Episode Transcript
Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.
Use Ctrl + F to search
0:00
What if you could take the
0:02
drug out of the trip? In
0:04
other words, recreate a
0:06
psychedelic-like experience without
0:09
reference to a
0:11
psychedelic drug itself? Hi.
0:14
I'm Elizabeth Coke. We all
0:16
live inside our own personal
0:18
private perception box. built by
0:21
our genes and the physical,
0:23
social, and cultural environment in
0:25
which we were born and
0:28
raised. In this podcast, we
0:30
explore how although the walls
0:32
of this mental box are
0:35
always present, they can expand
0:37
in states like awe, wonder,
0:39
and curiosity, or contract
0:41
in response to anxiety, fear,
0:44
and anger. I'd like to
0:46
introduce our esteemed hosts. to
0:49
incredible and distinguished minds. Dr.
0:51
Heather Berlin, professor of psychiatry
0:53
and neuroscience at the Icon
0:56
School of Medicine at Mount
0:58
Sinai in New York City. And
1:00
Dr. Kristof Koch, chief scientist for
1:02
the Tiny Blue Dot Foundation,
1:04
and the current meritorious investigator
1:07
and former president of the
1:09
Allen Institute for Brain Science.
1:12
Welcome to the Science of
1:14
Perception Box. Hi
1:18
everybody, welcome to Science of Perception
1:20
Box. I'm your co-host, Dr. Heather
1:23
Berlin. I'm your co-host, Dr. Kristof
1:25
Kaur. So every week we feature an
1:27
aspect of the Science of Perception
1:30
Box, highlighting the latest research together
1:32
with our expert guests. This week
1:34
we're exploring the powerful research
1:36
around psychedelics and dream states
1:38
in the practice of anesthesiology
1:41
with a researcher and doctor
1:43
who has been as fascinated
1:45
about consciousness as we are. Dr.
1:47
Boris Heifitz is a
1:49
board-certified anesthesiologist who specializes
1:51
in providing anesthesia for
1:53
neurological surgery. He's practiced
1:56
at Stanford since 2010.
1:58
In addition to treating pain... Dr.
2:00
Heifitz also directs clinical research and
2:02
basic neuroscience. His research group studies
2:05
how new rapid-acting psychiatric therapies, like
2:07
kenamine, MDMA, and cylacidin, produce lasting
2:09
changes in nervous system function, behavior,
2:12
and therapeutic outcomes. But first we
2:14
want to share our own connection
2:17
to psychedelics. They made me lose
2:19
a sense of self. They made
2:21
me lose my sense of body
2:24
of an external world, but they
2:26
were still consciousness. And they made
2:28
me lose my fear of death.
2:31
Well, that's pretty profound. So do
2:33
you think you're more relaxed person
2:35
law? Yes, certainly. You seem very
2:38
relaxed. Well, Boris, thanks for joining
2:40
us. Thanks for having me. How
2:42
did you become interested in psychedelics
2:45
as an anesthesiologist? So I became
2:47
interested in psychedelics well before I
2:49
became an anesthesiologist. I see. The
2:52
question is how did I become
2:54
interested in anesthesiology as a psychedelics
2:56
explorer? You mean psychonote? Well, I've
2:59
done some research on the topic
3:01
before before becoming a doctor. When
3:03
I was towards the end of
3:06
my PhD training. So I did
3:08
an MD and a PhD at
3:10
Einstein in New York. I had
3:13
this question of like, well, how
3:15
am I going to apply this?
3:17
What we're doing is we're looking
3:20
at synaptic responses in brain tissue,
3:22
we're looking at the inner workings
3:25
of how circuits function in the
3:27
brain. And I didn't want to
3:29
lose that, right? And I wanted
3:32
to pick a specialty that would
3:34
allow me the most access to
3:36
that kind of thinking, that kind
3:39
of environment. And, you know, not
3:41
to mention. this background interest in
3:43
psychedelics. I think when I started
3:46
medical school my mom told me
3:48
drop it. You can't have a
3:50
career. You can't have a career
3:53
studying psychedelics, it's hippiescience. This is
3:55
in 2000, probably good advice. But
3:57
when it came time to pick
4:00
a specialty, I was thinking about
4:02
psychiatry, neurology, neurosurgery, an anesthesiology. And
4:04
the most, the closest I could
4:07
get to applied neuroscience was actually
4:09
an anesthesiology. You give, you give
4:11
a drug, and you see its
4:14
effect on the brain, the body.
4:16
on consciousness itself and that to
4:18
me it never gets old. How
4:21
do you know that consciousness is
4:23
gone as compared to they're unable
4:25
to talk and they're unable to
4:28
signal that they're still there? How
4:30
do you know they're not present?
4:33
So we have a lot of
4:35
interviews with patients after surgery that
4:37
you know are it can attest
4:40
to that is that what's kind
4:42
of remarkable about anesthesia is that
4:44
it's not like sleep. And when
4:47
you close your eyes as you
4:49
go off to, as you are
4:51
anesthetized for surgery, when you open
4:54
them, many patients will feel that
4:56
no time is passed. It's very
4:58
different from when you wake up.
5:01
You kind of have an intuitive
5:03
sense of how long you may
5:05
have slept. So that's already one
5:08
difference. Just at the level of
5:10
what the patient experiences. One of
5:12
the earliest concerns for anesthesia, as
5:15
you know, we were developing anesthetic
5:17
techniques to keep people immobile. and
5:19
pain-free and amnestic, right, during surgery
5:22
is there's something called the Bryce
5:24
questionnaire. What's the last thing you
5:26
remember before going to sleep? What's
5:29
the first thing you remember when
5:31
you wake up? And did you
5:33
have any dreams during anesthesia? Do
5:36
you recall anything? And that's, you
5:38
know, that's basically how we set
5:41
standards for what depth of anesthesia
5:43
we use. So how often does
5:45
it happen that I guess depending
5:48
on the type of anesthesia and
5:50
the duration that people do? Recall
5:52
something that does relate in some
5:55
way to something that did happen
5:57
in the O. So it's around
5:59
one in a... So, awareness under
6:02
general anesthesia, even with EEG monitors.
6:04
So, you know, we're monitoring brain
6:06
function, we're getting a sense of
6:08
the depth, but we're clearly not
6:10
getting the whole picture because every once
6:12
in a while a patient will
6:14
recall something. Now, I want to
6:16
put a qualifier on that before,
6:18
you know, nobody ever has surgery
6:20
again after listening to this podcast.
6:22
I've seen one case like this,
6:25
and in an elderly woman, not who
6:27
I would have expected. And though
6:29
the things she said was everything
6:31
was so far away that everyone
6:34
was touching me really softly,
6:36
but she was recalling conversations in
6:38
the OR, so there's a deep
6:40
disconnection, but she was able to
6:42
maintain some sort of input from
6:44
the real world. So that, again,
6:46
that's one in a thousand. and
6:48
no pain because she didn't complain
6:50
of pain she didn't complain you
6:52
know it was a little bit
6:54
distressing to her because she'd kind
6:56
of she knew what happened but
6:58
she was you know she wasn't
7:00
paralyzed she didn't she the thing
7:02
that I was most worried about
7:04
is did you feel trapped and
7:06
you know thank God she didn't
7:08
and that's again advances in anesthesia
7:10
have allowed us to do surgery without
7:13
paralysis even in some cases So, again,
7:15
this may be a topic for another
7:17
time, but it's, you know, it's
7:19
a rare complication that we
7:21
do worry about. And again,
7:23
that was the early concern
7:25
of anesthesiologist, is that we
7:27
want to make sure that
7:29
consciousness is gone, right? Deep
7:31
disconnection, disconnected unconsciousness, that, you know,
7:33
that's reversible at the end of
7:35
surgery. Now, in the last 20 years,
7:38
you know, anesthesia has gotten a
7:40
lot safer. We've started... innovating
7:42
things like nerve blocks, for example.
7:44
So now we have a little bit more room
7:46
to think about, you know, how much do
7:49
I want to sedate this patient? What kind
7:51
of experience do I want to provide?
7:53
And that's where things start to
7:55
get a little bit interesting. So
7:57
before we get into the research, can you...
7:59
So tell us a little bit about
8:02
how psychedelics work in the
8:04
brain and how they can be used
8:06
as a therapeutic intervention. Sure,
8:08
so this is a very hot topic
8:10
right now. And you know, when we talk
8:12
about psychedelics, what are we
8:15
talking about? There's the classic
8:17
psychedelics, LSD, syllacide. The classics,
8:19
the greatest hits. And then
8:22
there are other drugs that
8:24
are, I would say, psychedelic
8:27
adjacent, that a lot of
8:29
people will identify psychedelic-like properties
8:32
in these drugs. Drugs like
8:34
MDMA, which is nearing approval,
8:36
potentially, for PTSD, ketedomy. Assisted
8:39
therapy. Yes, MDMA assisted therapy, which
8:41
we're going to return to that point,
8:43
I hope. So, and these drugs
8:45
are not, you know, they don't have
8:48
the same perceptual. effect, but they
8:50
clearly are, you know, they
8:52
are acutely psychoactive in a
8:54
profound way that is very
8:56
memorable and unmistakable. And what
8:58
ties all of these together
9:00
and what has, you know,
9:02
led to this explosion of
9:04
research and excitement is
9:06
that when you provide a space
9:08
for people to have a powerful
9:10
psychoactive drug in a
9:13
safe setting and just let them,
9:15
you know, let their mind wander.
9:17
Things come out. Things that, you know,
9:19
if you have PTSD, if you have
9:21
painful memories, if there are things that
9:24
you haven't been able to resolve in
9:26
your life, what people talk about is
9:28
getting a new perspective. You
9:30
know, whether it be like, what is
9:32
going on in my body? Where is
9:35
this pain coming from? Or, you know,
9:37
who is this person who's, you know,
9:39
always depressed and, you know, pessimistic, like,
9:41
all, you know, this perspective shift is
9:44
something that people across these
9:46
drug classes. will talk about
9:48
and to me that's
9:50
what defines this broad
9:52
class. They're acutely psychoactive,
9:54
powerfully so. Their effects are
9:57
rapid, that perspective shift
9:59
or how However you want to
10:01
call it, it's rapid. Change in
10:03
a perception box. You can put
10:05
it that way as well. And
10:07
finally, the effects are durable. This
10:10
is very different from modern mental
10:12
health care. You don't need to
10:14
take these drugs every day. You
10:16
have, long after the ketamine or
10:18
the MDMA or the sulcybin, has
10:20
cleared your bloodstream, you are still
10:22
feeling those positive effects. This is
10:25
a sea change in. in how
10:27
therapy is delivered. And I want
10:29
to emphasize again, it is far
10:31
more complex than just giving someone
10:33
a drug, putting them in a
10:35
room, you know, while they, you
10:38
know, have and have their experience.
10:40
So it's the synergistic effect between
10:42
the actual, you know, physical effects
10:44
of the drug in the brains
10:46
and this psychological effects or impact.
10:48
I'm going to challenge that. I
10:50
think that's actually one of the
10:53
biggest debates right now. And it
10:55
has implications. Is it the drug?
10:57
or the trip that's responsible for
10:59
these therapeutic benefits. Right, like if
11:01
you get the same benefits without...
11:03
Zooming, A and B, empirically, experimentally,
11:05
you can separate those two. That's
11:08
exactly it, right? That's why there's
11:10
the debate, is right now all
11:12
we're looking at is correlation. And
11:14
so tell us more about this
11:16
debate. It boils down to this,
11:18
as you can think of, you
11:21
know, the complexity of psychotherapy, you
11:23
can simplify into three basic stages.
11:25
There's preparation. which involves setting expectations,
11:27
building rapport. There's the drug experience
11:29
itself, possibly eight-hour extravaganza sometimes. 24
11:31
hours. Or 48, it depends, you
11:33
know, the doses, everything. And then
11:36
there's the integration, making sense of
11:38
what happens and trying to incorporate
11:40
those changes into your life. Which
11:42
can take weeks, right? And in
11:44
trials, that's how long it takes,
11:46
that you have weeks of after
11:49
therapy. So, you know, the inclination,
11:51
you know, based on decades of
11:53
experience in, you know, pharma, is
11:55
well, it's got to be that
11:57
little crystalline entity at the... in
11:59
the middle of all of this
12:01
that's driving these effects. But in
12:04
reality, as you pointed out, you
12:06
cannot possibly attribute to one factor
12:08
the therapeutic change unless you can
12:10
independently manipulate them. And this is
12:12
where the science comes in. I'm
12:14
going to put in a brief
12:16
plug for why any of this
12:19
matters. There are people who believe,
12:21
not without cause, that... It's enough
12:23
that it works. Widely, wide, get
12:25
so bent out of shape about
12:27
how it works, it's just enough
12:29
that it does work. And I
12:32
would answer to that is, you
12:34
know, one thing that anesthesia has
12:36
brought me in contact with is
12:38
some of the most advanced medicine
12:40
on earth. And when I see,
12:42
you know, one of the most
12:44
magical moments in residency, and I
12:47
don't need to gross you without
12:49
but watching a transplanted heart get
12:51
put into someone's chest, vibrulate and
12:53
then... convert to sinus rhythm. It's
12:55
like watching birth, like, or the
12:57
earth being born, or a total
13:00
eclipse. It was just all-inspiring. How
13:02
did we get from someone living
13:04
for 50 hours in the 60s
13:06
after a hard transplant to let,
13:08
you know, 80% at five years?
13:10
This is amazing. It's by understanding
13:12
the risk and... understanding the mechanism.
13:15
Mechanism, I mean science, this is
13:17
how science works. And so that
13:19
to me is the question, if
13:21
a therapy is truly potent, by
13:23
definition it carries risk, right? And
13:25
when you think about the early
13:27
days of chemotherapy, chemotherapy in 1975
13:30
was almost a death sentence in
13:32
itself. Thirty years later you have
13:34
the first rationally designed Kainase Inhibitor.
13:36
That blew my mind in 1999.
13:38
Guevak. cured leukemia and you know
13:40
what again that's you take something
13:43
that has it's a powerful chemotherapy
13:45
you know with powerful and crude
13:47
and we learn something and we
13:49
learn something and we learn something
13:51
and we learn something highly effective
13:53
in targeting. I'm not sure that
13:55
CNS, you know, that psychedelics are
13:58
going to go that way, but
14:00
there's a pretty strong track record
14:02
in every other field of medicine
14:04
for this approach. There are also
14:06
strong motives in the industry to
14:08
pursue that, because that's what the
14:10
entire medical system is based on.
14:13
You give one little therapeutic intervention
14:15
that the FDA proves that you
14:17
can then, you know, sell to
14:19
everyone that works. But in this
14:21
case, because this is the most
14:23
complex, you're talking to the most
14:26
complex piece of active matter in
14:28
the known universe. And I seriously
14:30
doubt, having studied my entire life,
14:32
that any one drug will be
14:34
a magic bullet that cures whatever
14:36
existential problems that brain or that
14:38
mine has. I'm going to turn
14:41
that on its head in that
14:43
I completely agree with you, but
14:45
how do you, without demonstrating the
14:47
centrality of like... My overall overwhelming
14:49
sense from all the work I've
14:51
done is that we need to
14:54
center the experience. My biggest question
14:56
is whether it's the molecule itself
14:58
or you need the psychological experience.
15:00
And can you isolate the psychological
15:02
experience so they don't have to
15:04
even take the drug? Are there
15:06
other ways to get to that
15:09
transformative experience? Yeah. That's what we're
15:11
trying to develop is that psychedelics
15:13
pose really fundamental challenges for, you
15:15
know, randomized control trials. Let's start
15:17
with that for a second. Because
15:19
you know you're on the drug
15:21
once you're on it. And so
15:24
why would that be a complication?
15:26
So it introduces all kinds of
15:28
biases in that, you know, the
15:30
randomized placebo-controlled trial was designed for
15:32
antibiotics and blood pressure medication. But
15:34
the power of not knowing whether
15:37
you're on the drug or not
15:39
is really to get around the
15:41
placebo effect thing is if there's
15:43
a certain amount of impact that
15:45
the drug can have just thinking
15:47
you've taken the drug. that can
15:49
have an effect. Probably not for
15:52
TB, I don't see. There are
15:54
some things where placebo effect, you
15:56
know, we should be so lucky
15:58
to have a cancer. or placebo
16:00
effect, right? You know, people don't
16:02
spontaneously, often, do not often spontaneously remit,
16:04
you know, just on the strength of their
16:07
belief, although, you know, there are all kinds
16:09
of stories. But it's important because,
16:11
you know, think about it from
16:13
a patient's point of view. You
16:15
have read Michael Paulin's book. You
16:17
are fascinated at the potential of
16:19
stilocybin. You have out-competed a thousand
16:22
other applicants to be in the
16:24
study on depression. You have already
16:26
one lottery. Right? Now you go
16:28
into the finale. And you've already
16:30
done two other trials. You've already
16:32
failed to other trials and like,
16:34
I think this is going to
16:36
be it. And so you have
16:38
an expectation. It's, you know, it's
16:40
obvious that if I'm, if I get
16:42
the drug... I'm likely to improve because look
16:45
at what all of these smart people say.
16:47
And now comes the moment, the moment of
16:49
truth. You're in, you know, your boyfriend,
16:51
your girlfriend drives you, you know, maybe
16:53
you fight about it because you've been
16:55
so kind of persistent in your pursuit.
16:57
You go through a lot of trouble
17:00
to get to that room, to that
17:02
therapist room and then you take the
17:04
drug and an hour later is either
17:06
a moment of... confirmation and acceptance
17:08
and being seen and being in an elite
17:10
group of people on earth who have a
17:12
bid in a syllacyban trial or a moment
17:15
of betrayal where why did I spend all
17:17
of this effort to be in the placebo
17:19
group? Because nothing happens to me. Because it's
17:21
obvious. It's to most, I mean, it's such
17:24
an obvious cycle act of effect. So, you
17:26
know, if you just, it's like, it's like winning
17:28
the lottery. What is the effect of winning
17:30
the lottery? And you tell me, does that,
17:32
what does that have to do with depression?
17:34
I guess like winning the lottery could
17:36
be a short-term anti-depressive, but that's
17:39
sort of the heart of it. And
17:41
so because they've been told in the
17:43
red that these are wonderful drugs, they
17:45
were less likely to be depressed afterwards.
17:47
That's what you said. That's what the
17:49
placebo factor is. Exactly. But because they
17:51
know if they're on the placebo or
17:53
not with psychedelics, it's very hard to
17:55
control for that. So do you have
17:57
a way that you're trying to get around this?
18:00
So, again, if that's one of the
18:02
biggest problems facing psychedelic medicine is identifying
18:04
a drug-specific effect, it requires some innovative
18:06
solutions. And I'll want to talk about
18:09
a couple. One is efforts by David
18:11
Olson and Brian Roth, you know, two
18:13
great chemists and many others who are
18:15
re-engineering the molecule itself. They're basically trying
18:18
to take the trip. out of the
18:20
drug. Crystal, I'm thinking that's no fun.
18:22
Where's the fun in that? They want
18:25
to take the fun in that? This
18:27
is the science. That's the, that to
18:29
me, it's crucial, right? Like you have
18:31
to test. How can you not? I
18:34
assume you can do that. Let's just
18:36
say for a minute it's possible. You
18:38
will get some answer there is can
18:41
you just encode resilience, you know, biochemically
18:43
without anybody noticing, right? What about giving
18:45
the psychedelic while somebody's under anesthesia and
18:47
they have no experience? That happens to
18:50
be what we did. So I'm so
18:52
glad you have. So it requires a
18:54
lot of different approaches that this is
18:57
the one we took. Now I'm an
18:59
anesthesiologist and one, you know, it's hard
19:01
to escape the idea that, you know,
19:03
you have all these people that come
19:06
in from all walks of life, many
19:08
with pre-existing depression PTSD. That's usually not
19:10
what we're focused on. We're usually focused
19:12
on getting them through surgery. And we,
19:15
you know, we saw this as an
19:17
opportunity is that patients are put on,
19:19
you know, they're put under general anesthesia
19:22
and while they're anesthetized, like there's, you
19:24
know, there's no there there, they're not
19:26
there for it, right? That's kind of
19:28
the goal. So what if we gave
19:31
a psychedelic class drug like ketamine during
19:33
anesthesia during anesthesia? not using ketamine as
19:35
an anesthetic. We're using drugs like propafal,
19:38
drugs like cepaphoring. These are standard anesthetic
19:40
cocktails. And we're getting everyone to a
19:42
pretty even cruising depth of anesthesia before
19:44
we give them either ketamine or placebo.
19:47
So they're deep. So if you do
19:49
surgical cut, they don't. That's the goal.
19:51
They are there for surgery. Now, part
19:54
of how we were easily able to
19:56
get approval for this is that ketamine
19:58
is an anesthetic adjunct. So we were,
20:00
you know, in patients for whom there's
20:03
what we would call equipoise about, you
20:05
know, ketamine is kind of, you don't
20:07
need to give it. There's nothing in
20:09
the case that screams out this patient
20:12
should definitely get ketamine. We're able to
20:14
do this trial. And we ran it
20:16
like a psychiatry trial. And this is
20:19
with actual psychiatrists. like Laura Hack and
20:21
Alan Shasberg who helped, you know, quite
20:23
a bit on this study, but we
20:25
ran a psychiatry trial in the operating
20:28
room. And they give half a milligram
20:30
per kilogram over 40 minutes to minimize
20:32
the psychoactive effects. In a regular wake
20:35
person there would be strong psychoactive, yes.
20:37
Yes, yes, and that's what we've seen.
20:39
We've done other, worked on a trial
20:41
with no one Williams where we're giving
20:44
ketamine to awake patients. At this dose.
20:46
And patients, well, they'll have what's called,
20:48
you know, they'll dissociate. They'll get into
20:51
a dreamy state, you know, they might
20:53
hallucinate. If you listen to what they
20:55
say, there is a lot of overlap
20:57
with psychedelic-like effects. And let's put that
21:00
on pause for a minute, but that's
21:02
the trip of ketamine that we're actually
21:04
trying to see, like, do you need
21:07
that in order to benefit from ketamine?
21:09
So now you give them this dose,
21:11
which normally in a wakeful person they
21:13
would have a sort of psychedelic effect,
21:16
but they're under anesthesia and you have
21:18
a placebo controlled, meaning you're going to
21:20
give them another substance, that's not, you
21:22
just give them no kidney. We just
21:25
give them normal saline. Okay. It's, you
21:27
know, a fluid with the same volume
21:29
and I guarantee you the patients were
21:32
not aware. Everyone was blinded in the
21:34
study. So they wake up and what's
21:36
the measurement? What do you assess? So
21:38
again, we want to copy what's been
21:41
done before when I reinventing anything. We're
21:43
using a standard scale of depression called
21:45
the Montgomery Asberg Depression Rating Scale. It's
21:48
a clinician rated scale, meaning I, if
21:50
let's say you're my patient, I'll ask
21:52
you questions about, you know, tell me
21:54
about your fatigue levels or you know,
21:57
how is your... appetite and there's kind
21:59
of standard degrees of severity but you're
22:01
expecting there to be an impact right
22:04
away right after the surgery. That's the
22:06
beauty of ketamine. S-S-S-R-I is you give
22:08
a patient and maybe six weeks later
22:10
they say you'll feel something maybe and
22:13
it's very hard to like make the
22:15
connection between the drug and the impact
22:17
but with ketamine they give it in
22:19
the psych ER and it really can
22:22
knock out suicidality. And there's new ones
22:24
to everything but essentially yes. That's the
22:26
design of this. therapy is that it's
22:29
rapid acting antidepressant. So you give them
22:31
this measure of depression right when they
22:33
come out of surgery. You don't know
22:35
who's had it and who has it.
22:38
So we waited a day. There's a
22:40
lot of things that happened right after
22:42
surgery. But again, we're copying other studies
22:45
where you have the peak effect, the
22:47
peak antidepressant effect of ketamine is one
22:49
to three days after infusion. Long after
22:51
the drug is gone, you know, where
22:54
we started this description. And that's where
22:56
we're taking our primary measure. is looking
22:58
at depression scores in the one to
23:01
three days post-infusion, post-surgery. And what you
23:03
found was? Well, all of the patients
23:05
who got ketamine did great. They, you
23:07
know, 50% response, 30% remission from patients,
23:10
many of whom had treatment-resistant depression. So
23:12
what about the other patients? Well, so
23:14
the placebo group also did great. 50%
23:17
response, 30% remission remission remission. So it's
23:19
remarkable. So you think whether or not
23:21
they got the ketamine. Both groups on
23:23
average showed the same degree of improvement.
23:26
You could not separate them. The key
23:28
here is that both were massive, massive
23:30
improvements. And there's a couple fine points
23:32
here because I got a lot of,
23:35
you know, wasn't exactly fan mail about
23:37
the study, but people who looked at
23:39
the study say, are you saying tetamine
23:42
doesn't work? And there are a couple
23:44
points to bring out about this. And
23:46
the first point is, what was the
23:48
patient experience? likes and you'll see how
23:51
this is important in a minute I
23:53
think. And again keep in mind this
23:55
very large placebo effect that we saw
23:58
that we were absolutely not expecting. from
24:00
a patient, let's say you're coming in for
24:02
surgery, for 20 years you've been dealing with
24:04
a lot of trauma, the holdovers from a
24:07
rough childhood, etc. And now you're, you know,
24:09
you go see your surgeon and do you
24:11
think your surgeon is going to ask you
24:14
about your mood? We can venture against
24:16
it, usually not. It's the rare surgeon
24:18
that has time because the priorities, there
24:20
are other priorities. So from your point
24:23
of view, you're getting something in your
24:25
email saying we care about your mental
24:27
health and recovery after surgery. you know,
24:29
would you be willing to fill out the
24:31
survey and talk to us? That's our first
24:34
contact with the patient a few weeks before
24:36
surgery. And then you come in, you get
24:38
a consent. It's about an hour long where
24:40
you hear all about the study. Kenamine,
24:42
we think it's an antidepressant
24:44
in other circumstances. We're wondering
24:47
whether this has therapeutic value
24:49
during surgery. Now you come in for
24:51
a two-hour interview with, you know, four of
24:53
us, a nurse, myself. a research coordinator. Who
24:55
always interviewed? Justice, part of the work of
24:58
the study. Yes, we want to know everything,
25:00
you know, and from a patient. So you're
25:02
sort of priming, first of all, they're
25:04
getting more attention, you're talking about their
25:06
mood, you're giving, and you're priming them
25:08
to this drug might really help your depression.
25:10
Exactly, and there have been studies of
25:13
depression during surgery before, and I don't
25:15
think they went all out all out like
25:17
this. We were looking for a particular type
25:19
of patient, type of patient, I was so
25:21
happy to get. each one of them, each
25:23
of these 40, that you know, we really,
25:25
we learned a lot about all of them.
25:27
And so two hours where, you
25:29
know, we heard about their trauma,
25:31
their mental health history, their
25:34
physical, you know, their physical history,
25:36
and then, you know, the morning
25:38
of surgery, I, again, I wanted
25:41
to make sure things go out
25:43
without a hitch. In many cases,
25:45
I held their hands as
25:47
they went off to sleep, right? I
25:49
mean, stop there for a second.
25:52
Why was I so blithely
25:54
unaware of the possibility that
25:56
we might induce this massive
25:59
placebo effect? It's because think the
26:01
broader context is surgical anesthesia. Surgery and
26:03
anesthesia are associated with the higher risk
26:05
of heart attack, stroke, cognitive dysfunction, kidney
26:07
injury, lung injury. Actually, all of our
26:09
literature points to all these things getting
26:11
worse after surgery. Nice. That and putting
26:13
people at risk for opioid use disorder.
26:15
So that's what I came in with.
26:17
All right. I was not thinking that
26:20
placebo would be a problem or that
26:22
the study would even be about placebo.
26:24
It was a big surprise. Well, this
26:26
is my question. I mean, what is
26:28
the takeaway here? Is the takeaway that
26:30
for the effects of ketamine, you don't
26:32
need the psychedelic effect of ketamine for
26:34
there to be an improvement? The takeaway
26:36
is this. It's in the placebo effect.
26:38
We can't say much about ketamine in
26:41
this trial, but what I think we
26:43
can say something about is full of
26:45
the trials going on in the psychedelic
26:47
space. Again, I painted that picture for
26:49
you of winning the clinical trial lottery,
26:51
right, and going through that process and
26:53
all the confirmation bias that might go
26:55
along with it, there are a lot
26:57
of non-drug factors there. So inadvertently, just
26:59
the structure of this trial with preparation,
27:02
a big central event, surgery and anesthesia.
27:04
And then, you know, close follow-up in
27:06
the aftermath, we had, you know, we
27:08
had replicated a lot of the key
27:10
elements of most psychedelic studies and driven
27:12
a placebo effect that is enormous. In
27:14
some ways, this is really good news.
27:16
I mean, first of all, well, where
27:18
is this published, this paper? Nature Mental
27:20
Health. Okay. So, I think everyone should
27:23
take a look at the paper, but
27:25
it's a warning to say, look, look,
27:27
we really need to structure these psychedelic
27:29
studies, these psychedelic studies in a different.
27:31
Like for instance... Your mind can't change
27:33
your body. Yes. So we don't need
27:35
the drugs. We need something. These patients
27:37
went through something. I think that's a
27:39
key part of this. An experience though.
27:41
It was an experience. Yeah, but they
27:44
need this belief. If they don't have
27:46
this belief, so you have to tell
27:48
them that... This thing is magical, whether
27:50
it's a ceremony or dance or beauty
27:52
or molecule. But it's got to be,
27:54
you can't just tell them, you need
27:56
good placebo. We had really good placebo.
27:58
We had the best placebo. Tell me,
28:00
what about the, does the believe that
28:02
the patient, because you must have asked
28:04
the patient, did you think you got
28:07
it or did you think you were
28:09
in the placebo? How does that affect?
28:11
So, you know, they said we weren't
28:13
expecting to come up. So we didn't
28:15
ask people until the very end of
28:17
the study, which group did you think
28:19
you were in? I will say this
28:21
is like one of the only, maybe
28:23
the only, truly blinded study of a
28:25
psychedelic class drug, so that was a
28:28
small victory. But when we asked them
28:30
what they thought they got, so nobody
28:32
knew, first of all, but what in
28:34
talking to them, they're, you know, in
28:36
my conversations with these patients, if they
28:38
got better, they attributed it to the
28:40
ketamine group, because I feel better. which
28:42
suggests that they had some prior belief,
28:44
right? If you wouldn't say that you
28:46
got ketamine unless you believe the ketamine
28:49
is therapeutic. Surely there must have been
28:51
some people who believe it, or maybe
28:53
not, they will believe they were on
28:55
the placebo. And those are the ones,
28:57
yeah, because they didn't get better. They
28:59
didn't get better. And they're like, well,
29:01
I must have been in the placebo
29:03
group. So what that shows, if anything,
29:05
is that we did a job unwittingly
29:07
or not, a good job of instillingling
29:10
a sense of hope. that this has
29:12
the potential for therapeutic benefit. And I
29:14
think that's how you conclude in the
29:16
last paragraph of the paper. This is
29:18
called convention. Is it the short name
29:20
of business hope? Yes. So there is
29:22
a dangerous side to it, not dangerous,
29:24
but we can draw some of the
29:26
wrong conclusions from this work. One is
29:28
that, you know, the convention, placebo is
29:31
an old, an old word, and it
29:33
literally means like, I please. To please.
29:35
Right. One of the kind of awkward
29:37
things is if someone gets better after
29:39
getting placebo and then you tell them
29:41
they got placebo, it's more than a
29:43
little awkward. They're like, well, all that
29:45
stuff I said, well, the throes of
29:47
placebo like was that all like it
29:49
wasn't real right and so it's very
29:52
you know it's it people need to
29:54
feel seen need to feel heard and
29:56
you know, that that idea that placebo
29:58
is just something that you trick children
30:00
with, right? We have to dispense with
30:02
that idea. I think we need to
30:04
harness the placebo. I've always said it's
30:06
harness the placebo effect and use it
30:08
in medicine. Yes, exploit the placebo effect.
30:10
Well, and good doctors do it. Yes.
30:12
Real and in psychical therapy. That's why
30:15
you have Dr. Bowers, I said. Well,
30:17
because if I believe you just like
30:19
a shaman, if I believe you or
30:21
the shaman, then I'm more likely. That's
30:23
true. That has to be part of
30:25
it and there has to be a
30:27
strong experience at the center. I had
30:29
a bar mitzvah when I was 13.
30:31
My father said, today, my son, you're
30:33
a man. And I can tell you,
30:36
my voice did not drop, but I
30:38
felt different. People looked at me different.
30:40
They treated me differently differently. And that's
30:42
right. I mean, and how does that
30:44
happen? It's not a person in isolation.
30:46
It's certainly not a drug effect. It's
30:48
a door that you walk through. that
30:50
is held up by, you know, the
30:52
collective understanding of the community that surrounds
30:54
it. And that is a very devilishly
30:57
hard thing to study with, you know,
30:59
conventional scientific methods. A lot of it
31:01
is the power of suggestion, right? If
31:03
they come in and they're depressed and,
31:05
you know, they're low, I'm never going
31:07
to meet anybody, you know, you give
31:09
them the sort of hope, and they
31:11
believe you because you have all these
31:13
credentials and stuff, like rather than me
31:15
just giving my friends, you know, you
31:18
know, they take your... I really feel
31:20
like you're going to meet someone. I
31:22
really have, you know, I trust that.
31:24
And then they start to believe it
31:26
because someone in a sort of authority
31:28
position gives them that hope. It's not
31:30
about the SSRI, they're taking, but studies
31:32
do show in conjunction, you know, you
31:34
get some powerful effects from the SSRI,
31:36
some from therapy, when they work together,
31:39
you get the synergistic effect. But I
31:41
want to just talk about, because I
31:43
know there's other research you do in
31:45
terms of dreams you do in terms
31:47
of, So going with
31:49
this theme of
31:51
experience, in this study
31:53
that we're just
31:55
wrapping up, the experience,
31:57
they all went
31:59
through something. It's not
32:02
the experience on
32:04
drug, but it's the
32:06
larger experience of
32:08
being in the study
32:10
and going through
32:12
a door, right? Having
32:14
surgery and coming through the other
32:17
side. Was there any
32:19
therapy afterwards? No, like
32:21
every other catamines study, including
32:23
no therapy. And how long did
32:25
you? How long did you?
32:27
Two weeks. And so you
32:29
didn't talk to these people still? Not
32:31
formally. I mean, I kept up with some
32:33
of them and some of the stories,
32:35
I'd love to share some of them. It
32:37
really may be a question whether I
32:39
should be in science or not, when I
32:41
couldn't tell like, is this woman in
32:43
the ketamine group? Because how do you get
32:45
that kind of transformation without something to
32:47
account for it? Of course, she's in the
32:49
placebo group. So
32:52
there's another way to look at
32:54
this. said, coming back to this
32:56
theme of preparation and
32:58
the drug, the day of the dosing, the
33:00
drug and the trip and then integration and
33:02
really focusing on the drug and the trip, which
33:05
you can either take the trip out of the
33:07
drug and we've now talked about a couple
33:09
of ways to do that. What if you could
33:11
take the drug out of the trip? In
33:14
other words, recreate a psychedelic -like
33:16
experience without reference to a
33:18
psychedelic drug itself. That would
33:20
be a pretty interesting way
33:22
to test this idea of
33:24
how special is the serotonin
33:26
to a receptor, right?
33:29
And there's just some very
33:31
exciting research. But now we're talking about
33:33
how do you create hallucinations
33:35
without the psychedelic. So wouldn't
33:37
it be an interesting test if
33:40
we could induce a psychedelic -like
33:42
state without a psychedelic and get
33:44
some of the same physiology, descriptions
33:47
of experience and therapeutic effects? Would
33:49
that maybe turn on its head
33:51
the idea that there's a powder
33:53
at the center of all of
33:55
these therapeutic effects? And that's almost
33:58
by accident what about.
34:01
we've, what we've stumbled upon, working with
34:03
anesthesia and dreams. And I have a
34:05
colleague, Harrison Chow, he's a, I was
34:07
going to say an artist, he's an
34:10
anesthesiologist, but he's, you know, this is
34:12
the art of medicine. He's spent many
34:14
years in private practice, you know, minor
34:16
procedures. Patients would come in for, you
34:19
know, hernias or, you know, doscopies, minor,
34:21
minor stuff, and he would, you know,
34:23
he would try and make the experience
34:25
as pleasant as pleasant as possible. So
34:28
he would do this thing where he
34:30
would watch the EEG monitor and look
34:32
for what he thought was dreaming and
34:34
then patients would wake up and say
34:37
I had the best dream. I had
34:39
the best sleep I've ever had. But
34:41
as you emerge, you know, one of
34:43
the things that you can notice if
34:46
you really pay close attention to the
34:48
EEG, right? This is brain state monitoring.
34:50
So you're watching them as they calm
34:52
out of anesthesia and the wave starts
34:55
beating up and they're getting higher frequency.
34:57
you know, my chair put him together
34:59
with me because he knew I was
35:01
like a psychanot drug nerd into non-ordinary
35:04
states of consciousness and if you shouldn't
35:06
talk to each other and like maybe
35:08
figure something out. It was a great,
35:10
great partnership. Harrison is really like into
35:13
the idea of making anesthesia more pleasant.
35:15
I'm, you know, a little bit more
35:17
scientific and I was very skeptical at
35:19
first. So we agreed. We're going to
35:22
get a portable EEG. a sideline, the
35:24
same, you know, a kind of couple
35:26
leads. We have a team, you know,
35:28
includes a psychiatrist. We are going to
35:31
do, you know, interviews, diagnostic interviews, and
35:33
we're going to follow these patients. I
35:35
want to know is what you're saying.
35:37
Are you bullshitting me? Or is it
35:40
real? Is there something there? And not
35:42
long after we had our first case.
35:44
And this is a case of a
35:46
woman. This is published in 2022. Where
35:49
this woman, she... She had been attacked
35:51
at close quarters by a relative with
35:53
a knife, horrifying. As you might imagine,
35:55
she had nightmares, right? She went to
35:58
the emergency room and they said, go
36:00
to Stanford. get your hand fix and
36:02
you know in the intervening time. The
36:04
hand got injured. Yeah, exactly. So the
36:07
surgery was too helpful. The surgery was
36:09
for her hand and she you
36:11
know in the intervening two weeks
36:13
she's basically a non-functional human right
36:16
she's hypervigil and she's you know
36:18
having difficulty. So due to the
36:21
time frame we call it acute
36:23
stress disorder but this is somebody
36:25
who would more likely than not we
36:28
would be worried she would go on
36:30
to develop PTSD. So she's in the
36:32
pre-op area, right? She's just there for
36:34
her hands. You know, and Harrison finds
36:36
her, again, still, you know, it's all
36:38
she can talk about, and she's really,
36:41
you know, hard to reach. And they,
36:43
again, talking about like innovations and
36:45
anesthesia, they put her arm to
36:47
sleep. She gets a nerve box and
36:49
Harrison does a dream thing. Wait, so
36:52
is her arm anesthetized? Yeah, her arm
36:54
is anesthetized. So she doesn't need to
36:56
be so deeply anesthetized that she can't
36:59
feel it. When you say the stream thing,
37:01
it's just that you're controlling the amount
37:03
of profafal. And watching the EEG and
37:05
getting her into the sort of sweet
37:07
spot where he knows that the dreams
37:09
occur. Yeah. How long is this state of
37:12
dreaming in this patient? About 10 minutes.
37:14
Well, very agree. relatively brief,
37:16
but there's a lifetime in 10 minutes.
37:18
Yeah. So what she, you know, what
37:21
she says immediately upon waking is the
37:23
nightmare. It was it was there
37:25
again. I had the nightmare again and
37:27
it was looping just like it always
37:30
does, but instead of rocketing her into
37:32
consciousness, right, which what a
37:34
nightmare is, by virtue of this anesthetic
37:36
suppression. She stays in that state and
37:38
she actually in her dream moves past
37:40
the attack. She in her dream goes
37:42
to the emergency room, goes to the
37:44
operating room. She's back home running errands
37:46
with their hand. All in this 10
37:48
minutes. A lifetime in 10 minutes. Just
37:50
to go back for a second, just
37:52
because in terms of therapy, you know,
37:55
often like I'm working with traumatic with
37:57
patients who've had trauma and the idea
37:59
is to help. them, you know, work through
38:01
the emotions is to sit with
38:03
the thing that makes them anxious
38:05
or uncomfortable long enough. Yeah, exposure
38:07
long enough to get through it
38:09
and resolve it and then move
38:11
on. But often people, as soon
38:13
as they get, you know, the
38:15
cortisol comes or whatever, they want
38:17
to avoid it and they never
38:19
get through that. So maybe that
38:21
being in the stream state allowed
38:23
her to get past the anxiety
38:25
part of it enough so that
38:27
her brain was able to kind
38:29
of process it. We're thinking along
38:31
similar lines. So first of all,
38:34
we followed her for a day,
38:36
a week, a month, a year,
38:38
and what is still remarkable to
38:40
me is she's able to just
38:42
talk about this attack. No nightmares,
38:44
like, you know, she's basically, she's
38:46
functional. She's a functional person who
38:48
has recovered from trauma. You're seeing
38:50
you two 10-minute dream. Well, we've
38:52
done it now about 600 times.
38:54
And patients, so we've been, this
38:56
is clinical care, this is gentle
38:58
clinical care that now we've added,
39:00
you know, an observational study on
39:02
top of just seeing what happens
39:04
when we do this at scale.
39:06
And, you know, the technique isn't
39:08
perfect. We have, you know, somewhere
39:10
between 60 and 85% hit rate
39:12
for getting patients to have these
39:14
vivid dreams. And I can tell
39:16
you, you know, the things that
39:18
patients say upon awakening, you know,
39:20
this was more real than real.
39:22
I expected to be somewhere else.
39:24
Patients are having a very powerful
39:26
experience. It's more than just regular
39:28
dreaming. And do you find that
39:30
they're less anxious after or less?
39:32
Well, so what we actually, the
39:34
real clincher, or what was so
39:36
surprising is, you know, just by
39:38
chance, two patients that came through
39:40
Stanford operating rooms had bona fide
39:42
PTSD in both cases. Coincidentally, it
39:44
was the loss of a child,
39:46
an adult child, either due to
39:49
drug overdose or suicide. You know,
39:51
again, a horrible thing to live
39:53
with for years. And, you know,
39:55
as you can imagine, nightmares about
39:57
trying to save your child, like
39:59
it's really, we didn't know this
40:01
before they went to sleep. This
40:03
all came up in the immediate
40:05
aftermath, where putting... them into these
40:07
states for 10 to 15 minutes,
40:09
they emerge, one of our patients,
40:11
Mayor, and she's, there's a story
40:13
on the Stanford Med School blog
40:15
now, detailing her experience. She dreamt,
40:17
you know, she re-experienced the birth
40:19
of her son, instead of being
40:21
a traumatic birth, it was, it
40:23
was joyful. She was reunited with
40:25
her, with her son and with
40:27
her family, and they were, and
40:29
listening, we have a video also
40:31
on our family, on our family,
40:33
on our family, on our family,
40:35
on our family, on our, on
40:37
our, on our, on our, on
40:39
our... on my lab website, it
40:41
still gives me chills actually to
40:43
even listen to that she says
40:45
thank you for this, you know,
40:47
being able to have that experience.
40:49
And, you know, Mary, you know,
40:51
and Edie and the patients who
40:53
tell us these things have. invigorated
40:55
a small army of people in
40:57
the operating rooms who are now
40:59
like trying you know they want
41:01
to do this. But then it's
41:04
transformative after the fact then they're
41:06
less upset about these traumatic events.
41:08
She's had nightmares her entire life
41:10
and especially after this you know
41:12
this traumatic event and now you
41:14
know what she's telling us a
41:16
year later is that she has
41:18
not had nightmares. I mean what
41:20
this tells me is that you
41:22
know However you get to these
41:24
transformative experiences, whether you take MDMA
41:26
and that allows you to sit
41:28
with the trauma and work through
41:30
it and process it, in this
41:32
sort of drug-induced dream state, that
41:34
allows you to sit with the
41:36
trauma and process it, or the
41:38
psychedelic experiences. And even these intense
41:40
placebo experiences, there is something about
41:42
the psychological effect of sitting with
41:44
the anxiety and working through it.
41:46
And however you can, psychedelics are
41:48
a way to get there, but
41:50
they're not the only way. This
41:52
woman, it sounds wonderful, but it's
41:54
just one other three, you said
41:56
there's several hundred, or it's most
41:58
patients? So most patients, so again,
42:00
this is, we're doing this on
42:02
the background of providing clinical care.
42:04
So these patients... So they all
42:06
come in again for other types
42:08
of surgery. Yeah, they're coming in
42:10
for, you know, thyroid surgeries, plastic
42:12
surgery, you know. But they know,
42:14
they always get football before. But
42:16
the goal is always the same,
42:19
is during emergence, we are targeting
42:21
a state, right? We've had patients
42:23
report this with CIVA foreign, with
42:25
propafal, and Remy fentanyl. Again, this
42:27
hits at the point, it's not
42:29
the drug. So that being said,
42:31
in terms of what we're starting
42:33
to learn from the science and
42:35
from these experiments, is that it's
42:37
not necessarily the chemical molecule itself
42:39
that's having the impact on transforming
42:41
these. patients or people who have
42:43
anxiety and mood disorders. It's the
42:45
psychological experience that seems to be
42:47
the most impactful therapeutic here. And
42:49
trying to understand what are the
42:51
bounds, like how do we define
42:53
that, what are the characteristics that
42:55
need to be present in order
42:57
for it to be transformative? At
42:59
the end of every episode, we
43:01
ask a perception box question, and
43:03
our question today is... What aspects
43:05
of yourself are you now grateful
43:07
for that in the past you
43:09
struggled with or hated? I can
43:11
give, I can speak up. I
43:13
used to start as a little
43:15
child and I went to logotherapy
43:17
for like six weeks and I
43:19
was, you know, I just couldn't
43:21
do it particularly when people were
43:23
looking at me like in this
43:25
scenario. But then I sort of
43:27
challenged myself and did this on
43:29
purpose to get into a situation
43:31
where I needed to control my
43:34
language. And I don't know because
43:36
it was when I was nine
43:38
and 10 and 11, I did
43:40
the sort of mindfulness training, etc.
43:42
Then I managed to overcome that
43:44
and use that as a way
43:46
to learn how to give talks
43:48
and how to convince people. So
43:50
I think this has turned to
43:52
benefit. Might even overcompensated younger brothers,
43:54
10 years younger, but so for
43:56
10 years, I was an only
43:58
child. And I developed for, you
44:00
know, I was very, you know,
44:02
in my own world. And for
44:04
many years, I would kind of
44:06
hold ideas and I would not,
44:08
I didn't want to share them
44:10
until they were perfect and they
44:12
would marinate and stew and one
44:14
day, you know, during my PhD,
44:16
you know, during my PhD, something
44:18
clicked being in a, in a
44:20
lab and my mentor, Pablo Castillo
44:22
really nailed this for me. in
44:24
the arguing, the joy of, you
44:26
know, exposing yourself and your ideas.
44:28
It takes a certain amount of
44:30
courage or a jump. It's, you
44:32
know, these are things like my
44:34
ideas, my dreams, like these are
44:36
things that are really deeply special
44:38
to me that, you know, I
44:40
want to hold on to and
44:42
I want to kind of protect,
44:44
but what I found very quickly
44:46
is that by exposing them and
44:48
talking, you know, and really just...
44:51
pairing them apart in a way.
44:53
Some of them didn't survive. But,
44:55
you know, we have the saying
44:57
in my lab now that, you
44:59
know, if an idea can survive
45:01
this room, my office, after talking
45:03
about it for three hours, there
45:05
might be something there. That's the
45:07
power of discourse, which I think
45:09
we're losing now. A lot of
45:11
people are, you know, trigger warnings
45:13
and safe spaces and whatever, but
45:15
to actually be able to have,
45:17
you know... arguments and discourse. So
45:19
it's deeply enjoyable. I engage in
45:21
this community of people. Yeah, absolutely.
45:23
And it shifts your perspective and
45:25
your ideas. So my, I think,
45:27
I don't know if it's more
45:29
superficial or not, but the first
45:31
thing that came to my mind
45:33
is, you know, I used to,
45:35
you know, being an academic and
45:37
being a woman, I, a lot
45:39
of the time, and this was
45:41
like, you know, in the 90s
45:43
and the early 2000s, just wasn't
45:45
taken seriously if I was... too
45:47
feminine like wearing makeup dress whatever
45:49
so I used to like dislike
45:51
my femininity and dress down and
45:53
not wear makeup and wear baggy
45:55
clothes and whatever to be taken
45:57
seriously at places like Oxford and
45:59
Harvard and all the, and there
46:01
was this unconscious bias and stereotype,
46:03
and if I would walk in
46:06
to give like a lecture or
46:08
a keynote, immediately, expectations were lower,
46:10
if I was more, let's say,
46:12
feminized. And so I really tried to like
46:14
hide that aspect of myself, and
46:16
then I, as time went on, and I got
46:18
more confident, and I know what I know,
46:20
and I know the science, and I had
46:23
confidence, I didn't have to like
46:25
pretend anymore that I wasn't. feminine
46:27
and I could wear makeup and dress
46:29
the way I want and whatever and
46:32
still be taken seriously and actually take
46:34
advantage of those lower expectations because if
46:36
I come in like oh you're the keynote and
46:38
then they have lower experience rather someone
46:40
like crystal because with the German acts
46:42
whatever they're expecting to say something very
46:44
intelligent but he doesn't and it's a
46:47
big disappointment whereas I have this great
46:49
advantage where I they have lower expectations
46:51
and then I can you know show
46:53
what I know so I'm very grateful
46:55
now for that, but that was a part
46:57
of myself that I used to kind
46:59
of try to downplay. So yeah, that's
47:01
mine. It's a bit superficial, but I
47:04
think still meaningful. That runs pretty
47:06
deep. Yeah. Well, I want to thank
47:08
you, Forrest, for being here with us
47:10
today. Thank you. Thanks for having me.
47:12
This is a lot of fun. Fascinating
47:15
conversation. So if you'd like to
47:17
learn more about your own perception
47:19
box, spend some time this week
47:21
answering the same perception box questions
47:23
that we asked your guest, and
47:26
check out other questions on the
47:28
website at unlikely collaborators.com. You could
47:30
also subscribe to our YouTube channel
47:32
and watch the show or listen
47:34
wherever you get your podcast. This
47:37
has been Science of Perception Box
47:39
created by Unlikely collaborators in partnership
47:41
with pod people. I'm Dr. Heather
47:43
Berlin. Thank you very much.
Podchaser is the ultimate destination for podcast data, search, and discovery. Learn More