

51:34

pain, difficulty, hostility,

51:38

mistreatment, you

51:40

have to remember it. You have

51:42

to try to process it and

51:44

deal with it. That's

51:46

not just optional. It's

51:48

like essential. The

51:51

second piece, what I think

51:53

you left out of the workplace,

51:55

is the staggering amount

51:57

of distraction that people

52:00

have now. So

52:02

not only are they

52:04

in a workplace

52:06

with maybe more interactions

52:08

and more possible

52:10

difficulties, their minds

52:12

are frazzled even before

52:14

they deal with anybody

52:17

by emails, by texts,

52:19

by relentless checking, by

52:21

social media, by

52:23

all of the just

52:25

demands on their

52:27

attention. So they

52:30

come into work

52:32

already with a higher

52:34

level of arousal

52:36

because they're so used

52:38

to getting stimulated

52:40

by this relentless attention

52:42

and, you know, like,

52:44

I'm at you all day

52:46

long. So that has to

52:48

be factored in there as well.

52:52

So we received a number of listener

52:54

comments and questions from people who

52:56

decided to sever a relationship with someone

52:58

else. Here's a question we

53:00

received from a listener named Lydia. I

53:03

have been living with

53:05

what I imagine can

53:07

be called a grudge

53:09

towards my sister for

53:11

the past almost eight

53:13

years. How can I

53:16

tell whether what I'm doing, which

53:18

is just I no longer interact

53:20

with her, I don't speak to

53:22

her anymore. Whether that's

53:24

truly a grudge or

53:26

is it just me

53:28

keeping myself safe and

53:30

my peace of mind?

53:33

I can't imagine that

53:36

all transgressions need to

53:38

result in forgiveness and

53:40

the ability to continue

53:42

the relationship as though

53:44

nothing happened. Or

53:46

am I wrong about that? So

53:49

what do you think Fred?

53:52

I can see in some

53:54

ways both sides of this

53:56

equation I might distance myself

53:58

from someone because I'm holding

54:00

a grudge But I'm also

54:03

could distance myself from someone

54:05

because I've thought about this

54:07

relationship and I don't want

54:09

to continue it any longer

54:11

I'm gonna add a third

54:14

piece often we Move away

54:16

from somebody because we can't

54:18

handle the disturbance, suffering, stress

54:20

that come up in us

54:22

and we use distancing as

54:25

a self -regulation strategy. So

54:28

if my sister

54:30

causes me every time

54:32

I'm near her

54:34

to feel anxious, angry,

54:37

unsettled, Sometimes

54:39

if I don't have

54:41

sufficient skill at going

54:43

inward and rebalancing or

54:45

whatever, I'll just keep

54:47

them away to try

54:49

to manage my own

54:51

reactivity. That's incredibly

54:53

common. On

54:55

the polarities that the

54:57

listener sent you, it's

55:00

not so simple. One

55:02

of the mistakes that

55:04

she presented was You

55:07

can forgive someone and

55:09

choose not to have contact

55:12

with them. It's not

55:14

either or. She could look

55:16

at her sister and

55:18

say, whatever you

55:20

did, I'm not

55:22

holding anything towards you.

55:24

My work clean. But

55:27

enough is enough. You go

55:29

your way. I go mine.

55:31

But there's no bitterness in

55:33

me. There's just. You

55:35

know, we tried this for

55:38

40 years. It didn't work

55:40

for me and have a

55:42

lovely life for a while

55:44

and thank you. So they're

55:46

not mutually exclusive. What

55:50

I would suggest is

55:52

when you're dealing with

55:54

your sister and like

55:57

you want to see

55:59

if you can rehearse

56:01

even connecting with her,

56:03

like are you able

56:06

to create a bond

56:08

or an outreach even

56:10

just in practice? If

56:14

not that significant information

56:16

that this may go

56:18

really deep. Second,

56:20

on the other side

56:22

of it, if you get

56:25

into a really quiet Clean

56:27

gentle space like you know

56:29

you're lying on the beach

56:32

in Hawaii. It's 92 degrees

56:34

and you just got out

56:36

of the water. If

56:38

you're inside still tell

56:40

you that no this

56:43

person is not safe

56:45

for you. It's probably

56:47

deeply in you that

56:49

that's what you feel.

56:52

But if it's just

56:54

that every time you

56:56

think of joining or

56:58

connecting, you get anxious

57:00

or upset, that may

57:02

not be enough inner

57:04

guidance to follow long term.

57:08

You know, we like to think that

57:10

when we forgive someone, our relationship with

57:12

them is going to come out stronger

57:14

on the other end. Unfortunately, that's not

57:16

always the case. Here's a question we

57:18

received from listener Sue. I'm

57:21

just wondering how

57:23

you actually forgive someone

57:25

if it's a

57:28

person who has pretty

57:30

strong narcissistic tendencies

57:32

would never admit to

57:34

have done anything

57:37

wrong. This is

57:39

my mom. She's since

57:41

passed. And, you

57:43

know, I am doing therapy

57:45

and I've done the radical

57:47

acceptance and all that. But

57:49

I imagine, you know, if she

57:52

were alive and if I were to

57:54

say I forgive you, she would

57:56

probably burst out laughing and say, you

57:58

know, I haven't done anything wrong. And

58:01

so I'm wondering technically

58:03

how you actually forgive someone

58:05

who would never have

58:07

admitted they did anything wrong.

58:10

You know, if what I

58:12

heard from her, if

58:14

the word narcissist

58:16

tendencies, is real and

58:19

not just a becoming

58:21

a cultural way of describing

58:23

people that we don't

58:25

like what they did. But

58:28

if somebody has

58:30

real narcissistic tendencies, you

58:32

can't expect a full

58:34

reciprocal relationship from them.

58:37

You simply can't. So

58:40

within that context, any

58:42

forgiveness is just for

58:44

your own peace of

58:46

mind. So that

58:48

you will calm your

58:50

brain down, you will open

58:52

your heart back up, never

58:54

with the thought that that's

58:56

going to improve them or

58:59

have them see you as

59:01

you are, because a

59:03

real narcissist can't see you

59:05

as they are. They see

59:07

themselves. Again,

59:10

no, there's this confusion.

59:13

between reconciliation

59:15

and forgiveness. You

59:18

can forgive someone

59:20

who's unrepentant simply because

59:22

you don't want

59:24

to carry that in

59:26

you. You make

59:28

no assumption that that

59:30

will change them. Now,

59:34

somebody who has weaker

59:36

narcissistic tendencies and

59:38

you forgive them and

59:40

you show up sometimes

59:43

that does influence them

59:45

to reduce their side

59:47

of it, but there

59:49

is no guarantee. So

59:52

in Sue's case, Fred, her mother

59:54

is now dead. She

59:56

may or may not have been a narcissist, but

59:58

there's no way that she's going to be able to

1:00:00

accept that she did something wrong because she's not

1:00:02

with us anymore. But I think

1:00:04

even there, the point that you're making

1:00:07

still stands, which is from Sue's point of

1:00:09

view, her forgiving her mother might

1:00:11

be good for Sue, regardless of what

1:00:13

her mother may have said or done. We

1:00:16

can't know what will

1:00:18

be good for someone

1:00:20

else, and we

1:00:22

can't know their perception

1:00:24

of what happened. We

1:00:27

can only be as

1:00:29

clear as possible about what

1:00:31

our choices are and

1:00:33

how our responses were. We've

1:00:36

talked on many episodes of Hidden Brain

1:00:38

about political divisions and how to engage

1:00:40

with people in our lives who hold

1:00:42

political beliefs that are different from our

1:00:44

own. Here's a message we received from

1:00:46

a listener named Ezra. For

1:00:49

years now, I've

1:00:51

been holding a

1:00:53

grudge with my

1:00:55

family regarding their

1:00:57

political stance, especially

1:01:00

around queer and

1:01:02

transgender rights. My

1:01:05

parents have voted for

1:01:07

Trump and I cannot

1:01:09

tolerate the emotional cognitive

1:01:11

dissonance of having parents

1:01:13

who on one hand

1:01:16

love me very much

1:01:18

and on the other

1:01:20

hand are literally voting

1:01:22

to take my rights

1:01:24

away as a US

1:01:26

citizen. I

1:01:28

am stuck between wanting

1:01:30

to forgive wanting

1:01:33

to protect myself, feeling

1:01:36

fear and rage

1:01:38

and love altogether.

1:01:41

I would love some advice

1:01:43

and help for how to

1:01:45

move forward in a way

1:01:47

that feels grounded, loving,

1:01:51

protective, respectful.

1:01:55

What do you think, Fred? What advice would you give

1:01:57

to Ezra? What

1:01:59

a lovely description

1:02:01

of the poles of

1:02:03

a mind that

1:02:05

is trying to integrate

1:02:07

the heart but

1:02:09

knows this is tough

1:02:11

sledding. The

1:02:13

real question is not

1:02:15

should we forgive, but

1:02:19

what's our most skillful

1:02:21

action? And what's

1:02:23

the best mindset

1:02:25

for moving ahead?

1:02:28

Like, forgiveness is a

1:02:30

pathway to the

1:02:32

best mindset. It's not

1:02:34

the mindset in

1:02:36

and of itself. So

1:02:39

when you're in

1:02:41

a situation where

1:02:43

you're vulnerable, like,

1:02:45

you know, that, you know,

1:02:48

listener just called in

1:02:50

and legitimately afraid. That's

1:02:53

not trivial, and

1:02:56

that can't be, you know, swept

1:02:58

under the rug by saying,

1:03:00

well, I forgive them. It's not

1:03:02

that simple. But

1:03:04

at the same

1:03:06

time, we recognize that

1:03:08

simply hating or

1:03:11

demonizing whatever it is

1:03:13

we think caused

1:03:15

our vulnerability, after

1:03:17

a while, will diminish

1:03:19

our own ability to

1:03:21

take skillful action because

1:03:23

we're tired from our

1:03:26

anger and our resentment

1:03:28

and clouds our judgment. So

1:03:31

part of it

1:03:33

is grieving and admitting

1:03:36

our vulnerability and

1:03:38

loss. Secondly,

1:03:41

joining together with other

1:03:43

people who have similar experiences,

1:03:47

so there's some

1:03:49

strength in numbers. And

1:03:52

then really focusing

1:03:54

on within the

1:03:56

legitimate experience of

1:03:59

our life, what's

1:04:01

the most skillful

1:04:03

action? How do we

1:04:05

behave in a way that

1:04:07

will get us closer to

1:04:09

what our real goals are? and

1:04:12

not just have us

1:04:14

dominated by fear or resentment.

1:04:17

That's the piece that

1:04:19

forgiveness clears out. Say

1:04:23

more about this idea when you say

1:04:25

that the goal is to come up with

1:04:27

skillful action. How do we

1:04:29

know what skillful action is for Ezra in

1:04:31

this case? Well,

1:04:33

it can be a couple

1:04:35

of things. One, that

1:04:38

the goal or

1:04:40

motivation you align

1:04:42

with even when you're

1:04:44

not upset or even

1:04:46

when you're not meeting

1:04:49

with other people in

1:04:51

like shared outrage. But

1:04:53

yeah, that's really what

1:04:55

I believe in. Two,

1:04:59

is it experimentally

1:05:01

kind of

1:05:04

verifiable? Like, I try

1:05:06

this. Does it help? If

1:05:09

it doesn't help, Do I

1:05:11

admit that I was not doing

1:05:13

the best thing and go

1:05:15

back and try something else? Do

1:05:18

I maybe ask for

1:05:20

advice as to other

1:05:23

people's experience? Do I

1:05:25

read up about past

1:05:27

skillful action? Is my

1:05:29

mind open to problem

1:05:32

-solving or is it

1:05:34

motivated by resentment and

1:05:36

revenge? Those are

1:05:39

very different motivations, and

1:05:41

they give us different

1:05:43

minds to evaluate the outcome.

1:05:47

I'm wondering whether any research has

1:05:49

been done about the utility

1:05:51

of actually asking the person who

1:05:53

we feel has harmed us

1:05:55

their advice on what we should

1:05:57

do. So in Ezra's case,

1:05:59

for example, I'm wondering what

1:06:01

if he went to his parents and said, Here's

1:06:03

what I'm feeling. I know that you love

1:06:05

me very much. I also feel like you have

1:06:07

betrayed me. I'm struggling between my

1:06:09

feelings of my love for you and

1:06:11

my admiration for you and my resentment

1:06:14

for you. Help me figure out what

1:06:16

I should do with these feelings. You'd

1:06:19

have to be

1:06:21

incredibly emotionally competent to

1:06:23

say that. And

1:06:26

you would have

1:06:28

to have really emotionally

1:06:30

competent parents to

1:06:32

hear that but it

1:06:34

is a phenomenal

1:06:37

strategy and you could

1:06:39

extrapolate that strategy

1:06:41

to so many interpersonal

1:06:43

difficulties just so

1:06:45

many you could imagine

1:06:47

how many intimate

1:06:50

partners. Could bring

1:06:52

that to each other,

1:06:54

you know, you

1:06:56

said this it hurt

1:06:58

me part of

1:07:00

me wants to strike

1:07:02

back at you

1:07:04

part of me wants

1:07:06

to understand you

1:07:08

but underneath what you

1:07:10

said are some

1:07:12

incredibly skillful means of

1:07:15

I'm responsible for

1:07:17

my emotional state I

1:07:19

need to put what I'm

1:07:21

feeling into words, and

1:07:23

I trust people

1:07:26

enough to share my

1:07:28

vulnerability with them. None

1:07:31

of those are easy for

1:07:33

people to access. I'm

1:07:49

Shankar Vedanta. This

1:08:02

is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedanta.

1:08:05

This is Your Question's Answer, our segment

1:08:07

in which we bring back past guests

1:08:09

of the show to respond to listeners'

1:08:11

follow -up thoughts and questions about their work.

1:08:14

Today we're talking with Fred Luskin. He's

1:08:16

a psychologist and director of the

1:08:18

Stanford University Forgiveness Project. Fred

1:08:20

is also the author of the books,

1:08:22

Forgive for Good, a proven prescription for

1:08:24

health and happiness, and Forgive

1:08:27

for Love, the missing ingredient for a

1:08:29

healthy and lasting relationship. Let's

1:08:33

turn again to listener questions. Our next one

1:08:35

is from a listener named Laura, and

1:08:37

it's a version of a question that we have

1:08:39

received from a number of listeners. What

1:08:41

if the person that you

1:08:44

need to forgive is actually

1:08:46

yourself? Could you

1:08:48

apply the same concepts? What

1:08:52

do you think Fred? Is it possible

1:08:54

to essentially hold a grudge against yourself? The

1:08:57

answer to that is yes.

1:09:00

There's a couple of steps

1:09:02

that you know people need

1:09:04

to look at. One

1:09:06

of them is to

1:09:08

legitimately acknowledge what

1:09:11

you have done. That

1:09:13

there really is

1:09:16

no self -forgiveness

1:09:18

without some acknowledgement, some

1:09:21

remorse. You

1:09:23

know, if you look

1:09:25

at Truth and Reconciliation in

1:09:27

South Africa, it's just

1:09:30

a general model. You

1:09:32

know, there was a

1:09:34

pretty strong public forum for

1:09:37

a boy did

1:09:39

we do bad and

1:09:41

this can't be

1:09:43

hidden so the first

1:09:45

step is to

1:09:47

whatever degree possible admit

1:09:49

it to yourself

1:09:51

if safe admit it

1:09:53

to anybody you've

1:09:55

harmed and allow oneself

1:09:58

to feel remorse There

1:10:00

is literature, you know,

1:10:03

research on the value

1:10:05

of a sincere apology,

1:10:08

which is, I did

1:10:10

it. My bad hurts

1:10:12

you. I make that

1:10:14

link. I'm sorry. And

1:10:16

if I can, I won't do it

1:10:19

again. It is

1:10:21

hard for many of

1:10:23

us to have the humility

1:10:25

of a sincere apology. The

1:10:28

last step is taken

1:10:30

from the wisdom of

1:10:32

the 12 -step programs,

1:10:34

which is whenever you

1:10:36

can make it right, make

1:10:39

amends. Now, if

1:10:41

the person that you've

1:10:43

harmed is just you, like

1:10:46

you didn't harm anybody

1:10:48

else, but you got drunk

1:10:50

or you had a

1:10:52

ridiculous sexual escapade that harmed

1:10:54

your life, It's

1:10:57

helpful to share with

1:10:59

one or two trusted

1:11:01

people as an offering.

1:11:04

You do make a kind

1:11:06

of inner mea culpa, but

1:11:09

the amends is you

1:11:11

make sure it

1:11:13

doesn't happen again. You

1:11:16

go to therapy, you

1:11:18

go to 12 -step programs, you

1:11:21

take classes in anger

1:11:23

management, whatever it is. but

1:11:26

you need to do something

1:11:28

positive. When

1:11:30

those preconditions

1:11:32

are met, you

1:11:35

have absolutely no

1:11:37

need for negative self

1:11:39

-talk or bad feelings.

1:11:41

You have done

1:11:44

the basic requirements of

1:11:46

self -forgiveness. If you

1:11:48

have to, you go see a therapist for

1:11:50

a couple sessions. But you

1:11:52

do not need at all

1:11:54

that negative emotion once you've

1:11:56

done those steps. In

1:11:59

our initial conversation, Fred, we mostly focused on

1:12:01

how we can let go of grudges against

1:12:03

people who have wronged us. But

1:12:05

we've all probably been in situations where

1:12:07

we are the wrong doer, where

1:12:09

we have betrayed someone in our life.

1:12:11

Here's a question we received about

1:12:13

that from listener Booma. I

1:12:15

want to know how you

1:12:17

helped a loved one. get over

1:12:19

or let go of their

1:12:21

grudge or heart feelings or bitter

1:12:23

feelings against you. Let's

1:12:26

say you betrayed them.

1:12:29

A terrible incident happened in their lives

1:12:31

and you were not there for them. You

1:12:34

realized it. It's a mistake. You

1:12:37

regret it. You offered them

1:12:39

an apology and later you

1:12:41

have tried to be as best

1:12:43

as you can for them

1:12:45

and been supportive in their life.

1:12:48

But it looks like, you know,

1:12:51

we both are still reading from the

1:12:53

aftermath of the betrayal. I

1:12:55

want to help this loved one

1:12:57

and I want to help myself get

1:12:59

better. Do you have any advice? I

1:13:03

mean, that is a

1:13:05

very tough situation when

1:13:08

somebody feels genuine remorse,

1:13:10

wants to make it

1:13:12

right and is stymied.

1:13:14

I have heard that

1:13:16

multiple times. There's

1:13:20

two things that come to

1:13:22

my mind immediately. One

1:13:25

is some degree

1:13:27

of self -examination. What

1:13:30

was it that led me

1:13:32

to not be there when they

1:13:34

needed me? Is

1:13:36

there something characterologic

1:13:38

in me? Was

1:13:41

I frightened? Was

1:13:43

I preoccupied? What

1:13:45

was the flaw or

1:13:47

weakness in me? So

1:13:50

that I can

1:13:52

address it or I

1:13:54

can show this

1:13:56

person deep vulnerability and

1:13:59

reflection when I

1:14:01

talk to them. Not

1:14:03

just that I'm

1:14:05

sorry. but it

1:14:07

caused me to recognize

1:14:09

how hard something like

1:14:12

this is, or, wow,

1:14:14

I really struggle when

1:14:16

somebody's needs are way

1:14:18

bigger than mine, or

1:14:20

I may not always

1:14:23

hear at the moment

1:14:25

the depth of somebody's

1:14:27

pain. That would be

1:14:29

one, you know, first of all,

1:14:31

it helps her. but

1:14:34

secondly it creates

1:14:36

a shared pain. The

1:14:39

second thing is

1:14:41

sometimes we can't turn

1:14:43

back time and

1:14:45

there is a forgiveness

1:14:48

element there that

1:14:50

you know it's up

1:14:52

to you and

1:14:54

I respect as best

1:14:56

I can how

1:14:59

you deal with your

1:15:01

suffering. The

1:15:03

third piece

1:15:05

is now that

1:15:07

I have

1:15:09

learned this about

1:15:11

the dangers, or

1:15:14

even if I didn't

1:15:16

do anything deliberate, even

1:15:18

if it was just

1:15:20

a bad confluence of

1:15:22

events where absolutely nobody

1:15:24

is at fault, one

1:15:27

of the things that

1:15:29

she can do is move

1:15:31

ahead with a

1:15:33

renewed attention to

1:15:35

make sure that this

1:15:37

never happens again

1:15:40

so that her radar

1:15:42

are more finely

1:15:44

attuned to other people's

1:15:46

pain and she

1:15:49

can even use this

1:15:51

as a teaching

1:15:53

to tell other people

1:15:56

never, you know,

1:15:58

never underestimate when

1:16:00

people are suffering

1:16:02

how vulnerable or how

1:16:04

reactive they might

1:16:06

be. So she can

1:16:08

use her loss

1:16:10

to help others. That's

1:16:13

sometimes the best we can

1:16:15

do. We

1:16:17

received an interesting question from listener

1:16:19

Richard. He wanted to know

1:16:21

about the factors that might affect our ability

1:16:23

to forgive someone. Here he is.

1:16:26

Do you think someone having

1:16:28

more positive emotional states like

1:16:30

the joy of their first

1:16:32

child may increase their likelihood

1:16:34

of being more able and

1:16:36

willing to forgive. I

1:16:38

mean, if you ask

1:16:41

me about the factors that

1:16:43

influence the ability to

1:16:45

forgive, research has

1:16:47

picked up a few,

1:16:49

not that many. One

1:16:53

of them is

1:16:55

gratitude. and it may be

1:16:57

a surprise to you, but

1:16:59

people who see the good in

1:17:01

their lives are more capable of

1:17:03

letting go of the bad. And

1:17:06

that is a reasonably

1:17:09

robust finding that, you

1:17:11

know, if you wake up in

1:17:13

the morning and it's a beautiful day

1:17:15

out and you're delighted that you

1:17:17

had breakfast and you look at a

1:17:19

picture of your kid and you

1:17:21

go, I got a good life, then

1:17:24

when somebody calls you in

1:17:26

his rude, you're going to be

1:17:28

more likely to hold it

1:17:30

better. Now, let

1:17:32

me add one or two

1:17:34

other things to that. A

1:17:37

home where you

1:17:40

were raised, where

1:17:42

forgiveness is modeled,

1:17:45

is more likely to imprint it

1:17:47

on your brain than not. Did

1:17:50

you see your parents

1:17:53

forgive each other? Did

1:17:55

you see your parents

1:17:57

forgive you? That

1:17:59

is a huge modeling

1:18:01

that even can be

1:18:03

pre -verbal, you know,

1:18:05

like, oh my gosh,

1:18:07

I saw this. Second, our

1:18:11

nervous systems have something to

1:18:13

do with our ability to

1:18:15

forgive. There's a

1:18:17

quality of human

1:18:20

nervous system arousal where

1:18:22

people are called

1:18:24

hot reactors. They they

1:18:26

just you know, you've

1:18:28

met them they get aroused

1:18:30

very quickly. Those

1:18:32

people struggle like

1:18:34

crazy to forgive

1:18:36

because the adrenaline

1:18:38

just comes so

1:18:40

quickly and so

1:18:42

strongly and pulsates

1:18:44

into anger. that

1:18:47

that is a real

1:18:49

challenge. The last thing

1:18:51

that I will say is, it

1:18:54

also depends on the amount

1:18:56

of practice you put in.

1:18:58

You know, if you

1:19:00

practice just normally, when

1:19:03

you're cut off in

1:19:05

traffic, when somebody cuts

1:19:07

in front of you in

1:19:09

the supermarket line, when the

1:19:11

airplane is late and you're

1:19:13

gonna miss your connecting flight,

1:19:17

You're practicing what you might need

1:19:19

a year down the road when

1:19:21

somebody doesn't behave right Hmm. I

1:19:23

think one interesting theme that's come

1:19:25

up in many of these questions

1:19:27

and also came up in our

1:19:29

initial conversation Fred is that I

1:19:32

think when people think about grudges

1:19:34

They sometimes imagine themselves to be

1:19:36

in the position of judges. They're

1:19:38

asking themselves I'm a judge. Here

1:19:40

are the facts before me. Do

1:19:42

these facts justify forgiveness? Should I

1:19:45

hold on to the grudge? Should

1:19:47

I grant forgiveness? Should I not

1:19:49

grant forgiveness? And that is

1:19:51

one way of thinking about it. But

1:19:53

I think the point that you're making

1:19:55

is that a judge is making a

1:19:57

decision that really is in the public

1:19:59

good. They're deciding, should this person be

1:20:01

sentenced, should this person be let free?

1:20:03

And you're trying to make a judgment

1:20:05

that's in the public good. When it

1:20:08

comes to the forgiveness that you're talking

1:20:10

about, Fred, you're already talking about what's

1:20:12

in people's own good. And you're basically

1:20:14

saying, is it in my interest to

1:20:16

actually let this go, regardless of whether

1:20:18

it's in the other person's interest or

1:20:20

anyone else's interest? I'm gonna

1:20:22

add two things to that. One,

1:20:25

there's also the statute of limitations.

1:20:30

which most people forget

1:20:32

about, you know, even

1:20:34

if they're bringing an argument before

1:20:36

the judge, most

1:20:38

cases have five,

1:20:40

seven -year statute of

1:20:42

limitations. And

1:20:44

so that's the water

1:20:46

dampening on that

1:20:48

analogy, even though it's

1:20:50

true. Secondly,

1:20:53

forgiveness is not just

1:20:55

for the self. but

1:20:59

it's for the current

1:21:01

important relationships in one's

1:21:03

life that one wants

1:21:05

to maintain and grow.

1:21:09

And again, let's just use

1:21:11

the judge analogy. So

1:21:13

let's say that somebody

1:21:16

does something that's wrong. There's

1:21:18

nothing wrong with imposing a

1:21:21

one week sentence on them,

1:21:23

but then you also want

1:21:25

to be merciful. And

1:21:27

that's missing often

1:21:29

in the judge analogy.

1:21:33

Sometimes you let people out

1:21:35

for good behavior. Sometimes

1:21:37

you commute their sentence. So

1:21:40

we're looking at

1:21:42

a complex life and

1:21:44

we want to

1:21:47

be careful not to

1:21:49

lose the complex

1:21:51

life on our over

1:21:53

fixation with a

1:21:55

negative experience. Fred

1:22:02

Luskin is a psychologist and director

1:22:04

of the Stanford University Forgiveness Project. He

1:22:06

is the author of Forgive for

1:22:08

Good, A Proven Prescription for Health and

1:22:10

Happiness, and Forgive for Love,

1:22:12

the missing ingredient for a healthy

1:22:14

and lasting relationship. Fred Luskin,

1:22:16

thank you so much for joining me

1:22:18

today on Hidden Brain. I thank you

1:22:20

very much. Hidden

1:22:26

Brain is produced by Hidden

1:22:28

Brain Media. Our audio production

1:22:30

team includes Annie Murphy -Paul,

1:22:32

Kristen Wong, Laura Quirelle, Ryan

1:22:34

Katz, Autumn Barnes, Andrew Chadwick,

1:22:37

and Nick Woodbury. Tara

1:22:39

Boyle is our executive producer. I'm

1:22:41

Hidden Brain's executive editor. We

1:22:43

had voice acting in today's episode

1:22:45

from Clara and Rose Dubois and

1:22:47

Scarlett McNally. We

1:22:52

end today with a story from

1:22:54

our sister podcast, My Unsung Hero.

1:22:56

This My Unsung Hero segment is

1:22:58

brought to you by Discover. In

1:23:02

2022, David Jefferson's

1:23:04

wife Jill was diagnosed with

1:23:06

cancer. Jill didn't have

1:23:08

many close friends, but one of

1:23:11

her former coworkers, a woman named

1:23:13

Nicole Kyle, started coming by to

1:23:15

try and cheer her up. Soon,

1:23:17

Nicole became a consistent comfort to

1:23:19

the couple. So in

1:23:21

2024, it didn't surprise David

1:23:23

when Nicole offered to help him

1:23:25

get Jill home after a hospital

1:23:27

stay. What did surprise him was

1:23:29

the lengths she went to once they

1:23:31

got Jill home. David

1:23:33

recorded his story in a park where he

1:23:35

and Jill hoped to one day hike together,

1:23:37

so you'll hear some wind in the background. Nicole

1:23:41

and I got Jill home. It

1:23:43

was during a winter snowstorm. The

1:23:45

pipes had frozen in the house,

1:23:47

but we got them thawed out.

1:23:50

Nicole and I gave Jill a

1:23:52

shower and put her to

1:23:54

bed. Some

1:23:56

hours later, Nicole showed

1:23:58

up with a bag. Show

1:24:01

me which room you want me

1:24:03

to sleep in and I'll stay until

1:24:06

the end with Jill. Nicole,

1:24:09

in a very unselfish

1:24:11

way, took care

1:24:13

of the laundry, food, house

1:24:16

cleaning. She invited

1:24:18

her friends to bring over food

1:24:20

along with some of the food

1:24:22

my friends were bringing. Nicole

1:24:25

would get up in

1:24:27

the middle the night

1:24:29

with me and help

1:24:31

roll Jill over and

1:24:34

attend to her private

1:24:36

needs as only another

1:24:38

woman could. Nicole

1:24:40

was a steady presence.

1:24:43

She never panicked. She

1:24:46

always had a smile and

1:24:48

willing to do whatever what was

1:24:50

next. Nicole

1:24:52

helped me. See

1:24:54

Jill to the

1:24:56

end of her

1:24:58

life, which was

1:25:00

February 3rd 2024

1:25:03

Nicole was an

1:25:05

angel and the

1:25:07

time of need

1:25:09

Now forever be

1:25:11

in her debt

1:25:13

This situation called

1:25:15

for a hero

1:25:17

and Nicole Kyle

1:25:19

is my Hero

1:25:21

for life Listener

1:25:36

David Jefferson. Jill

1:25:38

and David were together for 36 years. This

1:25:44

segment of My Unsung Hero was

1:25:46

brought to you by Discover. Discover

1:25:48

believes everyone deserves to feel

1:25:50

special, and celebrates those who exhibit

1:25:52

the spirit in their communities. I'm

1:25:55

a long -standing card member myself. Learn

1:25:58

more at discover.com slash

1:26:00

credit card. I'm

1:26:08

Vedantam. see you

1:26:10

soon.

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features