Why chimps are still in the Stone Age and humans are in the Space Age

Why chimps are still in the Stone Age and humans are in the Space Age

Released Friday, 22nd November 2024
 1 person rated this episode
Why chimps are still in the Stone Age and humans are in the Space Age

Why chimps are still in the Stone Age and humans are in the Space Age

Why chimps are still in the Stone Age and humans are in the Space Age

Why chimps are still in the Stone Age and humans are in the Space Age

Friday, 22nd November 2024
 1 person rated this episode
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

This episode is brought to

0:02

you by Shopify. Forget the

0:04

frustration of picking commerce platforms

0:07

when you switch your business

0:09

to Shopify. The global commerce

0:11

platform that supercharges you're selling,

0:13

wherever you sell. With Shopify,

0:16

you'll harness the same intuitive

0:18

features, trusted apps, and powerful

0:20

analytics used by the world's

0:22

leading brands. Sign up today

0:25

for your $1 per month

0:27

trial period at Shopify.com/ tech.

0:31

Hello and welcome to the latest dose

0:34

of science news from New Scientists. I'm

0:36

Penny Sasha joined by Roanhooper. Hello. Hello.

0:38

We've got a food special this week

0:40

or maybe a consumption special is a

0:42

better way to put it. We're looking

0:44

at why some people put weight back

0:46

on after they've lost it and we're

0:48

also going to be talking about the

0:50

effect those labels next to food on

0:52

menus have, you know, the ones that

0:54

show you the calorie content. And we're

0:56

also going to hear about quite a

0:58

strange, possibly terrifying new AI study showing

1:01

that a version of chat GPT is

1:03

able to replicate people's unique

1:06

thoughts and personalities. Do I

1:08

need to sort of prod you to

1:10

show everyone that we're really human? And

1:12

you've known me long enough. Can

1:14

you? Can you? What do you think?

1:17

We're going to need a watermark or

1:19

some sort of equipment. Yeah. Well,

1:21

so we're going to go to the

1:24

future later. We're going to start

1:26

with the past, our evolutionary past,

1:28

chimpanzee culture. There's a story this

1:30

week on chimpanzee culture, and it

1:32

gets to something that's really, really

1:34

puzzled me actually, and that is

1:36

the fact that chimps have tools.

1:38

We've known that since Jane Goodall's,

1:40

you know, legendary studies 60 years

1:42

ago, and we know they have

1:44

culture, but their culture's static, right?

1:46

So they've had tools and culture

1:49

for millions of years, but they're

1:51

it's like we had the stone age

1:53

and then we invented you know controlled fire and

1:55

before you know it we're on the moon right

1:57

yeah but chimps have been stuck

1:59

in the Stone Age. Yeah,

2:01

I've never thought about it like

2:03

that. Why is that? Oh, well, so

2:05

that's the thing. No one really knew, but

2:07

we've got a clue to it this week, and

2:09

I've been talking to some primatologists about it,

2:11

and we're going to hear from them. But first,

2:13

we're going to hear from reporter Krista Leste -Lesse.

2:16

She's here to tell us about it. Hi,

2:18

Krista, welcome to the pod. Hi. So,

2:20

go on, give us an intro to this

2:22

study. So, as you say, the

2:24

question is about chimp culture, but not

2:26

so much why they're in the Stone

2:28

Age, or you might even call it

2:30

the Plant Age, since most of the

2:32

tools are made from sticks and leaves,

2:35

which historically actually made their evolution tough

2:37

to study. But rather, it's about whether

2:39

chimp cultures may evolve cumulatively, much

2:41

as human cultures do. The assumption across

2:43

many research groups has been that

2:45

they just don't, but now a big

2:47

study looking at 35 chimpanzee study

2:49

sites across Africa suggests that there is

2:51

a generational buildup of culture. Oh,

2:53

well, that's brilliant, because that's just another

2:55

assumption about chimpanzees that gets completely

2:57

thrown out. That's exciting. Yeah, and it's

3:00

not just a generational buildup, but

3:02

the idea that only we have the

3:04

ability to develop more complex cultures

3:06

over time. I mean, yes, we know

3:08

that many animals are capable of

3:10

what we call social learning, where they learn

3:12

by watching each other, and that

3:14

sort of thing can get passed on to other

3:16

animals of the same and later generation. But

3:18

that's very different from saying, okay, we've got this

3:20

one skill that we know how to do, and it

3:22

works really well. But how about improving on

3:24

that and making it better? And

3:26

for the most part, scientists have believed that

3:29

this is something that really distinguishes humans from other

3:31

species. So I spoke to Andrew

3:33

Whiten of the University of St Andrews

3:35

about this, and I asked him about what

3:37

we know about how chimp culture develops,

3:39

and here he is. Some of

3:41

the chimpanzees' most complex technologies involve what

3:43

we call tool sets, where they use

3:45

two or more tools for the job. For

3:48

example, in the Congo region, they do

3:50

this to feed on underground termite

3:52

nests. First, they get a stout stick,

3:54

and they stab it right down

3:56

through the hard earth, sometimes as far

3:58

as half a meter. to create

4:00

a tunnel down to the nest, then

4:02

they get a different thing, a

4:04

long plant stem, they strip that through

4:06

their teeth to fray the end

4:08

into a little brush. They wet that,

4:10

form it into a kind of

4:12

point, and then they carefully feed that

4:14

down the tunnel. The termites

4:16

bite the invading brush tip and

4:19

the chimpanzee then carefully pulls out the

4:21

tool and eats the termites. If

4:23

you can imagine, you're a primatologist observing

4:25

this, you can't help but suspect

4:27

that all this didn't just spring up

4:29

in one go. Instead, it's

4:32

probably a culture that has had

4:34

to be built up over stages. The

4:37

problem with that is that these

4:39

tools are all perishable. So we

4:41

don't have any record of

4:43

their past history like we

4:45

do for say the wheel

4:47

or many other examples in

4:49

human culture. And that's where

4:51

our new study comes in

4:53

because it uses modern genetics

4:56

to provide us with a kind

4:58

of time machine to look

5:00

back into the past. It's based

5:02

on the fact that in

5:04

chimpanzees, sexually mature females who

5:06

migrate to breed in other groups,

5:08

that's their way of avoiding

5:11

incest, but it spreads their genes

5:13

between groups so that over

5:15

centuries, those genes spread far and

5:17

wide but those female chimpanzees

5:19

could also spread their native cultures.

5:21

For example, if they're one of

5:23

those who have that skill to

5:25

use a tool set to get

5:27

underground termites, they might take that

5:29

to a group that's ignorant of

5:31

it. So what this new

5:33

study has done is

5:35

to measure where distant communities

5:37

share those genetic similarities

5:39

and ask whether they're also

5:41

likely to share cultural

5:43

similarities because migrating females spread

5:45

both their genes and

5:47

their cultural knowledge. And sure

5:50

enough, we found that that

5:52

was strongly the case for those

5:54

complex tool sets. It was

5:56

rather less so where only a

5:58

single tool was used compared

6:00

to... foraging without tools. So what this

6:02

suggests is that inventing those complex

6:04

tool sets is achieved only rarely,

6:06

and for a community to show

6:08

it, they usually have to have

6:10

learned it from a knowledgeable migrating

6:12

female. Yeah, so essentially they have

6:15

these multi- tools that seem to just

6:17

build up from simple tools. And the

6:19

scientists here have very cleverly to use

6:21

genetics to map the way these chimps

6:23

spread with the way a culture spreads.

6:26

And they were taking advantage of methods

6:28

that scientists have already used to study

6:30

community culture in ancient hunter-gatherer groups. Yeah,

6:32

so that's why he says that it's

6:34

effectively a time machine to look in

6:36

a window onto the past. Yeah, exactly.

6:38

And you can sort of trace the

6:41

genetic link from simple tool use to

6:43

more complex ones, because essentially what they're

6:45

seeing is that there aren't any particular

6:47

genetic links among chimps who don't use

6:49

tools. And then there are some links,

6:51

they're fairly weak, but they're still there

6:54

among those that are using simple tools.

6:56

And then we get to the complex

6:58

tools. And those are three to five

7:00

times more likely to be associated with

7:02

sharing genes among those who use them.

7:04

So we're really seeing that evolution there.

7:07

Yeah, there's lots, there's so many lots

7:09

of fascinating things about this work. And

7:11

well, just as an aside, the lead

7:13

authors, Cassandra Gunasekoram of the University of

7:15

Zurich. She was only a PhD student

7:17

when she started, and this is her

7:19

first paper, and it's in science. So.

7:22

It kind of makes you a god

7:24

among men doesn't it if you get

7:26

a nature or science paper from a

7:28

PhD. So I spoke to Cassandra as

7:30

well about the work and I asked

7:32

why it's taken us so long to

7:35

figure this out seeing as we've known

7:37

about chimps and tool use for so

7:39

long and here she is. Cumulative culture

7:41

has been studied a lot in humans

7:43

and only recently our group has worked

7:45

on cultural exchange and mobility patterns in

7:48

hunter-gatherers using similar genetic genetic data. And

7:50

that study had showed that the hunter

7:52

gathers from the west to the east

7:54

of the coast. have been

7:56

connected through migrations

7:58

between groups over the

8:00

past 5 ,000 years

8:03

at least, and

8:05

that these connections correlated

8:07

with cultural exchange

8:09

of, for example, musical

8:11

instruments. So when this

8:13

genetic data came out for chimpanzees, just

8:15

when I had started my PhD, we

8:17

wanted to try this and see if

8:19

we can find some correlation in chimpanzees

8:21

as well. I really think

8:23

that we were very lucky because previously

8:26

this data just didn't exist yet,

8:28

but now we have this huge genetic

8:30

database that comes from collected poo. When

8:33

we then actually found the correlation

8:35

between migration patterns and complex

8:37

tools in chimpanzees, this was of

8:39

course very exciting, and now

8:41

having my first PhD chapter published

8:43

in Science is just amazing,

8:45

and I am very grateful to

8:47

everyone that contributed to this

8:49

paper. Also, I am

8:51

very excited to now continue

8:53

this work, and we wanted

8:55

to quantify the differences in

8:57

migration patterns between humans and

8:59

chimpanzees, and then further analyze

9:02

the complexification of culture to

9:04

have a proper understanding of

9:06

the differences in these processes

9:08

that correlate with cumulative culture

9:10

in these two species. Yeah,

9:12

exactly. So essentially they just didn't have

9:14

the doubt to do this kind of

9:16

research. That's really why it has taken

9:18

so long to get there. But now

9:20

our own cumulative culture, I guess you

9:22

could say, has led us to be

9:24

able to do the genetic analysis and

9:26

even create these huge data banks of

9:29

genetic information and make it accessible to

9:31

scientists around the world. Before that, as

9:33

Andrew was saying, they just didn't have

9:35

the chimps archeological artifacts around because the

9:37

vast majority were made from plants that

9:39

just degraded out of visible history. Yeah,

9:41

and actually we just had a story

9:43

about that in New Scientist about the

9:45

Botanic Age, about where our ancestors used

9:47

plant -based tools and the new discoveries that

9:49

are being made about that. But look,

9:51

there's still the question of, given

9:53

this work, so we know now

9:55

that chimps can evolve their

9:57

own cultures in a cumulative way.

10:00

that we thought only humans did. So

10:02

why are they still scrambling around

10:04

in the dirt and why haven't they

10:06

made particle accelerators yet? You know,

10:08

when people usually tackle that question, it's

10:10

about us, it's like, well, we're

10:12

better. Humans and tech. But here's a

10:14

nice way around to look at

10:16

it. And this is from Andrea Migliano.

10:18

She's a member of the research

10:20

team that put this paper out also

10:23

from Zurich. Over the millions

10:25

of years that separated hunter -gatherers

10:27

from chimpanzees in the human foraging

10:29

niche, there was strong selection

10:31

to access difficult to acquire resources.

10:33

So this meant that humans

10:35

became very dependent on to use.

10:37

While chimpanzees are proficient in

10:39

using tools and they use

10:41

it very regularly, they

10:44

are not dependent on that in

10:46

the same way. Some populations of

10:48

chimpanzees survive without the use of

10:50

any tool for foraging. On top

10:52

of that, humans also had to

10:54

become very cooperative, cooperating, for instance,

10:56

to access these resources to cooperative

10:58

hunting. And the

11:00

combination of these two

11:02

factors meant that human society

11:04

became very fluid. But

11:06

hunter -gatherers nowadays, living today,

11:09

moving very often between

11:11

camps to visit friends

11:13

and to find cooperators. And

11:15

also, of course, to

11:17

access these resources. So

11:19

this exactly process of

11:21

migration between camps

11:23

bring the possibility to recombine

11:26

information and produce new tools

11:28

and creates what we call

11:30

cumulative culture, which is this

11:32

culture that is so complex

11:34

that no single individual can

11:37

recreate from zero. So

11:39

what I think we show

11:41

in this paper that is really

11:43

cool is that the origins

11:45

of this cumulative culture process was

11:47

probably already in the common

11:49

ancestor between humans and chimpanzees. Chimpanzees

11:51

have very limited to use

11:53

and also very limited between group

11:56

migration, but they do have

11:58

both. And this migration is already...

12:00

for the beginning of cumulative culture to

12:02

evolve there. Now when the penny

12:04

dropped for me, the penny dropped.

12:06

No, yeah I never had that

12:08

one before. I didn't mean to.

12:10

Was when it said that chimps

12:12

aren't dependent. on tools like we

12:14

are. So they haven't needed to

12:16

do anything else selectively. It speaks

12:18

to that thing that you know

12:20

for a very long time in

12:22

human history we've seen ourselves as

12:24

like the pinnacle of evolution but

12:26

chimps are the best at being

12:28

chimps. Yeah absolutely and then not

12:30

a social and then the other

12:32

really important point is our cooperation

12:34

right that we're super cooperative. Yeah

12:36

we are and of course we've

12:39

also developed this complex spoken and

12:41

written language right? I mean that

12:43

has really helped share our knowledge

12:45

far and wide so beyond that

12:47

I think that maybe we ourselves

12:49

understand the benefit of sharing our

12:51

knowledge and skills towards some greater

12:53

and wider reaching goal. And the

12:55

goal of this podcast is to

12:57

make more people go to look

12:59

at new scientists. That's the pinnacle

13:01

here. Well what it reminds me

13:03

of is how the idea that

13:05

you get more innovation in in

13:07

cities in human culture because of

13:09

the greater mixing of ideas and

13:11

it kind of seems like in

13:13

chimp populations where you get this

13:15

multi-tool use building up they're like

13:17

prototypes of cities of like centers

13:19

of innovation where you get this

13:21

crossover of ideas. Yeah well they

13:23

certainly could be but you know

13:25

I think it's important to keep

13:27

in mind that like Andrew told

13:29

me cumulative culture remains very quote

13:31

tiny he said to compare it

13:33

to our own. So I don't

13:35

think we can expect them to

13:38

be, you know, creating an industrial

13:40

revolution in their communities, and they're

13:42

not going to be inventing computers

13:44

any time soon. This episode is

13:46

brought to you by, indeed. When

13:48

your computer breaks, you don't wait

13:50

for it to magically start working

13:52

again. You fix the problem, so

13:54

why wait to hire the people

13:56

your company desperately needs? Use Indeed

13:58

sponsored jobs to hire top talent

14:00

facts. And even better, you only

14:02

pay for results. There's

14:04

no need to wait. Speed

14:07

up your hiring with a

14:09

$75 sponsored job credit at

14:11

indeed.com slash

14:13

podcast. Terms and conditions

14:16

apply. Now are you

14:18

feeling hungry, Rhone? Now

14:20

you mention it. Once someone asks, right?

14:22

I could always eat. Great, well I've

14:24

got plenty for you to talk into

14:26

metaphorically. So we were going to talk

14:28

about two studies this week. One's looking

14:30

at calories and dietary choices and the other

14:32

is looking at why when you lose

14:34

weight it can often be really really

14:36

hard to keep that weight off and

14:39

instead you just put it all back on

14:41

again. food for thought. So first

14:43

off I wanted you know when

14:45

you see calories on a menu

14:47

next to a dish does that

14:50

influence your choice at all? If

14:52

I'm like ravenously hungry it might do

14:54

I might choose the one that's so

14:56

you're doing the opposite yeah I'm doing

14:58

what you're not supposed to do yeah

15:01

yeah I was thinking I I'm not

15:03

sure it really like I maybe would

15:05

factor in all that's a bit more

15:07

calories but I'm really not like looking

15:09

oh that's way worse than I expected

15:11

I wouldn't eat it then yeah so

15:13

I've been pretty skeptical about this idea

15:15

of whether putting calories or menus really

15:17

makes any difference at all and that

15:19

made me interested to see there is

15:22

a when Boris Johnson was Prime Minister

15:24

it was something that they introduced then.

15:26

Yeah it was part of this anti-abisty

15:28

campaign that was launched in 2020 there

15:30

were other things going on that year

15:32

but the idea is that large restaurants

15:35

or takeaway businesses have to print calories

15:37

alongside dishes so you know not small

15:39

independence but all the sort of mainstream

15:41

chains and it follows the same sort

15:43

of mandatory labelling that you already get

15:45

in the US. Okay and the whole idea

15:47

obviously is part of this nudge thing isn't

15:49

it's nudging people into what we want them

15:51

to do? Yeah making the right choices that's

15:53

the idea but there may well be other

15:55

effects people may think about like you said

15:57

if you're really hungry do you want to...

16:00

maximize how many calories you're going to

16:02

get, value for money, especially at the

16:04

moment food is really expensive. Yeah, I

16:06

mean, definitely as a student I would

16:08

choose the bulk. The bulk. The bulk

16:10

cheap purchases. Yeah, exactly. So what do

16:12

we know about whether it even works

16:14

or not? So a study just

16:16

out this week has modeled

16:18

the long -term consequences that we

16:20

might expect for this policy,

16:22

looking specifically at body mass

16:24

index and the risk of

16:26

cardiovascular problems in people in

16:28

England over a period of

16:30

20 years, and the results

16:32

as modeled predicted a decrease

16:34

in the prevalence of obesity

16:36

of just 0 .31 percentage

16:38

points and only 730 fewer

16:40

deaths from cardiovascular disease by

16:42

2041. Wow. So that's a lot of

16:45

effort for almost, for not much

16:47

that. Yeah, if you think about it,

16:49

the model predicts 830 ,000 cardiovascular deaths

16:51

during that time. So while 730 fewer

16:53

is not nothing, it's not a big

16:55

dent. The study did suggest that if

16:57

the policy was expanded to all restaurants

17:00

and cafes, like smaller ones, then the

17:02

model could prevent up to 9 ,000, 9

17:04

,200 deaths. So that's a bit more. Yeah.

17:06

Okay. I mean, it's

17:08

hard. It's really hard when you start

17:11

telling people what they can and can't

17:13

eat and what restaurants can and can't

17:15

serve, isn't it? I mean, people love

17:17

to eat certain things, so it

17:19

basically means we have to make available

17:21

the junk food that people like. Well,

17:23

right. Yeah. And some people depend on going

17:25

to fast food chains. It's their only option, given

17:27

the crazy shifts they're working on, the strange

17:29

places there. Yeah. But when I go out to

17:32

a restaurant for a treat, the

17:34

calories doesn't really come into

17:36

it if it's a one -off.

17:38

And also, calories aren't the

17:40

whole story. Some calories are

17:42

better than others. They might

17:44

be in far more nutritious

17:46

foods than a completely processed

17:48

fast food. Yeah. And also,

17:50

I think one of the

17:52

problems is that the idea

17:54

of putting the calories next

17:56

to items on the menu

17:58

and thinking that's going to

18:00

make a meaningful difference, it

18:02

sort of relies on the

18:04

idea that people eat unhealthily

18:06

due to a lack of

18:08

information or a lack of

18:10

education. And I'm not sure

18:12

that... the big problem here in terms of obesity.

18:14

But in any case, a survey of 3,000 people before

18:16

and after this UK legislation went through suggests that having

18:18

calories on menus made no significant change to people's eating

18:20

choices. Yeah. And another study found that a third of

18:22

the calories printed on the menus aren't actually that accurate.

18:24

So the guidelines require calories to lie within a 20%

18:26

margin of error, but one study showed that 35% of

18:29

the calories printed on sampled menus sat outside of

18:31

that margin of error, which sort

18:33

of makes sense if you think

18:35

about it. Food isn't this easily

18:37

standardized reproducible thing. Not the really good

18:40

stuff anyway. the fact that they don't

18:42

make much difference having their labels there.

18:44

Are there people who say they're actually

18:46

actively harmful having the, you know, these

18:48

labels here? Yeah, there's an argument that

18:50

sounds really, you know, it makes sense

18:53

to me that if you're someone who

18:55

struggles with disordered eating like anorexia or

18:57

bulimia. Yeah, if you're trying not to

18:59

let food kind of have that sort

19:01

of triggering a hold over you, having

19:03

the numbers right there next to them,

19:05

that's not not going to be very helpful

19:07

to be very helpful, is it? and when

19:09

the penny drops. You get rowing back on

19:11

that one. Thinking about, there's a lot to

19:13

weigh up when you're thinking about whether

19:16

it's worth doing calorie labelling or

19:18

not. Yeah, exactly. Okay, right. There's

19:20

another food related story that caught my

19:22

eye this week and that's, well, it's one

19:24

we've all probably got direct experience of or

19:26

we know someone who's experienced this and that's

19:28

the fact that some people can't seem to.

19:30

keep weight off when they lose it. You

19:33

know, they might go through so much hell

19:35

to get rid of a lot of weight

19:37

and then, you know, a few months later

19:39

it just piles back on. Yeah, this kind

19:41

of yo-go diet thing, it's an extremely

19:43

well-known phenomenon. And like, for example, around

19:45

85% of people with overweight or obesity

19:47

who managed to lose at least a

19:49

tenth of their body weight, 85% of

19:51

those will then regain it within a

19:53

year. So, you know, this is a

19:56

big problem for people who really are

19:58

trying to sort of sort of... of

20:00

lose that weight. Yeah, I mean it's super

20:02

hard to stick to to watch your calories.

20:04

Yeah, really carefully. And that you know these

20:06

horrible sort of really restricted weight loss

20:08

diet. Yeah. And it is miserable to

20:11

be hungry all the time. But that's

20:13

not the whole story either and we

20:15

know that. It's not just about sort

20:17

of willpower. Studies have also shown that

20:19

the brain interprets a sharp drop in

20:21

body fat as dangerous and responds by

20:23

making the body burn less energy and

20:26

that can potentially cause the body to

20:28

store up more fat again. more about

20:30

these kinds of processes that might be

20:32

happening. Researchers have now analyzed fat tissue

20:34

collected from 20 people with obesity just

20:36

before they had bariatric surgery which shrinks

20:38

the stomach to make people feel fuller

20:40

sooner and is quite an effective way

20:43

of losing a lot of weight. And

20:45

then they looked at these fat cells

20:47

again two years later when they had

20:49

lost at least a quarter of their

20:51

initial body weight. And then for comparison

20:53

they also looked at fat tissue from

20:55

18 people with a healthy weight. And

20:58

so when they compared those two... groups

21:00

before the surgery they found that the

21:02

activity of more than a hundred genes

21:04

differed between them between their fat cells

21:06

and that these differences remained when

21:08

they looked again two years later

21:11

after the bariatric surgery. Wow so

21:13

two years later the bodies of

21:15

previously obese people was still behaving

21:18

differently. Yeah, essentially it's quite astonishing,

21:20

isn't it? Yeah, yeah. And it looks

21:22

like these differences might be increased with

21:24

things like increased inflammation, disruption to how

21:27

fat cells store and burn fat, and

21:29

so you could see how those potentially

21:31

maybe raise the risk of gaining weight

21:33

again, although this experiment didn't sort of

21:35

go and show and show and prove

21:38

that. Okay, and so obviously the big

21:40

question is, can you change... gene activity?

21:42

Can you wrote about that in that

21:44

group? Well, the answer currently, you know,

21:46

if you want to actually kind of

21:48

target those specific genes, is no. Further

21:50

experiments in mice suggest that the changes

21:53

in gene activity that were seen in

21:55

this experiment are probably due to epigenetics.

21:57

So that's, you know, changes on the

21:59

outside. side of the DNA that affects how

22:01

active genes are, and we don't really

22:03

have a way to sort of precision tweak

22:06

that yet. But I don't think that

22:08

means the situation is necessarily hopeless. Our epigenome,

22:10

it changes in response to experience and

22:12

environment. So even though, you know, it's still

22:14

behaving the same way two years later,

22:16

that's not to say it wouldn't change at

22:18

some point. And potentially future studies could

22:20

have a look at what happens over a

22:22

longer period or if there are sort

22:24

of environmental ways that we could sort of

22:26

shift that gene activity the way that

22:29

you want it to go. Yeah. All

22:31

right, since we're talking about obesity

22:33

and weight loss, like the big

22:35

drug at the moment is ozempic.

22:37

The other one is wegovi. Those

22:39

are these glucogen like peptide one.

22:42

Analogs, GLP one, very big names

22:44

of drugs at the moment. How

22:46

do they fit into all this? Yeah,

22:48

I mean, people are saying significant weight loss.

22:50

There's a lot of excitement about it. It's

22:52

still sort of relatively early days. We don't

22:54

know long term, you know, are people going

22:56

to have to take these forever. But I

22:58

think when you sort of look at this

23:01

picture as a whole, what really stands out

23:03

to me is that the tools that we

23:05

currently have that we know are really good,

23:07

a large amount of weight loss, bariatric surgery.

23:09

And now these GLP one analogs, these approaches

23:11

probably work for a variety of reasons. But

23:13

one thing that really stands out to me

23:15

is that they all reduce appetite. And we

23:17

live in these very obesogenic environments now with

23:19

pretty sedentary. We're short on time and

23:21

money and healthy foods are often the cheapest

23:23

and most available. So if you think

23:25

about, you know, telling someone how many calories

23:27

are in something, it's hard to see

23:30

how that's going to have a bigger effect.

23:32

If somebody's body is working in a

23:34

way that it's just driving them to feel

23:36

really hungry and put that weight on. Now,

23:44

for this next story, I'm going

23:46

to break basically break a rule

23:48

of talking about stories in New

23:50

Scientist, which is we're never normally

23:52

allowed to invoke science fiction. But

23:54

this one, I just can't help

23:56

it because, well, you'll see why.

23:58

So this is computer scientists at

24:00

Stanford and they've got this. A

24:02

version of chat GPT to simulate

24:04

a thousand real people and basically

24:06

create digital twins of them. And

24:08

then when you test the AI

24:10

version later, the twin, this digital

24:12

twin manages to just replicate you,

24:14

your personality and really quite accurately.

24:16

Jacob Ehren's here to tell us

24:18

about it. Jacob, look, he's on

24:20

the screen here, it could not

24:22

him. It could not be him.

24:24

Well, take your word for it

24:26

that it's you, but ... I

24:28

promise you, I'm not a deep

24:30

fake. That's what they'd say. Tell

24:32

us about this story, then. So

24:34

the idea is that they've used

24:36

GPT for O, which is the

24:39

kind of most advanced version of

24:41

chat GPT, to interview people for

24:43

two hours, and this is a

24:45

voice interview, so people are essentially

24:47

just sort of naturally speaking and

24:49

having the AI talk back to

24:51

them. And using the transcript that

24:53

they get from that interview, they

24:55

then feed it back into the

24:57

AI and are able to generate

24:59

a replica of that person. Okay,

25:01

so often when you're looking at

25:03

personality in people, you use a

25:05

really standard questionnaire, but what they've

25:07

done here is rather than using

25:09

a generic form, they've actually had

25:11

a chat with the GPT model

25:13

to build up a view of

25:15

their personality. Yeah, so the AI

25:17

was sort of given a list

25:19

of standard questions to ask and

25:21

work through, but it could deviate

25:23

from them, and if the interviewee

25:25

said something in particular, the AI

25:27

was then able to respond to

25:29

it in the way that you

25:31

wouldn't normally get if you're just

25:33

filling out a form. And after

25:36

doing this for two hours, they

25:38

then ran the AI's through a

25:40

variety of tests. And when they

25:42

gave the same test to the

25:44

original humans, they found that the

25:46

AI's were able to replicate their

25:48

answers with 85 % accuracy. If you've

25:50

done this and you've set up

25:52

the AI model, Jake, right, supposing

25:54

they did one of me and

25:56

then they asked my replica, what

25:58

does Roe... and think about, what

26:00

do you think about, say,

26:02

laws against assisted dying, which is a big thing

26:04

at the moment, lots of people, you know,

26:06

lots of things in the news about it? Would

26:09

it give a similar response to the real

26:11

me? You know, is that what they're able to

26:13

do yet? So I think

26:15

it's hard to say whether it

26:17

would be completely accurate, you know,

26:19

if the AI Rowan would say

26:21

exactly what you would say. But

26:23

for example, part of the interview

26:25

involved asking people's views on healthcare,

26:28

you know, their sort of life

26:30

experience which will include people dying,

26:32

their views on vaccines, alternative medicine.

26:34

And you could imagine how with

26:36

all of that information, you could

26:38

infer what someone's views towards assisted

26:40

dying might be, just based on

26:43

sort of, you could cluster people together,

26:45

you know, if you have particular views on

26:47

healthcare, you also perhaps going to have

26:49

particular views on assisted dying. Yeah. Is

26:52

this a terrifying thing? Why

26:54

are they doing this? So the

26:56

goal of the research

26:58

is to replace existing models

27:00

that are used for forecasting policy

27:02

changes. So at the moment, say

27:04

you want to come up with

27:06

a new law or a tax

27:08

change, and you want to know

27:11

how that's going to affect the

27:13

general population, you might look at

27:15

a demographic model say, so you

27:17

could say, well, for people who

27:19

live in cities versus people who

27:21

live in the country, people of

27:23

certain ages and backgrounds, this is

27:25

how they might respond. And you

27:28

can sort of feed that into

27:30

the model and see what what

27:32

their reaction to your policy is

27:34

going to be. Obviously, all of

27:36

that involves quite a lot of

27:38

averaging and guesswork. The hope is

27:40

with something like this by more

27:42

closely replicating individuals. And I should

27:45

say that the 1000 or so

27:47

people they interviewed were representative of

27:49

the US population by interviewing these

27:51

people and being able to replicate

27:53

their answers in more detail, you

27:55

could then be able to get

27:57

sort of more subtle responses to.

28:00

policy changes? Should we worry about

28:02

the ethics of this? Like potentially

28:04

the ability to sort of replicate

28:06

human personalities or their emotions? That

28:08

could be a goldmine for all

28:10

sorts of reasons. I guess marketing

28:12

is the first one that brings

28:14

to mind. So yeah I imagine

28:16

marketers would absolutely love to have

28:19

this. The researchers behind the work

28:21

said they've been really really aware

28:23

about ethics throughout the whole experiment.

28:25

They've been sort of in constant

28:27

communication with their ethical oversight board.

28:29

They put in various controls so

28:31

that if someone who took part

28:33

in this interview now says, oh,

28:35

actually I don't want to be

28:37

involved anymore, they can retract their

28:40

consent, they can delete the model.

28:42

But I think it is a

28:44

bit of a genie out the

28:46

bottle moment, as you say, now

28:48

that someone's shown that this is

28:50

possible, it basically involves just using

28:52

chatGPT and tools that are readily

28:54

available. And if you were willing

28:56

to pay the people to do

28:58

their interviews and things like that,

29:00

I don't see why you couldn't

29:03

replicate this. Obviously, there are sort

29:05

of data protection laws and things

29:07

like that, but those vary from

29:09

country to country. I mean I

29:11

can see this becoming quite normal

29:13

quite quickly and then we'll get

29:15

used to that maybe we'll get

29:17

used to these things around and

29:19

also you know we project a

29:21

lot onto even you know very

29:24

obviously non-human robots and you know

29:26

vacuum cleaners and assign them agency

29:28

you know can imagine all the

29:30

more so in something that's very

29:32

much upper level you know we're

29:34

going to start assuming these are

29:36

real or conscious right? So I

29:38

don't know whether it actually appears

29:40

more conscious than chat-GPT already does.

29:42

It's more specific, I suppose, more

29:45

tailored to an individual, but it's

29:47

going to be just as fluent

29:49

as chat-GPT already is. And actually

29:51

we had a study earlier this

29:53

year showing that 20% of people

29:55

in the US actually already think

29:57

that chat-GPT is sent. I would

29:59

say most people would, or most

30:01

researchers would say that it is

30:03

very much not, but you can

30:05

see how in the public perception

30:08

it does appear to be this

30:10

sort of conscious thing that you

30:12

can interact with. Well, I mean,

30:14

and also, as these get more

30:16

complex, I could see you can

30:18

start to make an argument that

30:20

you might as well call them

30:23

conscious. I mean, your mind very quickly

30:25

goes to that black mirror scenario,

30:27

right, of you, you don't even

30:29

realize you've been replicated and you're

30:31

stuck on a server somewhere. When

30:33

Chris Dokle Walker, the reporter who

30:35

wrote this story for us, kind

30:37

of asked the researchers about this,

30:39

it was not a question they

30:41

wanted to engage with much as

30:43

you can imagine. I mean, they're

30:45

very much more focused on the

30:47

data privacy angle of things, which

30:49

I think is important. You know,

30:51

normally if you take a... part

30:53

in research or data might be

30:55

sort of anonymized, averaged in some

30:57

way. With this, the goal very

31:00

explicitly is to try and replicate

31:02

an individual. And so there are

31:04

data privacy questions and things like

31:07

that. When it comes to saying,

31:09

are these conscious? I think it's

31:11

really difficult, right? Like I said,

31:14

I don't think it's any more

31:16

conscious than chat GPT is normally,

31:18

and I would say chat GPT

31:21

is currently not conscious. Yeah, can I

31:23

just pour a bunch of cold water

31:25

in this for a moment? First off,

31:27

I don't know how it comes when

31:30

you're doing marketing or political modeling, but

31:32

from, say, a biomedical... perspective, 85% accurate,

31:34

is not accurate. You know, that's not

31:36

sort of, that's not particularly useful. And

31:38

also, the thing that I thought was

31:41

really stark about this is that just

31:43

two weeks later, real people when they

31:45

were answering the same questions again answered

31:47

them differently, 19% of the time. So

31:50

people are really variable and we know

31:52

about that from psychology, we change our

31:54

minds from one moment to the next.

31:56

And so doesn't that actually suggest that

31:59

AI models aren't? really useful in

32:01

this way Jacob. Well that's

32:03

really interesting right because in

32:05

in one way and it's

32:07

sort of described in this

32:09

way in the researchers paper

32:11

people are bad models of

32:14

themselves so people were 80%

32:16

accurate at repeating what they'd

32:18

said two weeks ago and

32:20

so then the AI was

32:22

85% accurate sort of repeating

32:24

their answers over those two

32:26

weeks. I think what's interesting

32:28

is they did a comparison

32:31

to those more sort of

32:33

demographic models I was talking

32:35

about earlier and they found

32:37

that the accuracy did increase

32:39

by 14 percentage points. So

32:41

there was a benefit to

32:44

doing this AI version rather

32:46

than the more simplistic demographic

32:48

modeling. You could record a version of

32:50

yourself when you're young and idealistic

32:52

and save that nice young, you

32:54

know, idealistic version before you become

32:56

old and jaded. I'm not speaking

32:58

from personal experience here. I like

33:00

the way you're saying you. One,

33:02

one could. And also, you know,

33:04

I don't want to get too

33:06

black-mirish, but you know, you could

33:08

also see how... you could chat

33:10

to someone after they're dead very

33:12

very easily right if this gets

33:14

a bit better and if you

33:16

spoke to one of these models

33:18

for more than two hours like extensively

33:21

then you could perhaps get a really

33:23

good model of them that you know

33:25

it might be a world leader you know not

33:27

just a loved one that you might want

33:29

to talk to so there's a lot

33:31

of mad possibilities aren't there? Well

33:33

there's a few things to unpack there

33:35

so first of all would talking for

33:38

longer than two hours actually provide more

33:40

gains in a way this is one

33:42

of the biggest questions about AI at

33:44

the moment can you continue scaling the

33:46

more data you feed into an AI

33:48

does it get more and more accurate

33:50

or do we hit a ceiling at

33:52

some point so that they've not done

33:55

the experiment yet and it might be

33:57

that you know the gains after two

33:59

hours on much more. The other thing

34:01

to keep in mind and coming

34:03

back to this idea that people are

34:05

not necessarily good models of themselves,

34:07

is that we actually do a lot

34:09

of work filling in the blanks.

34:11

So take mediums and psychics. They can

34:13

really convince people that they know

34:15

their deep dark secrets when actually they've

34:17

just got a few basic facts

34:19

and then your mind fills in the

34:21

rest. And you go, oh yeah,

34:23

my dead uncle would have said that.

34:25

So I don't see why we

34:28

wouldn't do the same thing with with

34:30

AI. And in a way,

34:32

obviously, these are really impressive

34:34

technological tools, but a lot

34:36

of the magic sort of

34:38

goes on in our own

34:40

brains as much as they're

34:42

going on in the software.

34:44

Not quite sure how that

34:46

reassures me or not. But

34:48

that's it for this week. Thanks to our guests.

34:50

Thanks to you for listening. Do tell all

34:52

your friends about us. Give us a five star

34:55

rating on Spotify, Apple Podcasts everywhere. We'll

34:57

be back next week. Bye, bye for now. Bye.

35:05

This podcast is produced by

35:07

OG Podcasts. Find out

35:09

more at ogpodcasts .co .uk

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features