Episode Transcript
Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.
Use Ctrl + F to search
0:00
This message comes from Capital
0:02
One. Say hello to Stress-Free
0:05
subscription management. Easily track, block,
0:07
or cancel recurring charges right
0:09
from the Capital One mobile
0:12
app. Simple as that. Learn
0:14
more at capital1.com. This is the TED
0:16
Radio Hour. Each week, groundbreaking
0:19
TED Talks. Our job
0:21
now is to dream
0:24
big. Delivered at TED
0:26
conferences to bring about
0:28
the future. don't
0:39
know what you're going to find. Challenge
0:41
you. We truly have to ask ourselves,
0:43
like, why is it noteworthy? And
0:45
even change you. I literally feel
0:47
like I'm a different person. Yes.
0:49
Do you feel that way? Ideas
0:51
worth spreading. From Ted and
0:54
NPR. I'm Manouche
0:57
Zamorote. And
0:59
today on the show, Small
1:02
but Mighty. So, I
1:04
want you to touch your
1:07
face. Go on, touch it.
1:09
What do you feel?
1:11
Soft, squishy, maybe
1:14
chafed. It's you,
1:16
right? You're feeling
1:19
you? That's not
1:21
quite right. You're actually feeling
1:23
over a hundred billion bacterial
1:26
cells on your face. Those
1:28
that are creating some of
1:30
the aromas of body odor
1:32
and stickiness. You're feeling some
1:35
of fungi that have floated
1:37
down from the rafters or
1:39
sealing today. Those that set
1:42
off allergies. You're likely coated
1:44
with some... material cells that
1:46
came from the fecal plumes
1:48
that arose out of the
1:51
toilet the last time you
1:53
flushed it. Oh yeah, and there's at
1:55
least two species of face
1:57
mites that are squishing their
2:00
way across your skin. And
2:02
they definitely had sex on
2:04
your face last night. This
2:06
is Anne Madden. She's a
2:08
microbiologist and self-described micro-brangler who
2:11
does not seem to be
2:13
bothered by what she just
2:15
described. We are an ecosystem
2:17
and so just as we
2:19
expect to have lots of
2:22
different creatures living in our
2:24
homes and on our bodies.
2:26
And while that's gross, it's
2:28
also pretty cool. Anne hasn't
2:30
always been passionate about
2:32
the microscopic jungle that surrounds
2:34
us. It certainly wasn't on
2:37
her mind when she went
2:39
to college. But I had
2:41
this opportunity to do an
2:43
internship in the rainforest of
2:45
Costa Rica. And so when
2:47
I arrive in the jungle,
2:49
I see these... trees that
2:52
are taller than cathedrals. And
2:54
there are these howling monkeys
2:56
that sound like dying jaguars
2:58
and these beautifully, vibrantly colored,
3:00
poisoned dark frogs that kind of
3:02
sound like a baby duck everywhere.
3:04
And I am in love. And
3:07
as I get to help all
3:09
of these different scientists study
3:11
venomous snakes and poisoned art
3:13
frogs and plants. I realize
3:15
we know nothing about our
3:18
beautiful magnificent world and that
3:20
the process of research is
3:22
this puzzle, this adventure, and
3:25
it never ends. But it
3:27
did end, at least for
3:29
Anne. She had to go home
3:31
and back to school. And I
3:33
went back to Wellesley College in
3:35
Massachusetts and everything was beige and
3:38
boring and there were no poison
3:40
dark frogs and I was as
3:42
far away from the jungle as
3:44
I could imagine. But
3:47
then I started research in a
3:49
microbiology lab, and I found out
3:52
that I'd never left the jungle.
3:54
There were microscopic species everywhere, just
3:56
beyond the line of sight, and
3:59
these species... We barely uncovered what
4:01
they were. Maybe we know one
4:03
to 10% of what these species
4:05
are. And if you grow them
4:07
on a Petri plate, they erupt
4:09
in colors and aromas and very
4:11
strange little behaviors. And many of
4:13
them can do remarkable things and
4:15
have been doing remarkable things. And
4:17
yet they never get credit for
4:19
it. And so I think that's
4:21
when I fell in love with
4:23
microbiology. I remember the first time
4:25
I discovered and got to name
4:28
a new species. Ann Madden continues
4:30
from the TED stage. It was
4:32
a fungus that lives in the
4:34
nest of a paper wasp. It's white
4:36
and fluffy, and I named it
4:38
Mukor niticola, meaning in Latin that
4:41
it lives in the nest of
4:43
another. I called up my dad, and
4:45
I go, Dad, I just discovered a
4:47
new microorganism species.
4:49
And he laughs, and he goes,
4:51
that's great. I hope you also discovered
4:54
a cure for it. Now my dad
4:56
is my biggest fan. So in that crushing
4:58
a moment where he wanted to kill
5:01
my new little life form, I realized
5:03
that actually I'd failed him. In my
5:05
years toiling away in labs
5:07
and in people's backyards, investigating
5:10
and cataloging the microscopic life
5:12
around us, I'd never made clear my
5:14
true mission to him. My goal is not
5:16
to find technology to kill the new
5:18
microscopic life around us. My goal is
5:21
to find new technology from this life
5:23
that will help save us. The
5:26
diversity of life in our homes
5:28
is more than a list of 100,000
5:31
new species. It is 100,000
5:33
new sources of solutions to
5:35
human problems. I know
5:37
it's hard to believe that anything
5:39
that's so small or only has
5:42
one cell can do anything
5:44
powerful, but they can. Tiny
5:46
organisms with extraordinary potential.
5:48
A single word that
5:51
can pack a punch.
5:53
A passing moment. that
5:55
changes everything in a
5:57
world of attention-grabbing headlines
5:59
and... seismic global events,
6:01
we sometimes forget the little
6:04
guy, the almost imperceptible but
6:06
powerful forces around us. And
6:09
so today on the show,
6:11
small but mighty. Forget big
6:13
and boisterous. We are talking
6:16
about things that are minuscule
6:18
or fleeting, but potent.
6:21
Like microbes, which can both
6:23
hurt and help us humans.
6:25
We live in a time of
6:27
a horrid pandemic, so I think
6:29
we're all familiar with how powerful
6:32
and how devastating one microbe can
6:34
be, in this case, a virus.
6:36
But many microbes around us are
6:38
equally powerful, but they're not devastating.
6:41
So there are microbes in our
6:43
dust bunnies that are the source
6:45
of many of our antibiotics, and
6:48
they've been saving our loved ones
6:50
for as long as we've known.
6:52
One of the first jobs that
6:54
Ann Madden ever had was working
6:57
in research at a pharmaceutical company.
6:59
And our goal was to find
7:01
new microbes with the idea being
7:04
that these new microbes could create
7:06
novel antibiotics. And so that's where
7:09
I really learned my microbrangling skills.
7:11
We were using new techniques to
7:13
grow microbes from soil that had never
7:15
been grown before and no one had
7:18
ever explored them in the lab. And
7:20
indeed, these bizarre, slow-growing, tenacious little critters
7:22
were creating novel antibiotics. And though they
7:24
haven't been commercialized yet, we were successful
7:27
in finding them. You knew what you
7:29
were looking for then. I mean, it's
7:31
not like you're like, here's a new
7:33
microbe, let's see what it does. Like,
7:36
what are some of the clues that
7:38
you look for when you are hunting
7:40
down microbes that you think might have
7:42
medicinal uses? Yeah, so part of it
7:45
is the idea of guilty by
7:47
association. So other microbes that live
7:49
in our soil are the source
7:51
of many of our antibiotics that
7:53
we use today. You'll actually know
7:55
if they're in the soil because
7:57
they produce the smell of fresh
7:59
turned earth. And so these microbes,
8:01
you grow them in the lab
8:03
and they actually look like tiny
8:05
little sheep. They're adorable and fluffy.
8:07
And one strategy is to then
8:09
look for species of microbes that
8:12
are related, the idea being if
8:14
your cousins can do it, maybe
8:16
you can too. Do you give
8:18
them like bits of disease and
8:20
see how they respond or how
8:22
do you test it? Yeah. So
8:24
there are different ways of assessing
8:27
whether a microbe can produce novel
8:29
antibiotics. one of them is a
8:31
gladiator test. So you actually put
8:33
them in a Petri plate next
8:35
to a microbe that causes disease.
8:37
So say you've got a microbe
8:39
that's MRSA, right? So it's a
8:42
horrible staph infection. And you can
8:44
slather it on a Petri plate
8:46
and have it grow. And then
8:48
you come back the next day
8:50
and you look to see who
8:52
grew and who died. Oh, wow.
8:54
Okay. Battle to the finish. Yeah.
8:57
And so that gives you one
8:59
hint. That's not the end of
9:01
the end of the story by
9:03
any means, by any means, but
9:05
it. It gives you a clue
9:07
that, all right, this fluffy microbe
9:09
has something that's killing off a
9:12
microbe that causes disease. So maybe
9:14
we can isolate that something, extract
9:16
it, and maybe it can do
9:18
what no other antibiotics could do
9:20
before. Okay, Anne, you have also
9:22
been working on projects that are
9:24
more about the simple pleasures in
9:27
life. like a good beer, which
9:29
brings us to another microbe story
9:31
that involves wasps. Yeah, so as
9:33
soon as you start talking about
9:35
wasp research, people, my entire life,
9:37
always said, what's good about a
9:39
wasp? Nobody likes wasps. These are
9:42
stinging creatures that often create nests
9:44
in our eaves. But it turns
9:46
out that yeasts, microbes that we
9:48
love because they make bread and
9:50
beer and all sorts of lovely
9:52
flavors flavors. Use wasps as airplanes.
9:54
We started with a pest. Inside
9:57
that wasp, we clucked out a
9:59
little-known microorganism species with a unique
10:01
ability. It could make beer. This
10:03
is a trait that only a
10:05
few species on... this planet have.
10:07
In fact, all commercially produced beer
10:09
you've ever had likely came from
10:12
one of only three microorganism species.
10:14
Yet our species, it could make
10:16
a beer that tasted like honey.
10:18
And it could also make a
10:20
delightfully tart beer. In fact, this
10:22
microorganism species that lives in the
10:24
belly of a wasp, it could
10:27
make a valuable sour beer better
10:29
than any other species on this
10:31
planet. This yeast could do what
10:33
no other yeast species could do
10:35
before it, which is make a
10:37
sour beer in record time, just
10:39
a few weeks. And so, brewers
10:42
could make sour beer more economically,
10:44
and people could enjoy new flavors
10:46
and understand the benefits of biodiversity.
10:48
And with that weird little moment
10:50
in time, we gave a yeast
10:52
the ability to make beer for
10:54
the first time in 150 million
10:57
years of its existence. And so
10:59
now, just a few years later,
11:01
brewing with these lactic acid yeast
11:03
is now commonplace. And there's something
11:05
magical about that. I'm here to
11:07
tell you that the next 100
11:09
years will feature these microscopic creatures
11:12
solving more of our problems. And
11:14
we have a lot of problems
11:16
to choose from. We've got the
11:18
mundane, bad-smelling clothes or bland food.
11:20
And we've got the monumental, disease,
11:22
pollution, war. And so this is
11:24
my mission. to not just catalog
11:27
the microscopic life around us, but
11:29
to find out what it's uniquely
11:31
well suited to help us with.
11:33
These creatures are microscopic alchemists, with
11:35
the ability to transform their environment
11:37
with an arsenal of chemical tools.
11:39
This means that they can live
11:42
anywhere on this planet, and they
11:44
can eat whatever foods around them.
11:46
This means they can eat everything
11:48
from toxic waste to plastic. I
11:51
keep thinking about what you've mentioned
11:53
a couple times now, which is
11:55
plastic eating microbes. I mean, that
11:58
is, we always hear about, you
12:00
know, the... plastic that goes into
12:02
a landfill and is going to
12:05
be around for thousands and thousands
12:07
of years. Is there a possibility
12:09
that that actually maybe that might
12:12
not be true? That if we
12:14
could add microbes it would in
12:16
some fashion break down? Absolutely. So
12:18
scientists have already found microbes that
12:21
are incredibly adept at breaking down
12:23
plastics, even the plastics that seem
12:25
to last forever. That's really what
12:28
they were designed to do. And
12:30
as we think about our future,
12:32
we need to think about not
12:35
just the microbes that are going
12:37
to break down plastic, but those
12:39
that are going to help us
12:42
build materials that are more sustainable.
12:44
And so right now there are
12:46
also microbes that generate PLA or
12:48
polylactic acid, which is the plastic-like
12:51
filaments that a lot of people
12:53
are using in 3D printers. And
12:55
those are made by microbes. Are
12:58
they more biodegradable? They are. And
13:00
there are groups that are now
13:02
using AI and machine learning to
13:05
enhance the activity of those microbe-made
13:07
enzymes that tear apart plastic. And
13:09
so it's going to be microbial
13:12
skill sets and human ingenuity linked
13:14
together to create that better future.
13:16
When we return, the most famous
13:18
microbe of all? I'm Manouche Zamorote
13:21
and you're listening to the TED
14:10
This message comes from Charles Schwab. When
14:12
it comes to managing your wealth, Schwab
14:14
gives you more choices, like full service
14:17
wealth management and advice when you need
14:19
it. You can also invest on your
14:21
own and trade on Think or Swim.
14:23
Visit schwab.com to learn more. This
14:25
message comes from Better Help Online
14:28
Therapy. Relationships of all kinds are
14:30
complicated, and they tend to come
14:32
with a lot of tough questions.
14:35
From questions about co-workers, to family,
14:37
to romantic partners. Therapy can be
14:39
your safe space to talk about
14:42
challenges you face in any of
14:44
life's relationships. Better Help offers therapy
14:46
100% online, and sign up takes
14:49
only a few minutes. Visit betterhelp.com/NPR
14:51
to get 10 off your first
14:53
month. That's better. This
14:59
message comes from Schwab. At Schwab,
15:01
how you invest is your choice,
15:03
not theirs. That's why when it
15:05
comes to managing your wealth, Schwab
15:08
gives you more choices. You can
15:10
invest and trade on your own.
15:12
Plus, get advice and more comprehensive
15:15
wealth solutions to help meet your
15:17
unique needs. With award-winning service, low
15:19
costs and transparent advice, you can
15:22
manage your wealth your way at
15:24
Schwab. Visit schwab.com to learn more. This
15:26
message comes from Carvana. This
16:11
is the immense world that we
16:13
live in at the microbial scale. I
16:15
do feel like the pandemic
16:17
changed my relationship to microbes.
16:20
Maybe it made me a
16:22
little bit paranoid in many
16:24
ways. And not just the
16:27
idea that there is a
16:29
virus circulating among us. but
16:31
potentially many scary microbes around
16:34
us. How do you feel
16:36
like people's relationship with microbes
16:38
has been changed by COVID?
16:41
Yeah, so I think during
16:43
the pandemic, particularly at the
16:45
beginning, watched as the word
16:47
microbe brought chills to everyone.
16:49
And I saw it in young
16:52
children too, where it was a
16:54
lesson that was hard learned microbes
16:56
kill. They destroy. They bring with
16:59
them. sadness and isolation.
17:01
And I think as we
17:03
navigate our microbial world, it's
17:05
important to also teach children
17:07
and students that, again, not
17:10
all microbes are evil. And
17:12
so while it's important that
17:14
we use hand sanitizer and things
17:16
like that to limit the spread
17:18
of certain microbes, we need to
17:21
remember the plurality of these species
17:23
and that there are microbes that
17:25
we can play with in the
17:27
soil. and that that brings with
17:30
it health, not just kind of
17:32
griminess or germinous of
17:34
what we think of. So when it
17:36
comes to good microbes, do you
17:39
worry that, you know, just as
17:41
certain species are going extinct, are
17:43
certain microbes also in trouble? Have
17:46
we humans caused them harm? Oh,
17:48
so what keeps me up at
17:50
night sometimes is thinking about all
17:53
the microbes that have gone extinct.
17:55
Because we have no idea what
17:58
they are. We have no idea. who
18:00
they were or what they did.
18:02
And I don't just think about
18:05
that in terms of those that
18:07
are in our house that we
18:09
might have accidentally killed off with
18:11
some kind of bleaching agent, but
18:14
I think about that in the
18:16
world in terms of habitats and
18:18
as we navigate worlds where we're
18:21
clear-cutting forests, we don't... really know
18:23
all the microbes that we've lost.
18:25
And there very well might be
18:28
microbes out there that are going
18:30
to create the solutions to our
18:32
future problems. And so one of
18:35
the reasons that I know a
18:37
lot of microbiologists, such as myself,
18:39
love uncovering new species to science
18:42
is that part of that discovery
18:44
involves conservation. We are asked when
18:46
we name a species or other
18:49
required to put some of those
18:51
cells into a deep freeze in
18:53
multiple countries. So they are sitting
18:56
in bio-banks where they can be
18:58
revived in the future. And we
19:00
don't necessarily know what any of
19:03
those microbes are going to be
19:05
doing right now. A lot of
19:07
them sit just as specimens with
19:09
Latin names that no one can
19:12
pronounce. But that doesn't mean that
19:14
they'll always be that way. There
19:16
might be a future where they
19:19
are revived and those cells will
19:21
help. people combat a future pandemic.
19:23
Or maybe they'll help them grow
19:26
furniture in space stations, or something
19:28
far more mundane. But the key
19:30
is that they still exist. That's
19:33
microbiologist Anne Madden. She's the founder
19:35
and chief scientist at the Microbe
19:37
Institute. You can see her full
19:40
talk at TED. On
19:43
the show today, how the little
19:45
things in life can make a
19:48
big difference, like the words we
19:50
use to express ourselves every day.
19:52
Our next guest studies how small
19:54
variations in language may mean big
19:56
distinctions in how we experience the
19:59
life. world. And
20:01
to demonstrate let's play a little
20:03
game. Close your eyes. Yes
20:05
well so if you're driving don't
20:07
close your eyes but if
20:09
you're not driving or operating another
20:11
kind of motor vehicle, close
20:13
your eyes and point southeast. Southeast
20:17
okay. Yeah. I'm pointing okay.
20:19
So if you do this in
20:21
a any kind of normal
20:23
room full of westerners. I want
20:25
you all to close your
20:27
eyes and point southeast. Keep
20:31
your eyes closed point. And then
20:33
you have people open their eyes.
20:35
They will immediately see that they
20:37
have pointed in every possible direction.
20:39
I see you guys pointing there,
20:41
there, there, there, there. I don't
20:43
know which way it is myself. You
20:47
have not been a lot of help. And
20:50
I do this to point
20:53
out that there can be really
20:55
big cognitive differences between groups
20:57
of people. And I've had a
20:59
chance to work in an
21:01
Aboriginal community in Australia where
21:04
they could do this task very easily.
21:06
I could ask a five -year -old girl,
21:08
hey can you point southeast? And she
21:11
would point immediately and without hesitation. And
21:13
you know that's a big
21:16
difference compared to say a
21:18
room full of distinguished scientists.
21:20
So I'll point in different
21:22
directions. This is cognitive scientist,
21:24
Lara Boroditsky. And I study
21:26
how humans get so smart,
21:28
how the languages and cultures
21:30
that we have help us
21:32
think the way that we do.
21:37
So Lara, it may not seem
21:39
obvious to some of us,
21:41
but what do you think
21:43
this exercise of pointing southeast
21:45
says about how language influences
21:47
the brain? Well, our
21:50
languages and cultures teach us
21:52
to pay attention to certain things
21:54
and not to other things.
21:56
There's basically an infinite set of
21:58
things that we could possibly... take
22:00
in process, but our brains can't
22:02
process all the information, can't take
22:04
in all the information, so we
22:06
have to make some choices. And
22:08
one of the ways that we
22:10
make those choices are by the
22:12
things that our languages and cultures
22:14
require of us. So in this
22:16
aboriginal community in Australia that I
22:18
mentioned, they, instead of using words
22:20
like left and right to give
22:22
directions or to talk about the
22:24
body, they use cardinal directions, more
22:26
or less north-south, east and west.
22:29
And they use these directions at
22:31
all scales. So for example, in
22:33
some languages like this, like in
22:35
Google Umatier, you would even say,
22:37
there's an ant on your southwest leg.
22:40
You would say, you know, move
22:42
your cup to the north-northeast a
22:44
little bit. In Kuktaya, this language
22:46
that I had a chance to work
22:48
on, the way you even say hello
22:51
is, which way are you going? And
22:53
the answer should be something like north-northwest
22:55
in the far distance. How about
22:57
you? Imagine as you're walking around
23:00
your day, every person you greet,
23:02
you have to report your heading
23:04
direction. Here's Lara Borodinsky on the
23:06
TED stage. That would actually get you
23:08
oriented pretty fast, right? Because you
23:10
literally couldn't get past hello, if
23:12
you didn't know which way you're
23:14
going. In fact, people who speak languages
23:16
like this stay oriented really, really
23:19
well. They stay oriented better than
23:21
we used to think humans could.
23:23
We used to think that humans
23:25
were worse than other creatures, ...
23:27
Some biological excuse, oh, we don't
23:30
have magnets in our beaks or
23:32
in our scales. No. If your
23:34
language and your culture trains you
23:36
to do it, actually, you can do
23:38
it. So for me, whenever I
23:40
come across an example like this,
23:42
the biggest lesson for me is
23:45
to not underestimate the potential of
23:47
the human mind, not just to
23:49
say that the things that I
23:51
can do or the things that
23:53
I can imagine, those are the
23:56
limits. Language allows us to recombine
23:58
elements in infinite new ways. and
24:00
create new ideas on the
24:02
spot. So right now I
24:04
could say, imagine a giraffe
24:06
river dancing at a pancake
24:09
while solving differential equations, right?
24:11
You've never had that thought
24:13
before? No, no, I'm glad
24:15
happy to say. Maybe it's
24:17
not the most useful thought,
24:20
but there's so many other
24:22
thoughts like that that people
24:24
throughout history have had, and
24:26
all of a sudden we
24:28
have. interesting ideas about time
24:30
travel that make us engage
24:33
with the future in different
24:35
ways. So this ability to
24:37
think beyond what is physically
24:39
present and imagine and work
24:41
in the realm of the
24:44
abstract is one of the
24:46
things that language opens the
24:48
door to. You
24:50
are really the latest in
24:52
a long, centuries-long line of
24:54
people who have been asking
24:56
this question. Does language shape
24:58
the way we think? Why
25:00
is this been such a
25:02
debated topic? Because when I,
25:04
you know, I remember the
25:06
first time thinking about it,
25:08
I just assumed it did,
25:10
but actually... This is not
25:12
something that anyone agrees on
25:14
how it works. Well, I
25:16
think often we disagree with
25:19
ourselves about it, right? I
25:21
think all of us have
25:23
both intuitions. And so the
25:25
idea that language shapes thought
25:27
is very similar to the
25:29
idea that physical exercises changes
25:31
the way that your body
25:33
looks and acts. Right. When
25:35
you speak a language, you're
25:37
practicing paying attention, you're practicing
25:39
categorizing something every day constantly.
25:41
And so it would be,
25:43
in fact, the most surprising
25:45
thing that the thing that
25:47
you do all day every
25:49
day, this practice of speaking
25:51
language, would have no influence
25:53
on your brain. And for
25:55
me as a scientist, what's
25:57
interesting is to... figure out
25:59
what are the times that
26:01
language shapes thought meaningfully, and
26:03
what are the times that
26:05
it doesn't. And so I think
26:07
one of the reasons that this idea
26:10
has gotten new life in the last 20
26:12
or 30 years is because we
26:14
started using experimental methods
26:16
like real scientific experimental
26:18
methods rather than just
26:21
arguing back and forth
26:23
about our intuitions. One
26:25
of your critics, John McWater,
26:27
the linguist, he said, well,
26:29
the gradual consensus is becoming
26:31
that language can shape thought,
26:34
but it tends to be
26:36
in rather darling obscure psychological
26:38
flutters. It's not a matter
26:40
of giving you a different
26:42
pair of glasses on the world.
26:45
Is this a small discovery that you're
26:47
making or would you beg
26:49
to differ? Obviously I would beg
26:51
to differ because there are many
26:53
different ways you could ask how
26:55
deep, big, important differences
26:58
are. And there are
27:00
studies that reveal really
27:02
big, interesting differences of
27:04
different kinds. So for
27:06
example, if we look at
27:08
color perception, different languages have
27:11
different words for colors, put
27:13
color boundaries in different places,
27:16
we can find that. language
27:18
influences even these tiny perceptual
27:21
decisions that are so early
27:23
on and so kind of stupid,
27:25
you know, like a pigeon could
27:27
make these decisions and yet somehow
27:29
even in these smart human brains
27:32
language is making a difference in
27:34
how you tell the difference between
27:36
two patches of blue for example.
27:39
Now that tells us that language
27:41
can have an extremely early influence
27:43
in cognition and if it can
27:46
influence something very early that means
27:48
it's influencing all of the other
27:50
things downstream. Language also differ in
27:53
how they describe events, right? So you
27:55
take an event like this, an accident,
27:57
in English it's fine to say Hebrew...
27:59
the vase. In a language like
28:02
Spanish, you might be more likely
28:04
to say the vase broke or
28:06
the vase broke itself. If it's
28:09
an accident, you wouldn't say that
28:11
someone did it. Now this has
28:13
consequences. So we show the same
28:15
accident to English speakers and Spanish
28:18
speakers. English speakers will remember who
28:20
did it. because English requires you
28:22
to say, he did it, he
28:25
broke the base. Where Spanish speakers
28:27
might be less likely to remember
28:29
who did it if it's an
28:31
accident, but they're more likely to
28:34
remember that it was an accident.
28:36
They're more likely to remember the
28:38
intention. So two people watch the
28:41
same event, witness the same crime,
28:43
but end up remembering different things
28:45
about that event. This is implications,
28:47
of course, for eyewitness testimony. The
28:50
language guides are reasoning about events.
28:53
So another example that's coming
28:55
to mind for me, and
28:57
tell me if this fits,
28:59
it's the use of gendered
29:01
pronouns in the US. So
29:03
one of my daughter's friends
29:05
uses non-binary pronouns, they them.
29:07
And my daughter and I
29:09
were talking to my mother
29:11
about this friend, and my
29:13
mother was getting so confused.
29:15
Every time we referred to
29:17
them, because she thought we
29:19
were referring to multiple people,
29:21
and it made me think,
29:23
like, does this come down
29:25
to a generational thing? The
29:27
way we change the way
29:29
that we use language, the
29:31
way we think about the
29:33
gendered sex of someone, that
29:35
is changing. Does this example
29:37
fit into what you're talking
29:39
about? Absolutely. Yeah, it's a
29:41
wonderful example. And it's another
29:43
wonderful example of how... Whatever
29:45
it is that we're used
29:47
to seems to be the
29:49
way that things naturally should
29:51
be. So for example, in
29:53
English, we mark gender on
29:55
third-person singular pronouns. So he,
29:57
she, his, her. We don't
29:59
mark... gender on those pronouns
30:01
causes some people incredible pain.
30:03
And they will argue on
30:05
second person pronouns. We only
30:07
have you. So in fact,
30:09
most of English pronouns are
30:11
gender neutral. It's just in
30:13
a third person singular that
30:15
we mark gender, but proposing
30:17
that there could be a
30:19
new pronoun or proposing that
30:21
we could not mark gender
30:23
on those pronouns causes some
30:25
people incredible pain. And they
30:27
will argue nothing will be
30:29
understandable if we don't have
30:31
gender. But there are languages
30:33
that mark gender on first-person
30:35
pronouns or at second-person pronouns
30:37
or on plural pronouns. Some
30:39
languages don't mark gender at
30:41
all, right? Like, take a
30:43
finish as an example. There
30:45
are no gendered third-person pronouns
30:47
in Finnish. And in experiments
30:49
looking at, for example, Hebrew
30:51
learning kids, English learning kids,
30:53
and Finnish learning kids. It
30:55
turns out that kids... learning
30:57
Hebrew is their first language,
30:59
figure out whether they themselves
31:01
are a boy or a
31:03
girl earlier. In Hebrew, even
31:05
the second person pronoun is
31:08
gendered, so the word for
31:10
you is gendered. English is
31:12
somewhere in between, and then
31:14
Finnish kids take about a
31:16
year, an extra year before
31:18
they can reliably classify themselves
31:20
with boys or girls. And
31:22
so that's an indication of
31:24
language forcing you to pay
31:26
attention to some dimension that
31:28
you may want to think
31:30
about it in a different
31:32
way. And so what may
31:34
seem like a very small
31:36
difference to one person might
31:38
seem a really big deal
31:40
to someone else. Of course,
31:42
if it applies to you,
31:44
it's going to be a
31:46
lot more important, and you
31:48
may feel like the language
31:50
that you're being forced to
31:52
speak is constantly forcing you
31:54
into one category or another
31:56
that doesn't fit. That has
31:58
always been the way language
32:00
change comes about. People feel
32:02
like the current language that
32:04
they're speaking doesn't fit
32:06
their thinking, doesn't fit the way
32:08
that they want to be in the
32:10
world, and so they start trying
32:12
to change the language and
32:15
inviting other people to also think
32:17
in this new way. And thinking in
32:19
new ways is painful. So what would
32:21
you say the goal then as of
32:23
your research? I mean, why should someone
32:25
just listening to the show know
32:27
this? What do you think it... does
32:30
for them? Oh, well, I think
32:32
whenever you're looking at another culture,
32:34
another language, the most important thing
32:36
you could learn is about yourself
32:38
in your own language, in your
32:41
own culture, right? So take the
32:43
mirror and turn it on yourself
32:45
and say, why do I think
32:47
the way that I do? Why
32:49
would I be surprised that someone
32:52
thinks differently? Like I have been
32:54
practicing speaking in this way, thinking
32:56
in this way my whole life.
32:58
And often we assume that whatever
33:00
it is that we're used to
33:02
is the way that things have
33:04
to be. But actually we have
33:07
many more options. And so for
33:09
me it is always an invitation
33:11
to one examine the assumptions that
33:13
I have and why is it
33:15
that I think the way that
33:17
I do? How could I think
33:19
differently? How do I want to
33:21
think? And then you can learn
33:23
a lot and you can expand
33:25
your own thinking. That's
33:29
Lara Boroditzki. She's a
33:31
professor of cognitive science
33:33
at the University of
33:35
California, San Diego. You can
33:37
see her full talk at
33:39
ted.com. On the show today,
33:41
small but mighty. I'm Anusha
33:43
Zamorote, and you're listening to
33:45
the TED Radio Hour from
33:48
NPR. We'll be right back. This
34:28
and the following message come from
34:30
Bolan Branch. Change your sleep with
34:32
Bolan Branch's airy blankets, cloud-like duvees,
34:35
and breathable sheets. Feel the difference
34:37
with 15% off your first order
34:40
at Bolan branch.com with code NPR.
34:42
Exclusions apply, C-site for details. Were
37:22
you like I don't even need
37:24
to think about Sam I can
37:26
concentrate on the other kids because
37:29
he's got this he's he's gonna
37:31
be just fine? Strangely, I think
37:33
the opposite happened. You know other
37:36
kids who were very vocal about
37:38
not being able to do something
37:40
it's very easy to catch and
37:43
help them but for a kid
37:45
like Sam I didn't want to
37:47
miss those little subtle signs. Yayun
37:50
was always trying to pick up
37:52
on those signs, those moments, when
37:54
a child needed help, like if
37:56
they fell down. Yes, when a
37:59
kid would fall down, they would...
38:01
didn't start crying immediately. There was
38:03
a few seconds of buffering that
38:06
was happening where the kid would
38:08
stand up kind of confused as
38:10
to what just happened before he
38:13
or she would have an emotional
38:15
reaction to it. And most of
38:17
the time it wasn't because they
38:20
were injured, it was more because
38:22
of the shock and that kind
38:24
of sense of reassurance that they
38:27
could get from the teacher. or
38:29
please console me that kind of
38:31
of a feeling almost. And as
38:34
a new teacher, Yea Yun really
38:36
wanted to be that person to
38:38
reassure them. She was just waiting
38:41
to share one of those moments
38:43
with a student. Yes, I kind
38:45
of felt that, but when I
38:47
saw that happening over and over
38:50
to other teachers but never happening
38:52
to me, that kind of became
38:54
something that I really wanted for
38:57
me to achieve in a way.
38:59
And then one day I heard
39:01
one of the students calling me
39:04
saying, teacher Sam fell down. So
39:06
I rushed to the indoor playground.
39:08
There Sam was looking, you know,
39:11
puzzled and kind of confused. And
39:13
he turned around and looked at
39:15
me and his lip started trembling
39:18
and just burst up. crying. So
39:20
I rushed to him and gave
39:22
him a hug and I said,
39:25
oh, Sam, it's okay. What happened?
39:27
Does it hurt? And it lasted
39:29
only a few seconds, but that
39:32
moment kind of imparted in me
39:34
as that feeling of, wow, this
39:36
connection finally happened. You felt needed.
39:39
Yes, I felt needed. I think
39:41
the fact that it was also
39:43
Sam made it a bit more
39:45
special. After
39:51
that year, Yayoon moved on
39:53
from teaching, and she didn't
39:55
really think about Sam that
39:57
much. Eventually, she landed a
39:59
job managing volunteer. for a
40:01
non-profit and she loved it.
40:03
I found the work very
40:05
enjoyable and it was so
40:07
much fun and I think
40:09
that was the beginning of
40:11
me justifying to myself that
40:13
hey you know 14-16-hour days
40:15
are fine as long as
40:17
I'm having fun and if
40:19
you're actually having fun is
40:21
it work. So I started
40:23
losing time that would help
40:25
me be me outside of
40:27
work. Kind of like Sam,
40:29
Yayoon did not want to
40:31
ask for help. Instead, after
40:33
a long day, she would
40:35
blow off steam by going
40:37
out. Exactly. And that's how
40:39
it kind of started. At
40:41
the end of a very
40:43
long workday, look for other
40:45
friends who have had long
40:47
days and have a drink,
40:49
and that would lead to
40:51
a few more drinks and
40:53
a few more drinks. Eventually
40:55
got to a point where
40:57
I didn't know what to
40:59
do with myself if I
41:01
wasn't working or if I
41:03
wasn't out drinking with friends.
41:05
I really thought that I
41:07
was doing it all. I
41:10
was kicking butt at work.
41:12
I was the entertainer in
41:14
my friend groups. Even if
41:16
there was a big work
41:18
day tomorrow, I would go
41:20
and still be able to
41:22
perform well at work. At
41:24
that time, I really didn't
41:26
see it as a problem.
41:28
I should have. Now I
41:30
know that I was a
41:32
very high-functioning alcoholic. A sign
41:34
that should have told me
41:36
you should seek help was
41:38
that I would often have
41:40
blackouts. One morning after one
41:42
of her blackouts, Yayune woke
41:44
up with a big cut
41:46
on her foot, and no
41:48
idea how it happened how
41:50
it happened. She could only
41:52
remember the previous night in
41:54
brief flashes. The flashes that
41:56
I did remember were horrifying.
41:58
I was extremely frustrated. and
42:00
afraid almost in
42:03
a state of
42:05
paranoia and crying
42:07
for help. So
42:10
did you have a
42:12
person in your world
42:15
then like Sam
42:17
found in you
42:19
this person who
42:22
you could be
42:24
vulnerable with he could
42:26
ask for help? When
42:29
that moment happened, I couldn't see
42:31
the help. I had friends who
42:33
were around me. We had gone
42:35
to dinner, but I was still
42:38
in that buffering phase, like Sam.
42:40
I was just kind of standing
42:42
up for what had happened, and
42:45
I think I was trying to
42:47
figure out how to get myself
42:49
out of this mess. So when
42:51
my friend noticed that I
42:54
was... not really participating in
42:56
conversation or answering questions. He
42:58
removed me from the place.
43:00
We went outside of the
43:02
restaurant. That's where he had
43:04
to shake me, shake me
43:06
and said, can you do
43:08
this? And I had to
43:10
say, no, I can't. Then
43:12
can I please? help you
43:14
and can I please get
43:16
the other people around you
43:18
to help you as well.
43:21
And when I was finally able
43:23
to say, yes, I do need
43:25
the help, it really felt like
43:27
almost like an out of body
43:30
experience where I could see
43:32
myself being bubble wrapped by
43:34
all the people around me
43:36
and it just felt so
43:38
light. There were so many
43:40
people ready and already helping
43:42
me that I just didn't
43:44
notice beforehand. I think
43:47
for some of us who see ourselves
43:49
as very independent, and
43:51
you'll notice I'm including
43:53
myself in this question,
43:55
admitting vulnerability feels weak.
43:57
It does. Did that
43:59
change? for you? Yes, I
44:01
wasn't really sure if I
44:03
wanted to so openly talk
44:06
about this because it was
44:08
really me admitting to people
44:10
that I work with on
44:12
a daily basis to say
44:14
I have struggled with alcohol
44:17
abuse and have burned out.
44:19
So I really didn't know
44:21
how it would come across.
44:23
I didn't want this to
44:25
be a way for other
44:28
people to think of me
44:30
differently. After sharing the story,
44:32
I'm kind of relieved that
44:34
people don't look at me
44:36
the same way that they
44:39
did before. I'm not just
44:41
this bubbly, you know, an
44:43
independent person that has everything
44:45
together. Yes, I am all
44:47
of that, but also I'm
44:50
deeply flawed and I'm struggling.
44:52
So when I felt like,
44:54
oh, yes, I do need
44:56
to get professional help. I
44:58
sought out a therapist and
45:01
after sharing the story... So
45:03
many people were coming to
45:05
me asking for advice on
45:07
how did I look out
45:10
for my therapist? How is
45:12
therapy? That really made me
45:14
feel like, oh, I did
45:16
the right thing of being
45:18
a little courageous to share
45:21
this story because so many
45:23
people came asking me for
45:25
help afterwards. I mean, that's
45:27
huge. Yeah. Especially from the
45:29
Asian context where... Going to
45:32
therapy is still very taboo.
45:34
It was really encouraging to
45:36
see people be more open
45:38
about it with me. To
45:40
me, this tiny little moment
45:43
of you and Sam locking
45:45
eyes. It's like a tiny
45:47
little push of a domino.
45:49
It sets off this cascade.
45:51
And then to see the
45:54
tables turned as an adult.
45:56
It sounds like this moment
45:58
of accepting the offer of
46:00
help from your friend was
46:02
also a huge turning point
46:05
for you. Yes. I would
46:07
imagine that if someone comes
46:09
to you for help and
46:11
looks to you and cries
46:13
and looks very hopeless, that
46:16
somehow you are the person
46:18
in the position with the
46:20
power. But very weirdly in
46:22
that moment, when Sam looked
46:24
at me, it didn't feel
46:27
that way. But rather... This
46:29
is such a privilege. He
46:31
had something so powerful and
46:33
he was willing to give
46:35
it to me by asking
46:38
me to be the one
46:40
to help him. It's really
46:42
just a four-letter word, help,
46:44
but when you say it,
46:46
it's powerful. That was yayoon
46:49
Kim. You can watch her
46:51
talk at ted.com. On the
46:53
show today, small... but mighty.
46:55
And we started off the
46:57
episode with the tiniest organisms,
47:00
microbes, and now we want
47:02
to end with another critter,
47:04
rats. Over the past 25
47:06
years, rats have been crucial
47:08
to helping sniff outland mines
47:11
in former war zones. But
47:13
where on earth did the
47:15
idea come to recruit lowly
47:17
rodents to do this life-saving
47:19
work? industrial engineer Bart Wittians
47:22
explained on the TED stage
47:24
in 2010. I'm here today
47:26
to share with you an
47:28
extraordinary journey, extraordinary rewarding journey
47:31
actually, which brought me into
47:33
training rats to save human
47:35
lives by detecting landmines. As
47:37
a child I had a
47:39
passion for rodents. I had
47:42
a passion for rodents. I
47:44
had all kinds of rats,
47:46
mice, hamsters, you name it,
47:48
I bred it and I
47:50
sold them to pet shops.
47:53
I became an industrial engineer,
47:55
engineering product development, and I
47:57
focused on appropriate detection technologies.
47:59
Actually, the first... appropriate technologies
48:01
for developing countries.
48:03
I started working in the
48:06
industry, but I wasn't really
48:08
happy to contribute to a
48:10
material consumer society. I
48:12
quit my job to focus on a real
48:14
world problem, landmines. Two thousand people,
48:16
every month, are killed or
48:19
made by mines around the
48:21
world. We're talking 95 now.
48:23
Princess Diana is announcing on
48:25
TV that landmines form a
48:27
structural barrier. to any development.
48:30
It is my sincere hope that
48:32
by working together we shall focus
48:34
world attention on this vital but
48:36
until now largely neglected issue. As
48:39
long as these devices are there
48:41
or there is suspicion of land
48:43
mines you can't really enter into
48:45
the land. Actually there was an
48:47
appeal worldwide for new detectors
48:50
sustainable in the environment
48:52
where they needed to produce
48:54
which is mainly in the developing
48:56
world. We chose rats. Now why
48:58
would you use rats? Rats
49:00
have been used since the
49:02
50s last century in all
49:05
kinds of experiments. Rats
49:07
have more genetic material
49:09
allocated to all faction
49:12
than any other mammal
49:14
species. They're extremely sensitive
49:17
to smell. Moreover, they
49:19
have the mechanisms to map
49:21
all these smells and to
49:24
communicate about it. Now how do
49:26
we communicate with rats? Well,
49:28
we don't talk rats, but
49:30
we have a clicker, a
49:32
standard method for animal training,
49:34
with which we can reinforce
49:36
particular behaviors. First of all,
49:38
we associate a clicksant with a
49:40
food reward, which is mashed banana
49:43
and peanuts together in a syringe.
49:45
Once the animal knows click food, click
49:47
food, so click is food, we bring
49:49
it in a cage with a hole,
49:51
and actually the animal learns... to stick the
49:54
nose in the hole under which the target
49:56
sent is placed and to do that for
49:58
five seconds, which is long. for a rat.
50:00
Once the animal knows this we make the
50:03
task a bit more difficult. It
50:05
learns now to find the target smell
50:07
in a cage with several holes, up
50:09
to 10 holes. Then the animal learns
50:11
to walk on a leash in the
50:13
open and find targets. In the next
50:15
step animals learn to find real
50:18
minds in real mine fields. They
50:20
are tested and accredited according to
50:22
international mine action standards just like
50:25
dogs. have to pass a test.
50:27
There's a number of minds placed
50:29
blindly, and a team of trainers
50:32
and their rats have to find
50:34
back all the targets. If the
50:36
animal does that, it gets
50:39
a license as an accredited
50:41
animal to be operational in
50:43
the field. Just like dogs, by
50:45
the way. Maybe one slight difference.
50:47
We can train rats at a
50:49
fifth of the price of a
50:51
trained demining dog. This is our
50:53
team in Mozambique. They have a skill,
50:56
which makes them much less dependent
50:58
on foreign aid. With this
51:00
small investment in a rat
51:02
capacity, we have demonstrated in
51:04
Mozambique that we can reduce
51:06
the cost price per square meter. If
51:08
we can bring in more rats, we
51:10
can actually make the output even
51:12
bigger. We have a demonstration
51:15
site in Mozambique. Eleven African
51:17
governments have seen that they
51:20
can become less dependent by using
51:22
this technology. They have signed a
51:24
pact for peace and treaty in
51:26
the Great Lakes region, and they
51:28
endorse hero rats to clear their
51:30
common borders of landmines. To conclude,
51:33
I would actually like to
51:35
say, you may think this is
51:37
about rats, this project, but in
51:39
the entity is about people. It
51:42
is about empowering vulnerable communities to
51:44
tackle difficult, expensive and
51:46
dangerous humanitarian detection tasks.
51:48
And doing that with
51:50
a local resource, plenty
51:52
of available. So something
51:54
completely different is to
51:56
keep on challenging your
51:58
perception about about the
52:01
resources surrounding you, whether
52:03
they are environmental, technological,
52:06
animal or human. And
52:09
to respectfully harmonize with
52:11
them in order to
52:14
foster a sustainable world.
52:16
Thank you very much.
52:22
That was Bart Witians. He
52:24
is an industrial engineer and
52:26
he founded the organization Apopo
52:29
25 years ago, which is
52:31
still clearing landmines with help
52:33
from rats in Mozambique, Angola,
52:36
Zimbabwe, and Cambodia. And they
52:38
are also training rats to
52:40
detect tuberculosis. You can find
52:43
Bart's full talk at ted.com.
52:48
Thank you so much for
52:51
listening to our show today.
52:53
Small but mighty. This episode
52:55
was produced by James Delahousie,
52:57
Katie Montelian, Fiona Guiren, and
53:00
Susanna Brown. It was edited
53:02
by Sanez-Meshkinpore, Andrea Gutierrez, and
53:04
me. Our production staff at
53:07
NPR also includes Rachel Faulkner-white,
53:09
Matthew Cloutier, and Hersha-Nahata. Our
53:11
fellow is Malvica-Dang. Our theme
53:14
music was written by Romteen
53:16
Arab-era-era-era Bluei. Josh Newell and
53:18
Joby Tenseco. Our partners at
53:20
TED are Chris Anderson, Colin
53:23
Helms, Anna Phelan, Michelle Quint,
53:25
Jimmy Gutierrez, and Daniela Balaorezzo.
53:27
I'm Anish Zamorote and you've
53:30
been listening to the TED
53:32
Radio Hour from NPR. This
53:34
message This
53:36
message comes from MintMobile. MintMobile took
53:39
what's wrong with wireless and made
53:41
it right. They offer premium wireless
53:43
plans for less and all plans
53:45
include high-speed data, unlimited talk and
53:48
text, and nationwide coverage. See for
53:50
yourself at mintmobile.com/SWIT. This
54:20
This message comes from Capella
54:22
University. With Capella's Flexpath Learning you
54:24
can set your own deadlines
54:26
and learn on your schedule. A
54:28
A different future is closer
54:30
than you think with Capella
54:32
University. Learn more more. EDU.
Podchaser is the ultimate destination for podcast data, search, and discovery. Learn More