Is Time Real? How Your Brain Perceives the Past with Dr. Lila Davachi

Is Time Real? How Your Brain Perceives the Past with Dr. Lila Davachi

Released Thursday, 6th March 2025
Good episode? Give it some love!
Is Time Real? How Your Brain Perceives the Past with Dr. Lila Davachi

Is Time Real? How Your Brain Perceives the Past with Dr. Lila Davachi

Is Time Real? How Your Brain Perceives the Past with Dr. Lila Davachi

Is Time Real? How Your Brain Perceives the Past with Dr. Lila Davachi

Thursday, 6th March 2025
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

time itself doesn't really exist

0:02

in memory. And I would

0:04

argue that time doesn't really

0:06

exist after a few seconds,

0:09

that it's really about where

0:11

we're putting our attention, our

0:13

internal mental context, how we're

0:16

basically orchestrating our mind, what

0:18

we're orchestrating our minds to

0:20

think about. Hi, I'm Elizabeth Coke.

0:22

We all live inside our

0:25

own personal private perception box.

0:27

built by our genes and

0:29

the physical, social, and cultural

0:31

environment in which we were

0:33

born and raised. In this

0:35

podcast, we explore how although

0:38

the walls of this mental

0:40

box are always present, they

0:42

can expand in states like

0:44

awe, wonder, and curiosity,

0:47

or contract in response

0:49

to anxiety, fear, and anger.

0:51

I'd like to introduce our

0:54

esteemed hosts. to incredible and

0:56

distinguished minds. Dr. Heather Berlin,

0:58

professor of psychiatry and neuroscience

1:01

at the Icon School of

1:03

Medicine at Mount Sinai in New

1:05

York City. And Dr. Kristof Koch,

1:07

chief scientist for the Tiny Blue

1:10

Dot Foundation, and the current

1:12

meritorious investigator and former president

1:15

of the Allen Institute for

1:17

Brain Science. Welcome to the

1:19

Science of Perception Box. Hi

1:25

everybody, welcome to Science and Perception

1:27

Box. I'm your co-host, Dr. Heather

1:29

Berlin. And I'm your co-host, Dr. Chris

1:32

of Koch. Every week we feature

1:34

an aspect of the Science of

1:36

Perception Box, highlighting the latest research

1:38

together with our expert guests. This

1:40

week, we're exploring the relationship

1:42

between memory, time, and perception.

1:44

I'm thrilled to welcome Dr.

1:47

Lela Devachi, a leading neuroscientist

1:49

at Columbia University, whose groundbreaking

1:51

work sheds light on the

1:53

mysteries of the brain and

1:55

its relationship with time. Dr.

1:57

Devachi investigates how we encode

1:59

and retrieve memories. Her work reveals

2:01

that our perception of time is more

2:04

fluid than we might think. In her

2:06

research, she uncovers how the richness of

2:08

our experiences, how varied and eventful they

2:11

are, can stretch or compress our sense

2:13

of time. Through fascinating MRI experiments, she

2:15

shows that the brain's hippocampus plays a

2:18

pivotal role in memory creation, while our

2:20

internal experience can transform the way we

2:22

perceive the external world. First, we want

2:25

to share our own connection to memory.

2:27

So, Krista, what do you see is

2:29

the relationship between perception and memory? Well,

2:32

what do you expect from a guy

2:34

who wears all yellow? Is this how

2:36

you want to be remembered? I maintain

2:39

my sunny disposition because I think partially

2:41

because what I choose to remember and

2:43

how whether those are positive or negative

2:46

effects. So I think there's a strong

2:48

relationship between the way you perceive the

2:50

world and what you focus on and

2:53

then what you remember. And that makes

2:55

up your experienced reality or perception walks.

2:57

Well, there's two different issues there. I

3:00

think one is, do you think that

3:02

very strong pronounced stimuli will help you

3:04

remember something better or is it that

3:07

you just want the memory to be

3:09

associated with a very positive emotion? the

3:11

latter primarily. I choose to, I think,

3:14

constantly choose to primarily remember good things

3:16

about the world. You choose to remember

3:18

good things about the world. That's the

3:21

way how I do it. And I

3:23

think we can all do that. We

3:25

can choose to remember troubled things or

3:28

insults or slides, and we can just

3:30

choose to remember the positive things that

3:32

we encounter. So basically, if we both

3:35

encounter the same scene or other people

3:37

encounter the same scene, what people remember

3:39

from that scene can vary based on

3:42

what their... sort of perception is or

3:44

what they focus on. Yes. What they

3:46

focus on it. I mean, so a

3:49

person who's depressed might just pick up

3:51

on all the sort of negative aspects

3:53

that's more salient to them versus somebody

3:56

who has a happy disposition might remember.

3:58

And if you do this consistently as

4:00

a... I think it builds up your

4:03

view of reality, your reality, because it's

4:05

the only one you have. Well, I

4:07

mean, we have an expert here

4:09

who can help us with answering

4:12

some of these questions. So, Leila,

4:14

welcome to the podcast. Thank you.

4:16

It's great to be here. Well,

4:19

it turns out that... emotions

4:21

do influence memory and negative

4:23

and positive emotions do so

4:25

in different ways, which is

4:27

relevant to what you were

4:29

just saying. Really negative experiences

4:31

produce a very vivid memory,

4:33

but that memory is fragmented

4:36

and it doesn't include the

4:38

context, what happened before, what

4:40

happened after, but you can

4:42

have a really sort of

4:44

invasive, almost ruminative, negative memory,

4:46

but positive experiences on the

4:48

other hand tend to produce

4:50

more flexible integrated memory so that

4:53

there's almost a bigger memory. There's

4:55

more to remember and it's more

4:57

flexible and research has shown that

5:00

when you're in an experience and

5:02

you're positively motivated, let's say searching

5:04

for reward, that you do activate

5:07

the hippocampus more and that creates

5:09

a more flexible memory representation. But

5:11

if you're in that same environment,

5:14

but you're avoiding a threat, you're

5:16

trying to escape, you don't activate

5:18

the hippocampus. In fact, you activate

5:21

cortical structures, and later on you

5:23

do remember that event, but you

5:25

don't remember much about it. It's

5:27

fragmented. So the central parts... of

5:29

a negative experience are remembered probably

5:31

too well, but all of the

5:33

important and interesting other parts of

5:35

that memory are not there for

5:37

negative experiences. That's really interesting. So

5:39

it's the first time I've heard

5:41

of this that... When it's a highly

5:44

negative experience, it doesn't activate the

5:46

hippocampus. That's been shown in human

5:48

fMRI and in some rodent work,

5:51

yes. But a positive experience would

5:53

activate the hippocampus. So patient who

5:55

doesn't have a hippocampus anymore because

5:58

it's been surgical or more... that

6:00

they would not remember truly negative

6:02

aversive event. Well, no, they remember

6:04

it from, it, she's, you know, it

6:07

activates the cortex, so there's, there's memory

6:09

areas in the cortex as well. Right,

6:11

so patient, this is really interesting, patients

6:13

with complete hippocampal damage who were amnesic

6:16

still retain some aversive memories that they

6:18

show in their behavior. So one of

6:20

my favorite examples of this had to

6:23

do with, patient HM who's very famous,

6:25

who had no memory. If I walked,

6:27

if I was him and I walked

6:30

out the door, I would never even

6:32

remember meeting you. But if you had

6:34

shook my hand and you had a

6:36

little sharp pin on the inside of

6:38

your palm, I would recoil. And if

6:40

I left the room, I would come

6:42

back and not remember you, even with

6:44

your yellow outfit, but I might not

6:46

want to shake your hand anymore. That

6:48

was an experiment that was shown. So

6:50

they avoid engaging with a person who

6:53

shocked them. So that tells you that

6:55

there's something that remained about that negative

6:57

experience. But it's unconscious. Without a hippocampus

6:59

campus, yeah. It appears unconscious, because they

7:01

can't really tell you much about the

7:03

past. But it can't be that negative

7:05

experiences. Sometimes they're sort of... hyper remembered,

7:07

you know, like when people

7:09

have PTSD and they can't

7:11

stop thinking about these negative

7:13

experiences. Yeah, no, no, no,

7:15

completely. So the central core. of

7:17

the experience in that moment when

7:19

something really negative is happening, your

7:21

pupil dilates, you're taking actually time

7:23

slows down, you're taking in a

7:25

lot more information in that moment,

7:27

at least that's the hypothesis, and

7:29

that's central information as well remembered,

7:31

but nothing in the surround, the

7:33

contextual stuff, actually contrary to what

7:35

people believe, they will claim. after

7:37

a negative experience that their memory

7:39

is really, really good. And we

7:41

call this flashball memories. But actually,

7:43

when tested, their memory for what

7:45

happened that day and the general context

7:48

is no better than remembering what you had

7:50

for lunch. It's not really special. But the

7:52

central core information is really well remembered. Oh,

7:54

that's interesting. And I do want to get

7:56

into in a little bit. I want to

7:59

get into time. perception during these experiences, because that's

8:01

something I'm actually really interested in, and had done

8:03

some research on time perception as well. So I want

8:05

to dig in there. But first, I just want

8:07

to ask a general question of just, what

8:09

do you think the purpose of memory is? I'm

8:12

so glad that you're asking that

8:15

question, because 10 years ago, no

8:17

one was asking that question. People

8:19

were asking, how does memory work?

8:21

With the assumption that memory is

8:23

there to remember, and

8:26

I don't think that's what memory

8:28

is for. I think now after

8:30

dedicating 25 years of my life

8:32

to studying memory, that memory is

8:34

there to help us understand the

8:36

world, to build knowledge. So in

8:38

other words, our memory

8:41

systems are built to care

8:43

about what's prioritized, what's

8:45

important to learn from every

8:47

day. And that's why

8:49

probably the thing you can rely

8:51

on the most about your memory is

8:53

forgetting. I think once you go

8:55

to sleep, the memory system just doesn't

8:57

consolidate and sort of literally trashes

8:59

most of the day, because it's not

9:01

relevant to your survival. In other

9:03

words, you've already learned so much about

9:05

the world. So what tends to

9:07

get consolidated during sleep, and this is

9:09

when the memory system really gears

9:12

up in action, is what is it

9:14

going to keep around? What kinds

9:16

of memories are going to undergo synaptic

9:18

plasticity? And that's when you're increasing

9:20

the communication between neurons so that you can

9:22

remember that information later. is doing

9:24

specifically during slow -wave sleep. This happens during

9:26

slow -wave sleep, and we actually have evidence that that

9:28

might be tagged during awake

9:30

resting states as well. That's what

9:32

we've been looking at recently. So

9:34

there's an interplay, I think, between

9:37

the awake brain at rest and

9:39

sleep. But so your memory system

9:41

is trying to figure out, did

9:43

you learn anything new today that

9:45

is important to keep around? And

9:48

it updates the cortex. It

9:50

tells, so during sleep, it's

9:52

teaching cortical neurons how to

9:54

represent any kind of new

9:57

important information. So really memory

9:59

isn't for remembering we think it's

10:01

really for gathering knowledge and updating

10:03

our knowledge of the world. So

10:05

would it make sense though and

10:07

I think this leads into the

10:09

time perception but the older you

10:11

get and the more buildup you've

10:13

had of knowledge so that maybe

10:16

we the less you needed the

10:18

less you need to remember in

10:20

a way right because I know

10:22

you know my children they're like

10:24

seven and ten now they remember

10:26

exquisite details of things that I

10:28

just They soak up everything. Everything,

10:30

and they remember everything. And, you

10:32

know, I forget most things. But

10:34

maybe that's adaptive. Maybe it's just

10:36

because my brain, it doesn't need

10:39

that information as much anymore. Absolutely.

10:41

I think that instead of continuing

10:43

to talk about aging as a

10:45

deficit model, like memory in the

10:47

lab, like little details aren't as

10:49

well remembered, it's important socially to

10:51

remember people's names, but people are.

10:53

generally bad at remembering people's names

10:55

no matter how old you are.

10:57

But it's true this could explain

10:59

this could explain what's typically thought

11:02

of as a deficit that as

11:04

you age your memories get worse

11:06

but what you are better at

11:08

are knowing how the world works

11:10

and you can call that wisdom

11:12

you can call that making predictions

11:14

and there is research now showing

11:16

that when Older adults are facing

11:18

the same memory question, or when

11:20

they're encoding what's thought of as

11:22

a novel item context experience in

11:25

the lab, they're activating more of

11:27

their cortex than their hippocampus. And

11:29

younger adults or children are activating

11:31

more of their hippocampus, and that's

11:33

another way of saying they're gathering

11:35

those details to be replayed during

11:37

sleep. But older people, maybe, you

11:39

know, if your brain were talking,

11:41

might be saying, oh yeah, I

11:43

know what's going to happen. No

11:46

need to. How much is this

11:48

process on a voluntary control? So

11:50

to what extent can I choose

11:52

to remember, for instance, negative events,

11:54

you know, some perceived insult that

11:56

I may not want to recall

11:58

and primarily focus on? positive memories

12:00

or is an illusion. Each of

12:02

us have our own natural

12:04

orientation but that doesn't mean

12:07

it can't be changed. Let's say

12:09

like Christoph you're up here and

12:11

you think of you see the

12:13

world through this like rosy glasses

12:15

and you're picking in that moment

12:17

if you're able to attend to

12:20

the positive aspects or reappraise in

12:22

the moment if someone is being

12:24

rude to you in New York City,

12:26

but you're thinking, oh, you probably had

12:28

a bad day, it's not about me.

12:30

Like that kind of thinking may now,

12:32

after many years, be where you are.

12:34

And I may be down here. I

12:37

may be really sensitive to perceived slights

12:39

in my environment, and that means when

12:41

I leave an experience, those memories are

12:43

going to pop up into my mind,

12:45

and then I can choose to ruminate

12:47

on them or not. I do believe

12:49

that everyone can shift a little bit.

12:51

Maybe, if I work on it. But

12:54

I don't think we're stuck, but I

12:56

do think that there are going to

12:58

be individual differences. And knowing where you

13:00

are is important in figuring out how

13:03

to move that. But I also believe

13:05

that it's much easier. If you can

13:07

change the memory before it's become a

13:09

memory, in the moment, if your interpretation

13:11

is there and the rosiness is already

13:14

there, then that memory is more likely

13:16

to be positive. It's much harder to

13:18

change a memory once it's become consolidated.

13:20

Once it's there is a memory, then

13:23

you're more suppressing it. And that is

13:25

only minimally effective. Suppression only

13:27

works in the moment, but it

13:29

doesn't seem to work in the

13:31

long term. I'm thinking of it

13:33

in terms of therapy, right? Therapy

13:35

can be used, like I say,

13:37

cognitive behavioral therapy, cognitive reframing, to

13:39

move you up a bit, you know, move the

13:42

needle up, but there is a limit in

13:44

terms of, say, our genetic predisposition or

13:46

the way our brains are wired. So

13:48

there's certain, our boundaries might be set,

13:50

but within a range, and that therapy

13:52

can get you a little higher, you

13:54

know, so we do have the ability

13:57

to change over time, we're not sort

13:59

of stuck in that. remember something, you

14:01

can rewrite it. And that part

14:03

of maybe also what therapy can

14:05

do is help you rewrite it

14:07

in a more positive way, in

14:09

a more beneficial way. Yeah, that's

14:12

the big hope. Definitely, every time

14:14

you reactivate a memory, it becomes

14:16

labile and open to change. But

14:18

so far how much you can

14:20

change that it's really hard to

14:22

make a memory more positive Not

14:24

that it's impossible, but you can

14:26

make it slightly more neutral Well,

14:28

so you're telling us I'm the

14:30

distinct memory of my first kids

14:32

when I was 15 and are

14:34

you telling me that every time

14:37

I recall that? I re-encoded. That

14:39

is what we think. So when

14:41

you activate a memory in the

14:43

brain, when the neural ensemble or

14:45

the n-gram is activated, it enters

14:47

into a state of being moderately

14:49

activated. And then that process can

14:51

lead to either elements dropping out

14:53

or elements coming in. Like if

14:55

you're thinking about your 15-year-old self

14:57

and your first kiss, and then

15:00

you quickly moved the kiss you

15:02

had last Saturday. in your mind

15:04

and they're co-activated. Not that you

15:06

would necessarily confuse them, that would

15:08

be the extreme, you might confuse

15:10

them, but they can become integrated

15:12

in the brain. But that really

15:14

means, A, the memory is radical,

15:16

different from a computer memory, right?

15:18

Absolutely. And it also means that

15:20

my memory is then amenable to

15:22

being reinterpreted by myself. Yeah. Or

15:25

what I tell myself about who

15:27

I was. Exactly. And that's why

15:29

therapy should work. Yeah. So in

15:31

the moment activating the memory and

15:33

then rethinking it, maybe perspective taking,

15:35

even just being calm, if your

15:37

body is not aroused in a

15:39

negative way when you're recalling a

15:41

negative memory, that can help to

15:43

disentangle the, I call it the,

15:45

the body memory, the fear memory,

15:48

the heart rate increase, you know,

15:50

the sweatiness. If you can feel

15:52

calm, that's why Propranolol all works,

15:54

because if you have a beta

15:56

blocker. which is

15:58

a drug that's

16:00

often used to, I'm

16:03

not a medical doctor. A beta blockers

16:05

are used for like stage fright. It's

16:07

often given to patients with cardiac issues.

16:09

It kind of calms down the nervous

16:12

system. So you're, you know. Keeps your

16:14

heart rate steady. When you're

16:16

re -remembering your memory while you take

16:18

that. While you're taking a beta blocker, what

16:20

you're doing is, if you're

16:22

retrieving a negative memory that would normally

16:24

cause a lot of heart rate increase

16:26

and now your heart's not increasing, the

16:28

brain has an opportunity to disentangle or

16:30

separate those two events. That's like the

16:33

hope for a lot of the use

16:35

of propanolol. It hasn't been super successful

16:37

yet, I will say, but there's still

16:39

hope in trying to use. For phobias,

16:41

if you give people propanololol and then

16:43

introduce them to, let's say, a spider

16:45

or something, you can actually unlearn a

16:47

fear. In that even one

16:49

trial, there's some studies that were done in

16:51

the Netherlands on that. the

16:56

brain has an disentangle But if you

16:58

mentioned a small plane, I might jump

17:00

away. But there is this idea with

17:02

therapy as well. You bring up a

17:04

sort of very negative emotion or memory

17:06

in a neutral context with a safe

17:08

person in a safe space that you

17:10

can sort of re -encode it in

17:12

a way that dissociates the negative feeling

17:14

from the memory itself. Why

17:16

does this sort of not work for PTSD? Well,

17:20

it could just be that memories are

17:22

so strong. I have my own

17:24

experience with using propranolol to get

17:26

rid of a fear of

17:28

flying. So when I was

17:30

on the job market for my very

17:33

first job and I was lucky enough

17:35

to have multiple interviews, I

17:37

would get on a

17:39

plane to go to the

17:41

interview. And my interpretation

17:43

is that I was preparing

17:45

and anxious for the

17:47

interview, preparing my job talk.

17:49

What was I gonna

17:51

say to the professors I

17:53

was meeting? And so

17:56

my physiology was up -regulated

17:58

on the plane. And then

18:00

there I was on

18:02

a plane that was like

18:04

shaking. Maybe there was

18:06

turbulence. So I think that

18:08

my brain is so... associated

18:10

the plane context and the turbulence with my internal

18:12

anxiety. So after the job market was done and

18:14

I landed my job, yay, I found out that

18:16

before all this I never was afraid on planes.

18:18

And now even getting on a plane to go

18:20

to the beach, I was anxious. All of the

18:22

turbulence bothered me. I became really afraid of flying.

18:24

And I first tried like, you know. alcohol consumption.

18:26

It didn't really work for me. I was still

18:28

afraid. Alcohol consumption. It didn't work in the

18:31

longs. And then I tried other medications

18:33

that would knock me out and make

18:35

me feel tired but didn't get rid

18:37

of my... my anxiety and fear of

18:39

flying. So then I thought, I need

18:42

to use science. And so my dad,

18:44

who was a cardiologist, gave me prescription

18:46

for a propranolololol, and I took beta

18:48

blockers on one, two, three, or maybe

18:51

six flights every time. And frankly, you

18:53

know, I was a little headache. I

18:55

didn't notice anything in the moment. I

18:57

still had that fear, but after... six

19:00

or seven times taking it, I now

19:02

am much more calm on flights. I

19:04

feel like I've unlearned that, my body

19:06

has unlearned that association. So it is

19:08

possible. I don't know, has it learned

19:10

this new association, this new positive

19:12

association, flying doesn't have to be

19:14

associated with the racing heart. That is

19:17

a great question and I don't, I think,

19:19

you know, I would like to say the

19:21

answer is usually in the middle. It's probably

19:23

a little bit of both. So

19:25

I did want to bring

19:27

in time, because this is

19:30

something that fascinates me. So,

19:32

you know, what is the

19:34

relationship between time and memory

19:36

and experience? And, you know,

19:39

why is it that sort

19:41

of time seems to expand?

19:43

I actually just, I recently

19:45

testified as an expert witness

19:48

in a court case, and it was

19:50

about this woman, she died.

19:52

unfortunately, in a

19:54

motorcycle accident. But she was

19:56

conscious and aware during that

19:58

time and how even though was just

20:00

a few seconds, when you're

20:02

in that moment of sheer

20:05

terror and you, you know,

20:07

impending death, that it can feel

20:09

way longer in those moments,

20:11

right? So subjectively,

20:13

you know, how do you

20:16

explain that? What's a relationship

20:18

between memory and experience

20:21

and time? So what I know about

20:23

that is that... And we've all maybe

20:25

had an experience where there's something

20:27

really arousing that demands our attention

20:30

in the moment that it can

20:32

feel like the world is slowing

20:34

down a little. So those experiences

20:36

are happening at a slightly slower

20:38

rate so that you can take

20:40

in more information. Bullet time in

20:42

the Matrix movie. Oh, bullet time.

20:45

When they shootness, bullet goes

20:47

incredible slowly towards a meal.

20:49

You're aware that that's fiction, but we'll

20:51

go on. Oh. Oh. So to make sure...

20:53

But it's a cinematic depiction

20:55

of the experience that you

20:57

mentioned. Yes, yes. So there

20:59

is a lot of data

21:01

suggesting that subjective measurements of

21:04

time, your experience of time

21:06

slows down in those really

21:08

highly arousing moments. What we

21:10

study in the lab is

21:12

understanding whether that how that

21:14

translates into memories and how

21:16

we remember time. And so...

21:18

That is what most of

21:20

our research focuses on is

21:22

memory for time. And in

21:24

that case, what we're seeing

21:26

is that time itself doesn't really

21:28

exist in memory. And I would

21:31

argue that time doesn't really exist

21:33

after a few seconds, that it's

21:35

really about where we're putting our

21:37

attention, our internal mental context, what

21:40

we're, how we're basically orchestrating our

21:42

mind, what we're orchestrating our minds

21:44

to think about. This is such

21:46

an extreme, so you're saying time

21:49

does not exist in our memory.

21:51

So something that occurred in, let's

21:53

say, real clock time that was only

21:55

a couple of seconds could feel like

21:57

an hour in your memory or something.

22:00

that lasted five hours could feel

22:02

like a few minutes in your

22:04

memory. Yeah, so we don't really

22:06

ask how long it feels in

22:08

objective time because I think people

22:10

don't even know how to do

22:12

that. I mean, in general, if

22:14

I ask you to retrieve a

22:16

memory from your traveling here this

22:18

morning, subjectively what you get is

22:21

not a continuous unfolding of the

22:23

trip, you get these like... Slightly

22:25

movements, just little snapshots of where

22:27

you were. And those snapshots are

22:29

probably happening at the times you

22:31

made a turn in the car.

22:33

Right, so there are these moments

22:35

that these experiences that we think

22:37

tile our experiences, these boundaries between

22:39

events. So whenever the context changes

22:42

a little, the brain is taking

22:44

in those moments. But how much

22:46

time is in between, whether it

22:48

was a really long trip on

22:50

Broadway or just two blocks? takes

22:52

up the same space in our

22:54

brain. From just a neural perspective,

22:56

when there's change, this variation, there's

22:58

dopamine that is released for novelty,

23:01

and that that might tag these

23:03

moments as being important to remember?

23:05

Yeah, absolutely. There's dopamine, there's also

23:07

norepine, which is related to our

23:09

attentional systems, and the norepinephrine is...

23:11

allowing, we think, is telling us

23:13

to take in, where are you?

23:15

Where's your contacts? Like when your

23:17

viewpoint changes in perception, the brain

23:20

really quickly wants to know where

23:22

it is. it really needs to

23:24

have a sense of the context

23:26

that it's in. And some people

23:28

refer to that as the event

23:30

model. What context am I in?

23:32

What behaviors are required of me

23:34

in this context? So that's what

23:36

the brain is really good about,

23:38

within a few hundred milliseconds knowing

23:41

exactly if it needs to change

23:43

what active representations are available to

23:45

you. Dopamine is also released, and

23:47

Dopamine has been correlated with your

23:49

ability to now form a new

23:51

memory of that experience. So it

23:53

has a more long term. effect

23:55

on the formation of the memory.

23:57

And this is an area that

24:00

is really kind of hot right now,

24:02

and there's a lot of work that's

24:04

being done to look at more epinephrin

24:07

and dopamine at event boundaries. So

24:09

tell me, what happened doing lockdown,

24:11

COVID, right, with time perception,

24:13

when you talk about that, because most

24:15

of us had this feeling, on the one

24:17

end, it was way in the past,

24:19

most of us don't even remember anymore.

24:22

On the other hand, this year collapsed

24:24

to one or two points. So what

24:26

happened there. We all went through this

24:28

similar experience and it was

24:30

very difficult for everyone, including

24:32

me, but it was also

24:34

very motivating for our research.

24:36

Before the pandemic, we had

24:38

already been studying event boundaries

24:40

and event representations and what's

24:42

the relationship between kind of the

24:45

ebb and flow of our daily lives and

24:47

what's a memory? We had been studying that.

24:49

The pandemic... You mean the beginning and the

24:51

end of a memory? Yeah, what is one

24:53

episodic memory? Where does it start? Where

24:55

does it end? And those are at

24:58

these moments of change, we've discovered. But

25:00

at the pandemic, there was no beginning

25:02

and middle and end. We were always

25:04

in the same context, the same, and

25:06

definitely so we were in the same

25:08

apartment, in the same house. Maybe you

25:10

were lucky enough to have a backyard.

25:12

You were in your backyard. We all

25:14

were having groceries delivered delivered delivered to

25:16

us. And so what I think has

25:18

happened is that there's been a global

25:20

reduction in overall brain health over the

25:22

pandemic. So not having those moments

25:24

of novelty where nor epinephrine is

25:26

released, your pupil's dilate, the brain

25:29

is taking in information. It's like

25:31

a muscle, like really, like the

25:33

brain is like a muscle. If

25:35

you're not using it in the

25:37

proper way, you're going to see

25:39

a deterioration. And so... If we're in

25:41

that same context for months, let's say

25:43

March 2020, until if you were lucky

25:45

enough, September, you had some normalcy, but

25:47

here in New York, really it wasn't

25:50

until a year later where there was

25:52

people, you were able to go out. You

25:54

had those experiences at the end of the

25:56

day, you knew what had happened that day.

25:58

You still had like a... like a little

26:00

memory, a short-term memory, but now trying

26:03

to recall all those memories, they're all

26:05

represented in the same context, and so

26:07

they're completely interfering with each other. So

26:09

our normal consolidation mechanisms, when you go

26:12

to sleep, like I said, are built

26:14

to kind of rehearse. The brain rehearses

26:16

the important events, but it also rehearses

26:18

the context. When did it happen? Where

26:21

did it happened? And those keys help

26:23

you to unlock the memory later on.

26:25

So again, if I ask you, like,

26:27

what did you have for lunch yesterday?

26:30

Your brain is first going to go

26:32

to where were you? What context was

26:34

I in? And that's going to help

26:36

you to think about, oh, where was

26:38

the plate situated? Where did it come

26:41

in? Was I at a restaurant? But

26:43

if you had 100 meals in the

26:45

same room you're in, would not be

26:47

a key, that would work. for a

26:50

specific memory. It would work to tell

26:52

you generally where did we eat, but

26:54

it wouldn't really give you any information

26:56

about specifics. So we're all left with

26:59

very few memories from the pandemic. That's

27:01

so interesting. So it's really memory is

27:03

tagged to like sort of changes and

27:05

you tag it to things in the

27:07

environment and in your context. But if

27:10

everything is remaining the same. all the

27:12

time. You don't really have anything to

27:14

kind of latch on. What do you

27:16

latch on to? So it's not a

27:19

faithful record of what happened, but more

27:21

of the salient events at a new

27:23

novel and interesting. And so that seems

27:25

to explain then the paradox that seemingly

27:28

time was very slow during the pandemic.

27:30

Very slow. And in hindsight, I can't

27:32

even say what actually what nothing much

27:34

happened. Right. The other side of that

27:37

coin is when there's a lot happening.

27:39

in your day. Let's say you live

27:41

a really busy life and you're constantly

27:43

going from meeting to meeting or you're

27:45

on a vacation and you know you're

27:48

moving quickly through experiences. If you stop

27:50

in that moment and someone asked you,

27:52

how is your day going? How does

27:54

time feel? You would report it going

27:57

too fast. Like time is going too

27:59

fast. I kind of almost like you

28:01

don't like it. It's too much. Like

28:03

I feel frazzled. But that evening, in

28:06

retrospect, you actually have, you have all

28:08

these memories you can pick up and

28:10

recall in detail. And so your memory

28:12

for time expands. Later on, in the

28:14

moment, you say, I have no time,

28:17

time is short, time is short. Later

28:19

on, as you're retrieving the memories, you're

28:21

using those memories to infer time.

28:23

That's the paradox. And then you say,

28:25

oh, it was a really long day. I

28:27

had a lot going on. So much. So

28:30

what's the best for mental health? So

28:32

this is what I was thinking

28:34

about on the way. So the

28:36

best is not one or the

28:38

other. The best is, again, something

28:40

in the middle. And I actually

28:42

think it's even more precision focused

28:44

than that. I think what might be good

28:46

for you might be different than

28:48

what's good for me. And

28:50

unfortunately... our world, especially the

28:52

Western world, is built up

28:54

on from the time kids

28:56

are three years old, they're

28:58

being told when they need to

29:01

do things and how long they

29:03

need to be doing things. And

29:05

so we're trying to push people

29:07

into this box of 40-minute class,

29:10

five-minute break, and then in college,

29:12

and then in our jobs. And

29:14

I think that at some point,

29:16

what we should do for

29:18

ourselves... as adults, and then

29:21

hopefully in the educational system,

29:23

is figure out what timescale

29:25

works for us. In other

29:27

words, we need to figure out

29:29

how to interleave moments of stability,

29:32

so we're forming these integrative memories

29:34

and moments of change. So we need

29:36

to have both in the day. And not

29:38

all one or all the other. And what

29:40

about these flow states too when you get

29:42

into kind of a flow state and you...

29:44

You mean when you are now when you're focused

29:47

on... Something really intensely and it

29:49

feels like time flies by and you

29:51

know where does that fit in? I can't say

29:53

that I have been in a flow state ever

29:56

in my life. I'm feeling... Why don't you

29:58

describe it more? We need to talk... about

30:00

this. It's a feeling where you lose your

30:02

sense of time and self in place. You're

30:04

fully engaged with the world, like

30:06

when climb or long distance runings

30:08

you have to be hyper focused

30:11

in one sense, but it's not about

30:13

the ego. You experience time as Heather

30:15

said, suddenly you sort of become too

30:17

and realize, oh, here I am climbing

30:20

or running or writing code. A lot

30:22

of people have it when they've write

30:24

code, totally absorbs you, but you're very

30:26

content, very happy. I have an

30:28

example of where my perception of

30:30

time was completely distorted in my

30:32

memory and it happened when I

30:35

was giving birth. I'll tell you

30:37

my experience and maybe it's similar. No,

30:39

not in the full, but where time changed.

30:41

If only. If only. It was the last

30:43

like moments, what it felt like last

30:45

moments when you had to like push

30:48

and push and but eventually I had

30:50

to have an emergency C section because she

30:52

couldn't come out. But this pushing part of

30:54

it. Like you would have to wait until

30:56

the contractions come and then they would have

30:58

you push and push. And to me it

31:00

felt like I tried doing that for like

31:02

maybe 20 minutes or so until they said, you

31:04

know, it's just not going to work and we're going

31:06

to have to go for the C section. It

31:08

turned out it was like three hours or something

31:11

like that, which I still can't grasp my mind

31:13

around. Oh, you expensive. It's 20 minutes. Yeah, I

31:15

thought I tried pushing a few times and then they

31:17

were like, no, we got to go to go

31:19

to go to go to go to go to

31:22

go to go to the C section. I had

31:24

been in labor already for hours and hours and

31:26

hours so this was like at the tail end

31:28

of it because I was like you gave up

31:30

so quickly and took me to the C section

31:33

like couldn't we try it harder and they were

31:35

like you were there for three hours I did

31:37

not feel that at all I don't remember any

31:39

of that I just remember about 20

31:42

minutes of pushing but what was

31:44

your experience was it different

31:46

similar? almost 72 hours, mostly at home,

31:48

and it wasn't constant. So I would

31:50

be in active labor Monday to Thursday.

31:53

All night I was in active labor

31:55

about four or five minutes apart, and

31:57

they tell you to go to the

31:59

hospital. three or four. So I was

32:01

waiting to cross that threshold. Active labor

32:04

all night and then as soon as

32:06

the sun would come up it would

32:08

slow down. It was still there but

32:10

it would slow down. So this went

32:13

on for three days and it felt

32:15

forever. I had three turns of the

32:17

sun really slow, never left the apartment,

32:19

would slowly, tried all kinds of things

32:21

and it just felt like it was

32:24

never going to end. It felt very

32:26

long and extended. And I like to

32:28

contrast that with like the one minute

32:30

between. actually giving birth to my daughter

32:33

and being able to see her face

32:35

and holding her. And in that, literally,

32:37

I didn't, you know, it felt like

32:39

one or two minutes. I was looking

32:42

up at the clock and I heard

32:44

everyone in the room and you heard

32:46

the gel and the weight being called

32:48

out and my husband went and picked

32:50

her up and she came and looked

32:53

at me. And so now I have

32:55

this very rich memory of the sequence

32:57

of events that occurred in that two

32:59

minute period. But my memory of the

33:02

labor is... just I don't know me

33:04

floating around in a room is very

33:06

small and very short even though it

33:08

was a much longer period of time

33:10

so I think that exemplifies some of

33:13

the ideas that we're talking about. Yeah

33:15

I think really it does exemplify this

33:17

this what I find fascinating it really

33:19

explains a lot for me as well

33:22

as that the more sort of monotonous

33:24

the time is it could be really

33:26

long in real time but your memory

33:28

of it is much is contracted the

33:31

more either numerous changing events that occur

33:33

within a period of time or you

33:35

know emotionally salient events that occur can

33:37

be short in the real time but

33:39

in retrospect in your memory can be

33:42

expanded. Exactly that's the paradox so yeah

33:44

in some ways I think we like

33:46

to prioritize the experience if you're prioritizing

33:48

your experience then having that stability is

33:51

nice. So think of, you know, a

33:53

long day on the beach or a

33:55

Sunday where you decide you're just going

33:57

to stay home because you need to

33:59

recoup. You need time in the moment

34:02

to rest. But that's not going to

34:04

be a day that you remember later on.

34:06

And that's okay. Like sometimes we

34:08

need to have those moments. But

34:10

you don't want to have too

34:12

many of those because that is

34:14

what depression feels like. Nothing changes.

34:16

Your mental state is constantly in

34:19

the same space. And even though

34:21

you're walking around the world and

34:23

things are changing. I think that

34:25

depression is another way of thinking

34:27

about it is that you're being

34:29

drawn to this local minima of

34:31

your brain always working in a similar

34:33

way. The depressive people have worse memory

34:36

than non-deprest? They do. Yeah.

34:38

What many fallacies and computational people

34:40

emphasize these days that time itself

34:43

is a construct of your mind. Yeah.

34:45

doesn't really exist. It's not a

34:47

natural mapping between external time and

34:49

time in our head. It's a

34:52

construct. Yeah. Imagine then the ultimate

34:54

freedom where instead of waking up

34:56

the way we wake up, most

34:58

people wake up and think, oh, I

35:00

got to get to work, got to

35:02

have a meeting, what's your day look

35:05

like, your calendars? We have the spatial

35:07

representation of our day. Imagine waking waking

35:09

up and thinking, well, what do

35:11

I want to do today? What do

35:14

I want to accomplish? the space to

35:16

get deep into that context, your mental

35:18

context, to really let yourself sort out

35:20

problems. No, but we feel like some

35:23

of us have that freedom. We should.

35:25

Once you're tired, if you have a

35:27

bit tired. No, but I feel like that's

35:29

the problem. I think that's a

35:31

big problem with our field as

35:33

scientists, like universities and institutes hire

35:36

who they deem to be like

35:38

the most. you know, productive creative

35:40

people, and then they put them

35:42

on this crazy schedule that doesn't

35:44

allow their brains to be creative. So

35:47

do they have different memories? Do they,

35:49

because they don't have this busy, oh

35:51

my God, my first Zoom call is

35:53

in 10 minutes? That's a good question.

35:55

I don't know the answer to that, but

35:57

I do tend to notice people who

35:59

take up a new skill. They learn

36:01

the piano or they learn painting. They

36:03

allow themselves to get, skill learning is

36:06

really hard. It takes many hours and

36:08

you know, and I think they allow

36:10

themselves to pick up skills that are

36:12

quite impressive and we think of as

36:15

mostly children are good at picking up

36:17

those skills. But I don't know the

36:19

answer to how they feel about time

36:21

and memory. I think there's something to

36:23

be said with not over scheduling and

36:26

you know, allowing yourself to... Take the

36:28

time you need to think deeply or

36:30

get deep into something. Can you please

36:32

discuss this discussion? I was on the

36:34

phone call. And the last thing, you

36:37

know, I think time does exist in

36:39

the physical world. Like there is, you

36:41

know, time is a dimension in physics,

36:43

but our perception of time is a

36:45

creation of our minds. And I've even

36:48

looked at some of the research that

36:50

I would disagree with that. There's this

36:52

block time universe that says everything is

36:54

simultaneous. Like the block time. Maybe there's

36:57

no such thing as time, but our

36:59

perception of time. research that I've done

37:01

looks at when people have brain damage

37:03

to certain parts of the brain, the

37:05

dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex in particular, it

37:08

really affects their perception of time. People

37:10

who have also orbital prefrontal cortex lesions,

37:12

it affects their perception of time. So

37:14

we know that it's, our perception is

37:16

created in our brain and it can

37:19

vary depending on that neurochemistry or, you

37:21

know, the neuroanotomy. And we should just

37:23

take it all with sort of a

37:25

grain of a grain of salt, right?

37:27

It's all a construct. So

37:33

we end every episode asking a

37:36

perception box question. And we don't

37:38

know, I don't know what question

37:40

Christoph has chosen. He doesn't know

37:42

what I've chosen. So we're going

37:44

to throw it out there and

37:46

we all can answer the question

37:48

or not, depending on how you

37:50

feel. So do you have a

37:52

question? Yes and no. Okay, go

37:54

for it. What is the one

37:56

memory that you keep on returning

37:58

to? Is there a sort of

38:00

a dominant memory that you keep

38:02

on returning to? And why would

38:04

that be so? I have to

38:06

think of this. Is there a

38:08

dominant memory? Or something that sort

38:10

of spontaneously unbidden keeps on coming

38:12

back once a day? My mind

38:14

is like a playground, right? And

38:16

you get to choose where you

38:18

go in it, right? When you're

38:20

just sort of daydreaming. So I

38:22

have less intrusive. I don't have

38:24

so many intrusive thoughts or memories,

38:26

but sometimes I'm like, it would

38:28

be fun to remember this right

38:30

now, or I want to go

38:32

live in this place for a

38:34

moment. I had to go into

38:36

general anesthesia just one time in

38:38

my life for back surgery, and

38:40

I was very nervous about it.

38:42

And I was told to just

38:44

like, as you're getting wheeled into

38:46

the OR, like, before you go

38:48

under, like, try to kind of

38:50

think of something, a memory that

38:52

brings you to a sort of

38:54

your happy place. So I did,

38:56

you know, my PhD was at

38:58

Oxford at this at Maldon College,

39:00

this beautiful college that was like

39:02

magic to me. C.S. Lewis went

39:04

there and, but it had these,

39:06

this like the cloisters. And whenever

39:08

I'd walk in these cloisters, it

39:10

just brought me such peace and

39:12

comfort and just like I was

39:14

connected to like history and whatever,

39:16

and it's beautiful like wisteria growing.

39:18

And that is my happy place.

39:20

So whenever I feel nervous or

39:22

anxious, I will just go back.

39:24

And in my mind, it's like

39:26

so vivid, I can see every

39:28

detail, I can see myself walking

39:30

through these cloisters and the bells

39:32

chiming in the background, it brings

39:34

me such peace and joy. So

39:36

that's a memory I go back

39:38

to a lot on purpose. Yeah.

39:40

Yeah. Yeah. I don't have an

39:42

answer for you. I mean, I'm

39:44

cued by what you said and

39:46

I can think I love nature

39:49

and I would choose to be

39:51

in nature because I feel more

39:53

calm. But I'm constantly planning. And

39:55

so I like I'm a listmaker

39:57

so my brain is oriented towards

39:59

the future I can't recommend Matt

40:01

for everyone. Every researcher who's focused

40:03

on the future. Right. So I

40:05

am efficient. I'm reading a book

40:07

by Miranda July and there's a

40:09

distinction between people who are parkers

40:11

versus drivers. Like are you a

40:14

parker or a driver? And I

40:16

love that distinction because I'm definitely

40:18

a parker. I'm a parker even

40:21

about driving. So unfortunately,

40:23

I can't, I don't have, I mean, I

40:25

do, my brain does tend to. I will admit,

40:27

it does tend to reactivate situations

40:29

that I wish would have gone

40:32

differently. Like I play a lot

40:34

of counterfactual thinking from the past,

40:36

but I'm often planning. What about

40:38

you? Could he answer your own question?

40:40

Yeah. Well, so I too incredible

40:42

meaningful, one this near-death experience that

40:44

I think about every day, it

40:46

happened almost, you know, for an

40:49

years ago, I returned to time

40:51

after time, because it was so

40:53

meaningful, and then I had this...

40:55

sort of what people call a

40:57

mystical experience again, and you come back

40:59

to this bidden or unbidden

41:02

constantly. Both, by the

41:04

way, have no time. So my question

41:06

is, is there any action that

41:08

you or something you want to

41:10

do that or actually want to

41:12

take that you just don't feel

41:14

ready yet to do or to take?

41:17

Yes, and it's what you alluded to.

41:19

To go to the state where I

41:21

wake up and the entire day... There's

41:23

nothing planned. I'm not doing anything.

41:25

I'm not giving a Zoom talk

41:27

or, you know, writing a paper

41:29

or a book or whatever. But

41:31

I'm also afraid of getting to

41:33

that state. So what about you? Yeah.

41:35

Well, I would think that you should be

41:38

able to do that any time. I think

41:40

that that is a great goal for all

41:42

of us. Again, but the plan for mine.

41:44

But then you actually have to

41:46

do it for you. That's more

41:48

challenging. Oh my God. I'm not

41:51

going to do anything today. How

41:53

does it make me feel and

41:55

look? Yeah. I think for me,

41:57

I think a lot about wanting

41:59

to be... in a state

42:01

where I'm closer to nature and

42:03

particularly animals. I have this dream

42:05

to swim next to a whale.

42:07

So this is like this, and

42:09

I don't think that will ever

42:11

happen in my lifetime. So I

42:13

think that I think of myself

42:15

as diving into the water. I

42:17

have this image of complete freedom.

42:19

I could swim with dolphins. It's

42:21

true. I can probably make. But

42:23

that would be in a like

42:25

a closed. I think in a

42:27

big ocean. I would like to

42:29

be able to be more comfortable

42:31

with letting go of control. And

42:33

I love water and I feel

42:35

completely free when I dive into

42:38

the water. And so I would

42:40

love, that's what I think about,

42:42

I would love to be in

42:44

that space one day where I

42:46

jump off a boat and I'm

42:48

swimming with a well. I love

42:50

that. I would like to be

42:52

able to be more comfortable with

42:54

letting go of control. And it's,

42:56

you know. It's something I want

42:58

to work towards where, because I

43:00

always feel like I'm more comfortable

43:02

when I'm in control of things.

43:04

And I don't like surprises, I

43:06

don't like not knowing, and I

43:08

just, it's hard. It's easy for

43:10

me to let go in certain

43:12

contexts, like if I'm painting or

43:14

in a flow state or whatever,

43:16

then I can let go, but

43:18

like letting go in like the

43:20

external world and it scares me.

43:22

So I want to work toward

43:24

that. You're a park or two.

43:26

Yeah, and I'm really good at

43:28

panel of parking. Yeah, exactly. So

43:30

good that people clock. Yes. So

43:32

yeah, I want to be able

43:34

to figure out how to let

43:36

go of control a little bit

43:38

more. Well, I want to thank

43:40

you, Lila, for of... wonderful conversation

43:42

and for joining us today. And

43:44

if you'd like to learn more

43:46

about your own perception box, spend

43:48

some time this week answering the

43:50

same perception box questions that we

43:52

asked our guest and check out

43:54

other questions on our website at

43:56

unlikely collaborators.com. You can also subscribe

43:58

to our YouTube channel. and watch

44:00

the show or listen wherever you

44:02

get your podcasts. This

44:05

has been Science of

44:07

Perception Box created by

44:09

unlikely collaborators in partnership

44:11

with pot people. I'm Dr.

44:14

Heather Berlin. And I'm Dr.

44:16

Kristof Koch. Thank you very

44:18

much.

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features