Episode Transcript
Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.
Use Ctrl + F to search
0:00
After years of fine print contracts
0:02
and getting ripped off by overpriced
0:04
wireless providers, if we've learned anything,
0:06
it's that there's always a catch. So
0:08
when I first heard about Mint Mobile offering
0:10
wireless plans for just $15 a month with
0:12
a three-month plan, I thought there had to
0:15
be a catch. But after looking into it,
0:17
it all makes sense. There isn't one. Mint
0:19
Mobile Secret Sauce is brilliantly
0:21
simple. Instead of paying for
0:23
fancy stores and sells people,
0:25
they're focusing on what matters.
0:27
Delivering premium wireless service directly
0:29
to you. We're talking high-speed
0:31
data, unlimited talk and text,
0:33
all on the nation's largest
0:35
5G network. Plus, you can keep
0:38
your current phone. and number. No
0:40
hassle, no headaches. Say bye-bye to
0:42
your overpriced wireless plans, jaw-dropping monthly
0:45
bills, and unexpected overages. Mint Mobile
0:47
is here to rescue you with
0:49
premium wireless plans starting at just
0:52
15 bucks a month. Want to
0:54
stop throwing money away in overpriced
0:56
wireless? Here's what you need to do.
0:59
Go to mintmobile.com/charm.
1:01
Get premium wireless for just
1:03
15 bucks a month at
1:05
Mint mobil.com/charm. $45 upfront payment
1:07
required, equivalent to $15 a
1:10
month. New customers on a
1:12
first three-month plan only. Speed slower
1:14
above 40 gigs on an unlimited
1:16
plan. Additional taxes fees and
1:19
restrictions apply. See mint
1:21
mobile for details. Scores on
1:23
standard tests of creativity have
1:25
been going down since about
1:27
the late 1980s, early 1990s,
1:30
and going down even more
1:32
sharply in the last 10
1:34
years or so. If you're
1:36
just passively... being fed whatever
1:38
the algorithm is giving you
1:41
and not making
1:43
any active choices.
1:45
You're not learning
1:48
in the same way.
1:50
And you're not even
1:53
learning how to learn.
1:55
You're not learning how
1:58
to explore. have the
2:00
cheat code to succeed with people. Every
2:02
episode is jam-packed with actionable steps to
2:05
unlock the hidden superpowers inside of you.
2:07
Level up with us each week by
2:09
listening to interviews with the best in
2:11
business, psychology, and relationships. We distill thousands
2:14
of hours of research in the most
2:16
effective tools and the latest science so
2:18
you can start winning today. Let's face
2:20
it. In order to be seen and
2:23
heard, your communication needs to cut through
2:25
the noise. And we're going to show
2:27
you how. I'm AJ, successfully recovered introvert
2:29
entrepreneur and self-development junkie. And I'm Johnny
2:32
Zubak, former touring musician, promoter, rock and
2:34
roller, and co-founder here at the Artich
2:36
Arm. And for the last 15 years,
2:39
we've trained thousands of top performers and
2:41
teams from every background. We have dedicated
2:43
our lives to teaching men and women
2:45
all they need to know about communication,
2:48
networking, and relationships. You shouldn't have to
2:50
settle for anything less than extraordinary. All
2:54
right, let's kick off today's show. Today we're talking
2:56
with Alex Hutchinson about why your brain
2:59
is literally wired to explore and how
3:01
to harness exploration for success. Alex Hutchinson
3:03
is an award-winning journalist who writes about
3:05
the science of endurance. He's a former
3:08
long-distance runner for the Canadian National Team
3:10
and he holds a master's in journalism
3:12
from Columbia and a PhD in physics
3:15
from Cambridge. He's the author of The
3:17
Explorers Gene, why we seek big challenges,
3:19
new flavors, new flavors, and the blank
3:21
spots on the map. The real secret
3:24
to happiness isn't avoiding hard things, it's
3:26
embracing them. Alex shares five dead simple
3:28
rules to make this work for you,
3:30
whether you're switching careers or just trying
3:33
to break out of a rut. You
3:35
know that feeling of wanting to know
3:37
what's around the corner? It's not random,
3:40
it's how you're built. But here's the
3:42
kicker. Modern Tech is hijacking that natural
3:44
instinct and it's costing you big time.
3:46
All right, welcome the show Alex, great
3:49
to have you. Thanks so much AJ,
3:51
it's really awesome to be here. I
3:53
know Johnny and I are very curious
3:55
what fascinated you about this topic because
3:58
as we dug into the book we
4:00
were just blown away by not only
4:02
the neuroscience but also just thinking a
4:04
lot about ourselves and exploration. Yeah this
4:07
was definitely a kind of personal journey
4:09
for me in the sense that part
4:11
of, but you know, part of the
4:14
reason I was interested in exploring is
4:16
that I found myself drawn to explore
4:18
for reasons that I, not only wasn't
4:20
sure what the reasons were, but I
4:23
wasn't sure whether they were good reasons.
4:25
And I, you know, I'm talking about
4:27
career decisions, even the decision to write
4:29
the book itself was an exploring decision
4:32
for me moving away from my entrenched
4:34
career path. So I had all these
4:36
questions and I was looking for answers,
4:39
but I got some. Are all humans
4:41
drawn to explore or are there specific
4:43
patterns or subsets of humans that express
4:45
this more than others? So I would
4:48
say that all humans are drawn to
4:50
explore. Now I know that that statement
4:52
will run into resistance and I, you
4:54
know, it runs into resistance. So if
4:57
I if I trotted out at a
4:59
cocktail party or whatever, people will say,
5:01
I hate exploring. I hate, you know,
5:03
the things that you like to do
5:06
in the back injury, I hate that,
5:08
trying new foods, I hate that. And
5:10
I get it. But there's a I
5:13
think you have to define exploring in
5:15
a very personal sense, in a sense
5:17
of when are you pushing out of
5:19
your own rut or your own path
5:22
or your own directions. And I think
5:24
we can make a strong case that
5:26
everyone has some draw to that. Some
5:28
people have it more than others, but
5:31
no one who's healthy and functioning is
5:33
doing exactly the same things every day
5:35
over and over again, kind of groundhog
5:38
day style. To clear this up for
5:40
our audience, should we define it as
5:42
stepping out of your comfort zone? Yeah,
5:44
I mean, metaphorically, it's like, are you
5:47
going to stay on the path that
5:49
you're on where you know where it's
5:51
headed and there's street signs and everything?
5:53
Are you going to step off the
5:56
path into the unknown? But so you
5:58
can think of it in terms of,
6:00
you know, as a gambling problem, are
6:02
you going to... take the bet that's
6:05
more uncertain where there's a there's a
6:07
risk. There's a chance of a higher
6:09
payout, but there's also chance of a
6:12
lower pay out or a failure of
6:14
going bankrupt. So exploring is accepting the
6:16
accepting risk. in some sense. I think
6:18
when many people think of exploring, they
6:21
think of sailors, astronauts, scientists, but I
6:23
would almost argue we all are with
6:25
these devices in our pocket and we're
6:27
looking at the usage of smartphone going
6:30
up and up and up and drawing
6:32
our attention and so many people I
6:34
talk to are falling into rabbit holes
6:37
with their device, exploring topics and things
6:39
that fascinate them. Do you argue the
6:41
same? Do you feel... That way I
6:43
do and I would say it's a
6:46
double-edged sword. So first of all I
6:48
would say yeah if we do a
6:50
word association test then you say explore
6:52
I say Christopher Columbus or whatever that's
6:55
that's how we think of it but
6:57
I think moments where we decide whether
6:59
or not to explore everywhere in our
7:01
lives it's like you go into a
7:04
restaurant you go to the restaurant that
7:06
you're familiar with do you order the
7:08
burger that you always order or do
7:11
you take a chance on the special
7:13
that's a decision whether or not to
7:15
not to explore. a spoken dumpster fire
7:17
of a controversy these days as to
7:20
whether they're good for us or not.
7:22
And I think social media and information
7:24
technology gives us opportunities to explore in
7:26
ways that we never would have had
7:29
access to before, but they can also
7:31
be, give us kind of the illusion
7:33
of exploration. They can tap into the
7:36
brain circuitries that are drawing us to
7:38
explore and keep us addicted to wanting
7:40
to find out what's down the next
7:42
link, the next link. without giving some
7:45
of the payoff that we get from
7:47
actively exploring in the real world. Now,
7:49
let's talk about that because title is
7:51
the explored gene. So is there a
7:54
gene that is regarded as the gene
7:56
that leads us to exploration or what's
7:58
going on neurologically and biologically there? Yeah,
8:00
I mean, so the title of the
8:03
book is, let's say, slightly tongue-in-cheek or
8:05
actually very tongue-in-cheek. There's not a single
8:07
gene that defines it, but it definitely,
8:10
very early in my journey in my
8:12
journey on this topic. I read some
8:14
stuff about a specific gene that's related
8:16
to how our brains process dopamine where
8:19
there are different variants, some of which
8:21
are sort of more or less sensitive
8:23
to dopamine which end up... that meaning
8:25
that some people need a bigger surprise
8:28
to get that same hit of feeling
8:30
like that feeling of discovery. And that
8:32
leads these people, it plays out in
8:35
the real world that these are the
8:37
people who are more likely to be
8:39
skiing out of bounds at a ski
8:41
area. They're more likely to be diagnosed
8:44
with ADHD, which is an interesting kind
8:46
of rather whole too, because then you
8:48
start to see. ADHD as one end
8:50
of a spectrum that has been very
8:53
useful for our species over time, people
8:55
who are drawn to always need to
8:57
know what's what's over the horizon. And
8:59
you can see that these differences, these
9:02
genetic differences, actually play out on a
9:04
population level too. So, you know, 100,000
9:06
years ago, our ancestors were all either
9:09
in Africa or somewhere near Africa just
9:11
coming out of Africa, and then over
9:13
the course of the next 50, basically
9:15
every habitable spot on the globe. And
9:18
if you look at how far different
9:20
populations traveled, the farther a population traveled,
9:22
let's say the southern tip of South
9:24
America is about as far as people
9:27
could go, those populations tend to have
9:29
a higher proportion of people with the
9:31
explorers version of the gene. So you
9:34
can see that exploring is wired in
9:36
us right from the start, and it
9:38
actually has, it plays a role in
9:40
how people go through the world and
9:43
live. So is that unique to our
9:45
species? The short answer is scientists disagree.
9:47
I talk to some who are like,
9:49
no man, like maple trees explore in
9:52
the sense of spreading across the globe,
9:54
like their seeds helicopter down in the
9:56
wind. And so that's not, the fact
9:58
that we spread into new territory all
10:01
the time, that's not unique to humans.
10:03
But we are in a way that
10:05
no other mammals do, we really spread
10:08
rapidly. and to everywhere, even in situations
10:10
where we had plenty of resources, where
10:12
we were. I think that's a key
10:14
point. Like, if you look... Looking back
10:17
at ancient migration, you sort of think,
10:19
well, I guess it was, you know,
10:21
maybe there was bad weather, you know,
10:23
not much food, it was too crowded,
10:26
so people had to leave, and that's
10:28
why we spread. But the history of
10:30
human migration also includes lots of situations
10:33
where resources were fine and humans seemed
10:35
to spread even, you know, it seemed
10:37
to want to discover what was over
10:39
the horizon, even when life was okay
10:42
where they were. So that it's not
10:44
scarcity driven. even in abundance we have
10:46
this innate desire to explore. Yeah, now
10:48
certainly Scare City is a good driver
10:51
to tell people to explore, but yeah,
10:53
I think this is this goes deeper
10:55
than just wanting to have enough food
10:57
for the winter. There's an argument in
11:00
neuroscience. There's something called the free energy
11:02
principle, which has become in the last
11:04
20 years a sort of new idea
11:07
for explaining how our brains are wired
11:09
and actually how all of life is
11:11
wired. And its fundamental principle is that
11:13
we want to be able to predict
11:16
the world accurately. And if you accept
11:18
that premise, then the best way to
11:20
predict the world, one way, the bad
11:22
way is you say, I'm going to
11:25
lock myself in the closet and turn
11:27
out all the lights. Because now I
11:29
know exactly what's going to happen all
11:32
the time. Nothing. And this is called
11:34
the dark room problem in philosophy and
11:36
in neuroscience. It's like, well, if we
11:38
want to predict everything, why don't we
11:41
just head into the closet? over the,
11:43
you know, you want to be able
11:45
to predict the future too, you want
11:47
to know what's going to happen. The
11:50
best way to predict the future is
11:52
to know as much as possible about
11:54
the world. So it turns out we're
11:57
wired to leave the dark room to
11:59
get out of the closet, go out
12:01
of the world and actually actively seek
12:03
out the things we know the least
12:06
about. So when we see, we know
12:08
what's down the road to the left,
12:10
but we've never been down the road
12:12
to the right. There's something in our
12:15
brains that says. Man, you really should
12:17
go down the road on the right
12:19
because we don't know anything about that
12:21
and that is the richest opportunity to
12:24
learn about the world. It seems to
12:26
me like there's this desire to map
12:28
then what's going on in these situations.
12:31
And I have a bit of a
12:33
silly anecdote, but my wife is pregnant
12:35
and we were scheduling a hospital tour.
12:37
And for me, the hospital tour was
12:40
more about getting to the hospital during
12:42
labor, where to park the car, and
12:44
how to actually get her inside the
12:46
clinic. And for her, she was remarking,
12:49
oh, you're not really taking any notes
12:51
during the actual tour. And I was
12:53
like, well, I'm trying to mentally map.
12:56
how to get us there when I
12:58
know there's going to be a spike
13:00
of adrenaline, I've never been to this
13:02
wing of the hospital, it's a big
13:05
hospital in Los Angeles, parking can be
13:07
quite hairy, and I felt quite relieved
13:09
once I had mentally mapped, okay, this
13:11
is our house and this is exactly
13:14
how I get you to the clinic,
13:16
and for her it was all, I
13:18
want to know what's going on after
13:20
I get to the clinic, like what
13:23
are the next steps, but both of
13:25
us at the end of that tour,
13:27
felt so much more relief, felt so
13:30
much more relief, Yeah, and I think
13:32
this this idea of mental mapping is
13:34
a really powerful one. I mean, you've
13:36
got a smartphone and that smartphone has
13:39
Google Maps on it and probably has
13:41
Uber on it and stuff. And so
13:43
on one level, you don't need to
13:45
know how to get to the hospital.
13:48
The all-seeing internet is going to tell
13:50
you how to get there. But if
13:52
you don't know, I mean, you can't
13:55
feel comfortable if you don't have some
13:57
sense embedded in your brain. And there's
13:59
a region of the brain called the
14:01
hippocampus. literal maps, like there's when you
14:04
see a new place, you're encoding it
14:06
in your hippo campus, such that at
14:08
every geographic point in that space, there's
14:10
a specific neuron that fires that says,
14:13
I'm at that space. And so to
14:15
learn a place involves actually physically mapping
14:17
it in your brain. And there's famous
14:19
studies with, for example, London taxi drivers
14:22
who they. at least in the past
14:24
had to basically memorize the entire city
14:26
of London in order to pass their
14:29
taxi tests and the so these taxi
14:31
drivers turned out their hippocampuses would physically
14:33
grow in order to incorporate this memory.
14:35
They knew London in a way that
14:38
you don't know if you just kind
14:40
of fly into the airport and type
14:42
into your phone, take me to the,
14:44
take me to the, you know, Tower
14:47
of London or whatever. Hey everyone, quick
14:49
break. Johnny, I need to talk about
14:51
something that's been bothering us. That's right.
14:54
After training over 11,000 professionals in the
14:56
last 18 years, we are seeing a
14:58
pattern that is honestly frustrating to watch.
15:00
They're good at their jobs. They're doing
15:03
fine. But they're stuck in this dangerous
15:05
trap of being just average. Average influence,
15:07
average relationships, and average career trajectory. And
15:09
here's what's crazy. They're actually working harder
15:12
than ever. But that next level of
15:14
success, it just keeps slipping away. Because
15:16
here's the truth. Technical skills and hard
15:18
work just aren't enough anymore. Not in
15:21
2025. That's why we created X Factor
15:23
Accelerator. It's our proven system for breaking
15:25
out of average and developing unstoppable influence
15:28
in just eight weeks. And right now
15:30
we are doing something that we've never
15:32
done before. The next 10 people who
15:34
join will get a private blind spot
15:37
assessment with either Johnny or me personally.
15:39
We'll identify what's really holding you back
15:41
and build your customized roadmap for success
15:43
in the program. This is usually reserved
15:46
for our highest level clients, but we
15:48
want to make sure that you get
15:50
results fast. But here's the deal. This
15:53
isn't for everyone. If you're comfortable being
15:55
average, skip ahead. But if you're ready
15:57
to stop settling, if you're ready to
15:59
develop the kind of influence that transforms
16:02
careers and relationships, head to unlock your
16:04
X factor.com right now. Again, that's Unlock
16:06
your X factor.com right now. Again, that's
16:08
Unlock your X factor.com. Remember, only your
16:11
X factor.com. Remember, only your X factor.com.
16:13
Remember, only the next 10 people gets
16:15
this private assessment with AJ or myself,
16:17
what you say. But how they show
16:20
up? Exactly. And speaking of showing up,
16:22
I've got to tell you about my
16:24
latest obsession with Quince. You know that
16:27
shore jacket I was wearing at our
16:29
last boot camp? The one everyone kept
16:31
asking about? Wait, that was only $60,
16:33
right? Quinn has completely changed the game
16:36
when it comes to elevated style. I
16:38
used to think looking professional meant breaking
16:40
the bank, but they're offering these incredible
16:42
luxury essentials at prices that actually make
16:45
sense. I've got their leather jacket, and
16:47
I can't tell you how many compliments
16:49
that I get. The best part? It's
16:52
priced 50 to 80% less than similar
16:54
brands. And it's not just about the
16:56
price, though that's amazing. They're partnering directly
16:58
with top factories, cutting out the middlemen
17:01
and passing on those savings to us.
17:03
They only work with factories using safe,
17:05
ethical, and responsible manufacturing practices. The premium
17:07
fabrics and finishes are incredible. Whether it's
17:10
their cashmere sweaters, iconic leather jackets, or
17:12
they're flown at active wear that's perfect
17:14
for our coaching sessions, everything just works.
17:16
Here's the exciting part. Our listeners can
17:19
get free shipping on their order and
17:21
365 day returns. Just go to quince.com/charm.
17:23
That's quince.com/charm to get free shipping and
17:26
365 day returns. Indulge an affordable luxury
17:28
at quince.com/charm. Well that brings up a
17:30
very good point. How can we feel
17:32
at ease if we're not mental mapping?
17:35
Right? So our cell phones now are
17:37
our biggest distraction. We have all the
17:39
world's information at our fingertips. We can
17:41
watch cat videos for the rest of
17:44
our life. And we don't need to
17:46
be bothered with expanding our hippocampus. We
17:48
don't need to be bothered with mental
17:51
mapping because, well, I'm just going to
17:53
ask ChatGBT and it gives me the
17:55
answer and I'm going to go over
17:57
here and I'm going to get the
18:00
answer on TikTok. So our exploration has
18:02
been significantly cut down. And if that
18:04
is a human need, well then that
18:06
is going to result in the symptoms
18:09
of feeling. on ease, anxiety rising, having
18:11
an overwhelming feelings of despair, because we
18:13
haven't settled that mental mapping. And to
18:15
go on with AJ was saying, for
18:18
myself, when I go on trips, vacation,
18:20
I like to stay in a city
18:22
as long as possible. And I'm usually
18:25
high with anxiety walking around the city
18:27
until the first day that I can
18:29
go out for a jog, and I'll
18:31
go out for a jog, maybe it's
18:34
five, maybe it's 10 miles, but that's
18:36
what I'm doing, a mental mapping, and
18:38
I feel so much better after that
18:40
jog about the rest of my trip,
18:43
what I want to see, what do
18:45
I want to investigate because of that
18:47
simple mental mapping. So again, this comes
18:50
down to our own well-being. if we're
18:52
unable to do the exploring that our
18:54
mind needs. I think that's a really
18:56
important point. There's a few scientists I
18:59
spoke to who are actually really concerned
19:01
about the decline of mental matter, the
19:03
decline of knowing spaces, not just knowing
19:05
how to get to a place by
19:08
pressing a button, but actually knowing these
19:10
spaces. I mentioned the London taxi drivers.
19:12
Their hippo campuses get bigger because they've
19:14
had to learn so many spaces. The
19:17
flip side of that is if you
19:19
don't use your hippocampus, if you never
19:21
need to navigate, there's now evidence that
19:24
your hippocampus starts to shrink. And that's
19:26
really bad news because a shrinking hippocampus
19:28
has been found to be an early
19:30
predictor of a bunch of different conditions,
19:33
including Alzheimer's and other forms of cognitive
19:35
decline and anxiety and depression. And so,
19:37
you know, You want to kind of
19:39
avoid being the little boy or your
19:42
chicken little who's always word that the
19:44
sky is falling. There's a lot of
19:46
alarmism about technology, but I came out
19:49
of writing this book committed to being
19:51
thoughtful about using the GPS. navigation on
19:53
my phone. So I mean, the phone,
19:55
I need to use it to get
19:58
places sometimes, but I try to turn
20:00
it off until I need it. And
20:02
you know, one of the researchers, a
20:04
woman named Vernique Bobo, who's in McGill
20:07
University in Montreal, who's one of the
20:09
world experts on this, her advice is
20:11
like, okay, you need to use it
20:13
sometimes. It's the modern world. But keep
20:16
it in the kind of bird's eye
20:18
view as much as possible so that
20:20
at least. you're following it along, but
20:23
you can see where you're going. I'm
20:25
going north and I'm going to go
20:27
left there, I'm going to go east
20:29
there or however you think of directions,
20:32
as opposed to being in the ego-centric
20:34
view where it's showing you what the
20:36
world looks like. And then you really
20:38
have no idea how where you are
20:41
fits into the bigger picture. So it's
20:43
not about giving up technology completely, but
20:45
it's about being aware, being willing to,
20:48
you know, going back to the idea
20:50
of exploring one of the... important
20:52
components of exploring is being willing to
20:54
be lost sometimes. Now you don't want
20:56
to be lost in the Sahara Desert,
20:58
but if you're in a new town
21:00
in the hotel, hey, maybe you get
21:03
off track by a few blocks, you've
21:05
still got the phone in your pocket,
21:07
you can always find yourself, but be
21:09
willing to wander because by being willing
21:11
to be lost or to find your
21:13
way, you have to be looking around,
21:15
you're seeing your surroundings. because you're trying
21:17
to not get lost. And so that's
21:19
engaging your hippo campus. And it also
21:21
means you're experiencing the city, you're experiencing,
21:23
you're experiencing, you're experiencing, or as you
21:25
explore, even in your own city. You're
21:27
seeing things for the first time, because
21:29
you have to, instead of just looking
21:31
down at your phone and having your
21:33
experience mediated by the screen. Yeah, I'm
21:35
really curious about that last point there
21:37
and VR and AR and how. that
21:39
might change our brain because a lot
21:41
of what we're talking about here as
21:43
humans is exploration of actual world, the
21:45
landscape around us, the tactile nature, understanding
21:47
directionality, whereas now we're getting to a
21:49
place where we're strapping screens on our
21:51
face and trying our best to simulate
21:53
that as much as possible, but is
21:55
there any science around the difference between
21:57
actually experiencing the place or putting on
21:59
the... Tower goggles. Yeah, so it's an
22:02
emerging world, right? So maybe if
22:04
you extrapolate out to the future
22:06
and the virtual reality is so
22:08
complete that it's just like being
22:10
in the real world and where
22:12
you actually can get lost
22:14
in this virtual reality and you have
22:17
to figure out which way you're oriented,
22:19
I don't know. Maybe at that point
22:21
it's the same. But at this point,
22:23
what I would say is there is
22:26
quite a bit of research
22:28
showing the difference between actively
22:30
exploring somewhere and passively exploring
22:32
it. So you might say, what's
22:35
the difference between going to
22:37
Mount Everest and climbing it versus
22:39
watching a documentary about going to
22:41
Mount Everest and climbing it? You
22:43
see all the same sites. You
22:45
hear all the same sounds. So
22:47
in a sense, you're experiencing the
22:50
same thing. But there's quite a bit
22:52
of research showing that you process
22:54
information differently. if you're actively
22:56
exploring something compared to if
22:58
it's just shown to you
23:00
past something, even if it's
23:02
exactly the same information, that if
23:04
you're just a spectator, you don't absorb
23:06
it in the same way, you don't learn in the
23:08
same way. And so to me, it's a sliding
23:10
scale of kind of autonomy in exploration from
23:13
I'm going to go into the wilderness
23:15
and climb Mount Everest all by myself to
23:17
I'm going to hire a guy to take
23:19
me up Mount Everest because I don't know
23:22
the way to Everest and I'm going to
23:24
dive. So this guy's going to make some
23:26
of the decisions for me, but I'm
23:28
still be moving through the world and make
23:31
choices to, I'm going to lie in the
23:33
sofa and watch a documentary. And each of
23:35
those has a role to play, but I think
23:37
we want to always be pushing to
23:39
the most active form of exploration that
23:42
we're comfortable with in a given context.
23:44
I've never climbed that ever. when I
23:46
went trekking in Nepal a decade ago
23:49
and trekked to the base camp of Everest,
23:51
we did it without a guide because
23:53
we wanted to be looking around and
23:55
having the adventure exploring for ourselves as
23:57
opposed to just following someone along and
23:59
how having the equivalent of a kind
24:02
of walking documentary playing in front
24:04
of us. Now looking at obviously
24:06
the difference there in exploration on your
24:08
own versus a guide and having support,
24:10
you know, what are the downsides
24:12
to just avoiding this exploration in terms
24:15
of engaging that side of your brain?
24:17
You talked a little bit about
24:19
shrinking hippocampus, but you know, what are
24:21
some of the other ways that this
24:24
can impact us negatively if we
24:26
aren't exploring? Yeah, you know, one of
24:28
the... The phenomena that I came across
24:30
that I hadn't heard about before
24:32
was something that's called the creativity crisis.
24:35
So there's an effect called the Flynn
24:37
effect, which is that IQ tests
24:39
have been going steadily up since about
24:41
the 1920s and 1930s, and modern life
24:44
requires a lot of abstract thinking,
24:46
so it seems like we're actually getting
24:48
better at it. But there's a reverse
24:50
effect that scores on standard tests
24:52
of creativity. have been going down since
24:55
about the late 1980s, early 1990s, and
24:57
going down even more sharply in
24:59
the last 10 years or so, it's
25:02
hard to distinguish between correlation and causation.
25:04
We don't know exactly what's causing
25:06
it, but the rise of everyone sitting
25:08
on their phone and social media is,
25:11
it seems to me a likely
25:13
candidate. So the thing is, if you're
25:15
just passively being fed whatever the algorithm
25:17
is giving you and not making
25:19
any active choices. You're not learning in
25:22
the same way and you're not even
25:24
learning how to learn. You're not
25:26
learning how to explore. You're not learning
25:28
the ways that you can follow your
25:31
own interests and have that become
25:33
a passion that you then pursue for
25:35
the rest of your life or whatever.
25:37
And so we seem to be
25:39
doing worse on scores of creativity. And
25:42
if you, that's kids, testing kids. Now
25:44
you zoom out and look at,
25:46
let's say. You can do these huge
25:48
big data analyses of US patents or
25:51
of scientific papers. And there's various
25:53
metrics you can use to say, how
25:55
many breakthrough papers do we have? or
25:57
breakthrough patents that really changed something as
26:00
opposed to just incrementally adding on
26:02
to previous patents or previous discoveries. And
26:04
the rates of breakthrough discoveries seem to
26:06
have been going down for the
26:08
last few decades. So, you know, these
26:11
are big trends and it's hard to
26:13
pinpoint an exact culprit, but for
26:15
whatever reason it does seem like. as
26:17
a society both kids and adults were
26:20
getting less creative and I think
26:22
that may have something to do with
26:24
the decline of exploration. Our institutions also
26:26
have been highly screwed up and
26:28
there's whole walls of fields that are
26:31
walled off due to political agendas and
26:33
everything else that goes along with
26:35
it which prohibits a lot of that
26:38
exploring and innovation which makes it difficult
26:40
and with the 80s I mean
26:42
We're now talking about more channels added
26:44
to television, right? This is now cable
26:47
TV, HBO, Cinemax, MTV. All of
26:49
those things led to longer television hours.
26:51
So all of those things have certainly
26:53
contributed to it. And if all
26:55
you're doing is being fed entertainment and
26:58
you're just consuming, well, yeah, all of
27:00
the creative flow. and areas of
27:02
your brain that helps with that are
27:04
going on at your feet. One of
27:07
the sort of correlations with the
27:09
decline of creativity is the decline of
27:11
free play. So kids are sitting watching
27:13
TV. They're also sitting in the
27:15
car being driven to this lesson and
27:18
that lesson. My kids are eight and
27:20
ten and I wrestle with this
27:22
all the time because I spend a
27:24
lot of time in the car driving
27:27
them to their soccer practice, driving
27:29
them to hockey, because we're in Canada,
27:31
driving them to various. formal opportunities to
27:33
learn, which feels like, oh, we're
27:35
giving them every chance in the book,
27:38
but it's like, I really struggle to
27:40
try and make sure they also
27:42
have six hours to sit, you know,
27:44
alone, hopefully outside wandering. around in the
27:47
backyard and learning to listen to their
27:49
own instincts for what's interesting because
27:51
I think one of the best pieces
27:53
of advice to follow in terms of
27:56
exploring is to trust your instincts
27:58
as to what you think is interesting
28:00
because your brain has this fine-tuned mechanism
28:02
to try and look for areas
28:04
of uncertainty or the unknown as we
28:07
were talking about this earlier. Your brain
28:09
is that's a... One of its
28:11
fundamental purposes is to look for areas
28:14
where there's rich opportunities to learn. So
28:16
if your kid says, I'm curious
28:18
about that, you might think, well, that's
28:20
stupid. You should be curious about calculus
28:23
because then you'll get a good
28:25
job. It's like, no, you should trust
28:27
their instincts about what they're curious about
28:29
and let them follow those roads.
28:31
So after writing this book and looking
28:34
at all the research, I'm really curious
28:36
to hear what changes you made
28:38
in your own life and maybe in
28:40
your parenting style to really tap into
28:43
this curiosity and exploration and exploration.
28:45
I talked to a researcher in Denmark
28:47
who studies play in the context of
28:49
these new brain theories and I
28:51
was asking him like, come on buddy,
28:54
give me some tips. You've got young
28:56
kids too. What do you do
28:58
with them is like, because we all
29:00
struggle with balancing risk with opportunity, right?
29:03
Nobody wants their kid to be
29:05
falling off the top of the playground
29:07
or falling in the raging river or
29:09
being abducted by the guy in
29:11
the white van or whatever. And his
29:14
main advice was just that he. tries
29:16
to say yes as much as
29:18
possible. And so I've, that tries to
29:20
respect. So if his kids think something
29:23
is worth pursuing, he doesn't try
29:25
and superimpose his adult perspective on that.
29:27
It's like, if they think that sitting
29:29
in the backyard and piling up
29:31
sticks is a great way to spend
29:34
six hours on a Saturday, they should
29:36
do that. Maybe they're gonna be
29:38
an architect, maybe not, who cares? The
29:40
point is right now they find that
29:43
interesting. So I've really tried to embrace
29:45
that. In my own life, you
29:47
know, these are very sort of amorphous
29:50
things because I was hoping when I
29:52
started this project that I would
29:54
come out with. the formula of when
29:56
to explore, when to exploit. Because exploring
29:59
isn't always the right choice, right?
30:01
Like if you do nothing but exploring,
30:03
that's the classic like, you know, you,
30:05
it's like, of dating, for example,
30:07
you're always wondering if the next date
30:10
will be better than the one you're
30:12
with. Well, 50 years from now,
30:14
you're going to be like, ah, maybe
30:16
I should have settled down with someone
30:19
at some point. So you need
30:21
to know when to exploit, when to
30:23
take advantage of the information. you already
30:25
have, and there's no good formula
30:27
for that, despite the attempts of many
30:30
scientists. But I'm definitely trying to leave
30:32
myself open to actually following some
30:34
of those mental rabbit holes of like
30:36
when I get interested in something, hopefully
30:39
not just to the extent of
30:41
clicking on a bunch of links, but
30:43
yeah, if I'm interested in some music,
30:45
let me get that album. Let's
30:47
listen to the whole album. Let's see
30:50
what that's about, as opposed to just
30:52
moving on to the next thing
30:54
immediately. Go
32:54
further with the American Express
32:56
Business Gold card. Earn three
32:58
times membership rewards points on flights
33:00
and prepaid hotels when you book
33:02
through Amex travel.com. Whether your destination
33:05
is a business conference or a
33:07
client meeting, your purchases will help
33:10
you earn more points for future
33:12
trips. Experience more on your travels
33:14
with Amex Business Gold. Terms apply.
33:17
Learn more at American express.com/business dash
33:19
gold card. Build for business by
33:22
American Express. What
33:24
we've noticed in our clients is there
33:26
is this paradigm around uncertainty. So a
33:29
lot of our clients will come for
33:31
coaching because they've encountered an area of
33:33
their life, whether it's dating or career,
33:35
where there's an immense amount of uncertainty
33:38
that's leading to anxiety. And then anxiety
33:40
creates fears and a lot of things
33:42
that negatively impact your behaviors and your
33:44
thoughts about yourself, self-worth. And what's so
33:46
interesting in our immersive experiences especially is
33:49
once we get them through some exercises
33:51
that walk them through the uncertainty and
33:53
show that the outcomes that we're creating
33:55
anxiety are very rare if ever to
33:58
happen. So let's say someone. rejecting you
34:00
socially or someone throwing a drink in
34:02
your face at a bar for asking
34:04
them for their phone number, a lot
34:07
of these heightened emotions that come with
34:09
uncertainty, once you recognize that those outcomes
34:11
are very unlikely to happen, then it
34:13
like flips and they get excited about
34:16
chasing uncertainty in their life and putting
34:18
themselves more into uncertain situations to experience
34:20
that, to learn what it's like to
34:22
feel that uncertainty and then they'll come
34:25
back to us years later and be
34:27
like, That led to a huge leap
34:29
in my career in a startup idea
34:31
that I'd been kicking around for years.
34:34
So what's going on with that uncertainty
34:36
and that paradigm shift from, okay, it's
34:38
so much uncertainty, I'm frozen, I'm stuck,
34:40
to now I'm chasing uncertainty and I'm
34:43
excited to explore it fully? It goes
34:45
beyond just recognizing that the negative outcomes
34:47
are rare. I would actually say the
34:49
negative outcomes are important. They're part of
34:52
the game and you have to learn
34:54
to embrace. illustrations of the Explorer exploit
34:56
dilemma is I mentioned before restaurant ordering.
34:58
You go into a restaurant, you order
35:00
the dish that you know is okay,
35:03
or do you to take a chance.
35:05
So for like half a century, scientists
35:07
have been talking about this. But now
35:09
in the modern air, we can actually
35:12
look at big data of how people
35:14
order in restaurants. So some scientists at
35:16
Harvard took data from a couple hundred
35:18
thousand customers of a delivery service called
35:21
Deliveroo. So food delivery service that's bigger
35:23
in Europe. they analyze their ordering patterns.
35:25
So to say, how often do they
35:27
go back to the same old restaurants?
35:30
How often do they try new restaurants?
35:32
And what happens when they do that?
35:34
Do they, is exploring this wonderful thing
35:36
that brings them happy joy and wonderful
35:39
meals? And the answer is in the
35:41
short term, no. Actually, when they try
35:43
a new restaurant, on average, they rate
35:45
that meal lower than if they go
35:48
back to a known restaurant. So exploring
35:50
comes with a cost. And it's not
35:52
getting the drink thrown in your face,
35:54
in your face. The flip side of
35:57
that is that over time, people's average
35:59
ratings, the more they order, the more
36:01
their average ratings of food go up.
36:03
They drift upwards because over time as
36:06
they explore, they're finding some dud, some
36:08
restaurants they don't like and they're leaving
36:10
those behind and never going back and
36:12
they're finding some restaurants they like more
36:14
than the ones they knew and they're
36:17
adding those to the rotation. So without
36:19
the failures, without being willing to have
36:21
some bad meals, you never discover the
36:23
places that have better meals. And so
36:26
I think this is like a... you
36:28
know, not to be too grandiose about
36:30
it, but it's like a rule for
36:32
life. If you're not failing, if you're
36:35
not having some negative outcomes, if you're
36:37
not getting the proverbial or the figured
36:39
of drink thrown in your face, that
36:41
almost guarantees that you're missing out on
36:44
chances to find stuff that's better than
36:46
what you're doing now. So you should,
36:48
when you do have, when you see
36:50
uncertainty, you know that's an opportunity, like
36:53
you're saying. And then when things do
36:55
go wrong, you embrace that uncertainty and
36:57
sometimes you get a negative outcome, you
36:59
should say, this is a sign I'm
37:02
doing things right to some degree. I
37:04
mean, look, if you're getting drinks thrown
37:06
in your face every night, you're not
37:08
doing it right. But the negative outcomes
37:11
are an important precondition to getting improved
37:13
outcomes. It's funny. It goes to the
37:15
same way of how we express ourselves
37:17
as well. We could do that safely.
37:20
We could do that with what we
37:22
know works. or gets the job done,
37:24
or we can find new ways to
37:26
express us that to get our message
37:28
out there farther from the language we
37:31
use to the clothes that we wear.
37:33
And certainly if you're exploring yourself an
37:35
expression of fashion, there's going to be
37:37
maybe days where maybe you over overshot
37:40
it, right? You maybe wore something, everyone's
37:42
like, that's not you. But again, and
37:44
helps you find out what does work,
37:46
what makes you feel incredibly good when
37:49
you're wearing it, and even things that
37:51
people other folks may not vibe with
37:53
but you feel incredibly good with it.
37:55
Well then you're gonna you're gonna change
37:58
how it is perceived in other people
38:00
by how well you feel in wearing
38:02
it. And I think that's a super
38:04
example of the ways in which exploration
38:07
is not just about Christopher Columbus or
38:09
about climbing Mount Everest that it's there's
38:11
a lot of dimensions on which you
38:13
can be exploring things and All of
38:16
us need to find some of those
38:18
dimensions where we're exploring and pushing our
38:20
limits, whether it's physical or mental or
38:22
creative or emotional. I've been fortunate to
38:25
spend some time traveling through Europe with
38:27
my wife, and one of our favorite
38:29
things to do is first the digital
38:31
exploration of, okay, what are some spots
38:34
you want to check out based on
38:36
some short form videos that the algorithm
38:38
tells us about? And of course, you
38:40
explore a little bit of Tokyo or
38:42
Paris online, and then it starts feeding
38:45
you all these other recommendations. and we'll
38:47
map some spots in different neighborhoods that
38:49
we want to explore. But what's been
38:51
really funny as we look back on
38:54
these trips almost nine times out of
38:56
10, the meals that stand out the
38:58
most or the experiences that stand out
39:00
the most are the ones where we
39:03
stumbled into exploring ourselves versus relying on
39:05
the digital explorers before us who created
39:07
these nifty little short form videos to
39:09
tell us this is the best sushi
39:12
in Tokyo. We always laugh. Our favorite
39:14
spots are the ones that we explored
39:16
on our own, just getting lost in
39:18
a neighborhood and stepping into a new
39:21
environment instead of the ones we relied
39:23
on that digital exploration. That phenomenon is,
39:25
you know, it's even, it's pre-digital too,
39:27
because I can remember backpacking through Europe
39:30
in the 90s, and of course I
39:32
had the lonely planet and the rough
39:34
guide, and you'd go from city to
39:36
city, and it's like, okay, which place
39:39
got like two stars, Okay, maybe these
39:41
are the same people I saw in
39:43
the last city, but they're basically the
39:45
same people. We're all the same. We're
39:47
all ticking off the same boxes. And
39:50
some of those boxes are pretty cool
39:52
to tick up. So, you know, it's
39:54
not that you never want to go
39:56
to any place that other people recommend,
39:59
but I'm 100% with you that the
40:01
real joy is sort of wandering through
40:03
the street, ending up in a place
40:05
where nobody else is speaking English. And
40:08
you're like, okay, so I'm not just
40:10
here because this place was not built
40:12
to serve people like me who are
40:14
looking for the illusion of a genuine
40:17
experience. This is actually just a place
40:19
where people in the neighborhood go and
40:21
I'm going to go. I can remember
40:23
I was backpacking through Spain and we
40:26
were in a small seaside fishing village
40:28
and we went to this little restaurant
40:30
and we were way off the beaten
40:32
track so I was like this is
40:35
this is awesome. The menu is totally
40:37
in Spanish. I didn't speak any Spanish.
40:39
I was like I'm an intrepid explorer.
40:41
I'm willing to try anything. I just
40:44
pointed something on the menu. I had
40:46
no idea what it was. I'm such
40:48
a cool guy. An hour and a
40:50
half later. my meal finally arrives but
40:53
we're like starving. Basically the guy had
40:55
gone to the, I think he might
40:57
have gone out in his boat or
40:59
something and caught an octopus because what
41:01
I had ordered was boiled octopus. He
41:04
on my plate was this entire boiled
41:06
octopus with no seasoning. I saw, I
41:08
always finished my plate but I always
41:10
finished my plate but I could not
41:13
finish this. I just needed for like
41:15
an hour of completely stuff that tasted
41:17
gross. So I was like, this is
41:19
an example of where maybe a recommendation
41:22
that Sometimes if you're just going by
41:24
yourself, you have to risk the chance
41:26
that you're just going to get a
41:28
whole boiled octopus on your plate. That's
41:31
not going to be that good. I
41:33
feel so much comes out of that
41:35
experience versus collecting the knowledge through the
41:37
digital exploration or the guidebook or the
41:40
word of mouth from friends and referrals
41:42
versus you actually going through that experience
41:44
and recognizing now. Okay, well, I know
41:46
exactly what octopus is in Spanish and
41:49
I'll steer clear of that on the
41:51
next menu. So I do a fair
41:53
amount of backcountry travel, like backpacking and
41:55
canoeing. And it's almost... It's very difficult
41:58
to find any place where there aren't
42:00
at least a few people who've done
42:02
like video logs or blogs with photos
42:04
about it. And so it's like, oh,
42:07
thank God, I know what's coming on
42:09
this. Now I can plan properly. I
42:11
know whether I need to bring crampons
42:13
or whatever. But on the flip side
42:15
is, there's no more surprises. There's no
42:18
more like coming around the corner and
42:20
seeing this amazing waterfall, because she's like,
42:22
oh yeah, this is the one that
42:24
I saw on the one that I
42:27
saw on the block. Nobody is trying
42:29
to pretend that we're discovering the world
42:31
that knew, like we understand the whole
42:33
world has been seen, but there's a
42:36
feeling of discovering something from yourself that
42:38
is different from having it spoon fed
42:40
to you. And there's also a gratification
42:42
that comes with mental and physical effort
42:45
that is tied to how our brain
42:47
works. So what's going on there where
42:49
we're putting this mental and physical effort
42:51
into exploration that leads to this payoff
42:54
or the paradox around effort? There's a
42:56
misconception that the effort or the difficulties
42:58
that are... required in order to get
43:00
somewhere are the price we pay. So
43:03
like you climb to the top of
43:05
the mountain and it was really hard
43:07
to climb there but it's all that
43:09
negative side was worth it because you
43:12
get to the top of the mountain.
43:14
And I think that's actually the wrong
43:16
side was worth it because you get
43:18
to the top of the mountain. And
43:21
I think that's actually the wrong way
43:23
of thinking about it. And it's actually
43:25
a crucial part of what makes it
43:27
worth going up going up the mountain.
43:29
Like, there's nothing special about that. So
43:32
this, there's, this is what psychologists call
43:34
the effort paradox, the fact that even
43:36
though effort is supposed to be a
43:38
negative, we actually value things more highly
43:41
in a lot of cases when they're
43:43
hard. And that goes, there's even research
43:45
on IKEA furniture, for example, the IKEA
43:47
effect is that if you order a
43:50
piece of furniture, you order that coffee
43:52
table and you spend time wrestling with
43:54
those stupid instructions and the little Allen
43:56
keys and stuff, and you put it
43:59
together. the coffee table off you, you'll
44:01
ask for a higher price than if
44:03
you had gotten the exact same coffee
44:05
table pre-made for you. having struggled with
44:08
it makes it more valuable to you
44:10
and I think that's it's a nice
44:12
reframing to remember when something is hard
44:14
you remembered your you have to remind
44:17
yourself this is what's making the whole
44:19
experience meaningful to me and worthwhile. Well
44:21
you brought up a very good philosophical
44:23
point which is the the fundamental question
44:26
of what life is about and for
44:28
there's going to be a lot of
44:30
people that who are on the side
44:32
of it's about struggle and what you
44:35
make out of that struggle. And there's
44:37
a lot of people who think it's
44:39
about avoiding struggle and staying comfortable as
44:41
much as possible. And our religious texts
44:43
explain these ideas fully. Yeah, this question
44:46
of the meeting of life is clearly
44:48
not one that I can solve in
44:50
a simple manner, but what... So I
44:52
spoke to one of the psychologists who
44:55
works on the effort paradox and I
44:57
was asking him about this like, what
44:59
is the goal? What is our point?
45:01
And he has developed what he calls
45:04
the meaningfulness of effort scale. So it's
45:06
a way of assessing people to find
45:08
out how much effort, how much meaning
45:10
they derive from when something is hard.
45:13
So some people, if something's hard, they're
45:15
like, yeah, this is really important and
45:17
meaningful to me. Other people. don't feel
45:19
the same way. And so one of
45:22
the questions I asked him was like,
45:24
okay, but like, what do you mean
45:26
by meaning of life? What is the
45:28
meaning of life? And he's like, dude,
45:31
I don't know what the meaning of
45:33
life is. And nor does anybody ask.
45:35
Nobody can define it, but everyone understands
45:37
what we're asking. They can tell you
45:40
what, which. which activities are more meaningful
45:42
or less meaningful. Bringing up a kid
45:44
is meaningful even though it's hard. You
45:46
know, eating a bag of popcorn is
45:49
not meaningful even if it's tasty. So
45:51
we have a sense of which things
45:53
give meaning in our lives even if
45:55
we can't articulate them. And what he
45:57
has found is that people who derive
46:00
meaning from taking on challenge, whether that's
46:02
bringing up kids. or climbing mountains or
46:04
you know, launching a business or whatever
46:06
the case may be. Those people tend
46:09
to actually score higher on ratings of
46:11
happiness. They tend to end up with
46:13
higher incomes, more so more professional success,
46:15
more greater mental health. So this idea
46:18
that gaining meaning from doing hard things,
46:20
it has some empirical support in the
46:22
sense that of making people happier. Well,
46:36
with the name of your price
46:38
tool from Progressive, you can find
46:40
options that fit your budget and
46:42
potentially lower your bills. Try it
46:44
at progressive.com. Progressive Casualty Insurance Company
46:46
and affiliates. Price and coverage match
46:48
limited by state law. Not available.
46:55
Start your next California adventure behind
46:57
the wheel of an exciting new
47:00
Toyota hybrid with the largest lineup
47:02
of hybrid plug-in hybrid and electrified
47:04
vehicles to choose from Toyota has
47:06
the one for you. Every new
47:08
Toyota hybrid comes with Toyota Care,
47:11
a two-year complementary scheduled maintenance plan,
47:13
plus an exclusive hybrid battery warranty
47:15
and Toyota's legendary quality and reliability.
47:17
Visit your local Toyota dealer dealer
47:19
today. Toyota, let's go places. See
47:21
your local Toyota dealer for hybrid
47:24
battery warranty details. So
47:26
looking at dopamine, which obviously is a
47:28
core driver of all this, there's this
47:30
popular phrase of a dopamine detox and
47:32
people looking to get away from devices
47:34
and things that simulate dopamine in their
47:36
lives, what do you recommend in terms
47:39
of strengthening this exploration and this curiosity
47:41
and how can dopamine influence it? This
47:43
idea of like dopamine detox or dopamine
47:45
fasting, it's fascinating because you read about
47:47
it and it's like, this kind of
47:49
makes sense. There's not a lot of,
47:51
I would say there's, but not a
47:54
lot, there's no neuroscience behind the idea
47:56
that you need to detox from dopamine,
47:58
but on understanding the role that dopamine
48:00
plays and also trying to avoid getting
48:02
kind of enthrall to the easiest dopamine
48:04
hit, I think is actually a useful
48:06
way of thinking about things. So I
48:09
think the important thing to understand about
48:11
dopamine is it's not like the pleasure
48:13
chemical, which is what scientists originally thought
48:15
back in the 70s and 80s. They
48:17
thought it's like when something feels good,
48:19
it's because you've got to hit a
48:21
dopamine. Dopamine is really about. predicting the
48:24
experience you're going to have. And you
48:26
get to hit a dopamine when something
48:28
is better than you expect. Not just
48:30
when something is good, but when it's
48:32
better than you expect. So think about
48:34
that in terms of taking a drug,
48:36
for example. You take a drug for
48:39
example. You take a drug for the
48:41
first, and I'm not recommending this, but
48:43
you take a drug for the first
48:45
time, and I'm not recommending this, but
48:47
you take a drug for the first
48:49
time, and I'm not recommending it. 20th
48:51
time, you know exactly what you're going
48:54
to expect. So you no longer get
48:56
any dopamine hit at all from taking
48:58
the drug. So you have to raise
49:00
your dosage. And that's how one of
49:02
the ways that addiction, the cycle of
49:04
addiction starts. You have to keep raising
49:06
the dosage in order to keep doing
49:09
something that's better than expected. So what's
49:11
a better way of getting these stimuli
49:13
that are better than expected or different
49:15
than expected? It's to do things you
49:17
haven't done before. It's to keep looking
49:19
for opportunities of testing your limits in
49:21
new ways. it remains surprising and good.
49:24
Like, I have this debate with my
49:26
kids, like, I make them waffles for
49:28
breakfast a couple times a week, and
49:30
they're my favorite waffle recipe. They love
49:32
the waffle recipe. They want it every
49:34
morning. And I'm like, you can't have
49:36
it every morning, not because I'm not
49:39
capable of making it every morning, and
49:41
it's actually a pretty healthy recipe. It's
49:43
all like cottage cheese and eggs and
49:45
stuff, so in some ways, I would
49:47
be happy for you to have it
49:49
every morning. But I want you to
49:51
enjoy it as much as, you know,
49:54
I want you to enjoy it in
49:56
six months as much as you enjoy
49:58
it now and in a year, because
50:00
it's a pretty convenient recipe and I
50:02
don't want to have to come up
50:04
with a new breakfast. So it's like,
50:06
I'm giving it to you twice a
50:09
week because I want there still to
50:11
be novelty and not just. to be
50:13
as expected every morning I get up
50:15
to get my waffle. So I don't
50:17
know, that's the waffle theory of not
50:19
dopamine detox, but of just being mindful
50:21
about ways of preserving surprise. That goes
50:24
along with, there's a, well, it's not
50:26
really a new term, but a term
50:28
that has sort of been brought back
50:30
and it's now edging its way into
50:32
the mainstream, which is this idea of
50:34
virile, right, this zest for life. And
50:36
that. When you're depleting your dopamine, it's
50:39
cutting out and limiting that virile. And
50:41
when you allow that dopamine to build
50:43
up, it's used to turn into virile
50:45
to have that zest for life and
50:47
that excitement. It's like the rats in
50:49
the experiments where they can hit the
50:51
cocaine lever. You just hit it over
50:54
and over again. That's not satisfaction. That's
50:56
not the meaning of life. It doesn't
50:58
matter. How great the supposed stimulus is
51:00
going to be, if you're doing it
51:02
over and over again, it's going to
51:04
lose its attraction. Yeah, I think a
51:06
powerful way to look at it is
51:09
everything we're talking about here is illustrating
51:11
all these different ways that you can
51:13
engage dopamine. It could be physical, it
51:15
could be mental, it could be consuming.
51:17
So I think it's more about adding
51:19
variety to the ways in which dopamine
51:21
is interacting with your brain than just
51:24
relying on the one. way that seems
51:26
to work for you every single day
51:28
to lose its impact. You can't keep
51:30
going back to the well over and
51:32
over again. And they're just, you know,
51:34
the world is a complex place. There's
51:36
a lot to discover about it and
51:39
it can be very satisfying and there's
51:41
not one path or one way you
51:43
should explore, but the more you open
51:45
yourself up to put yourself in situations
51:47
where you don't know the outcome. You
51:49
know, it's another example of a metaphor
51:51
for this is like sports. The point
51:54
of sports is not to know what
51:56
the final score was. If sports was
51:58
nothing but box scores, it would not
52:00
be that. exciting. It's to be in
52:02
that situation where you don't know what's
52:04
going to happen. And if it's,
52:07
you know, if you're cheering for
52:09
one team, it's not that interesting
52:12
even if your team is ahead
52:14
by 50 points, right? Like, it's
52:16
engaging only to the extent that
52:19
you don't know how it's going
52:21
to end. And that's, you kind
52:23
of want that in all your
52:26
activities. I'd love to share those
52:28
with the audience. Let's put the
52:30
air quotes around rules, let's say principles,
52:32
even though I call them rules in
52:34
the book. The first one I think,
52:36
or the first one was explored then
52:39
exploit. And that's going back to this
52:41
idea that there are times when you
52:43
should be exploring and gathering wool, you
52:45
know, looking around and there are times
52:47
when you should be exploiting. There are
52:49
times when you need to knuckle down,
52:51
get up early, and do the work.
52:54
And there's some really interesting research, I
52:56
won't go through the whole spiel on
52:58
it, but there's some really interesting research
53:00
saying that best combination, the best way
53:02
to actually be productive and accomplish things
53:04
is to have a period of exploration
53:06
followed by a period of exploitation. And
53:09
this is logical, right? Like the exploration
53:11
is a time when you discover, you
53:13
cast your net as widely as possible
53:15
to figure out what you want to
53:17
focus on. And then you have to
53:19
eventually stop wandering around, pick.
53:21
pick a topic, pick a goal,
53:23
pick an idea, and work hard
53:26
on it. So explore then exploit
53:28
is rule number one. The second
53:30
one is to look for the
53:32
uncertainty sweet spot. So I've been
53:34
saying, isn't it wonderful when
53:37
you don't know what's going to
53:39
happen? And you should seek
53:41
out opportunities where there's a
53:43
lot of uncertainty. That approach
53:45
has its limits. So it's like. If you were
53:47
to say, hey Alex, why don't you read this
53:49
dictionary in Norwegian? It's going to be, you're not
53:51
going to know anything about what's going on. Isn't
53:53
that going to be exciting? And I'm going to
53:55
say, no, it's not going to be exciting because
53:58
I have no freaking idea what's going on. So
54:00
there's a, if we go back to
54:02
this idea of wanting to learn about
54:04
the world, there's a sweet spot of
54:06
uncertainty that maximizes your ability to learn
54:09
about the world. If there's no uncertainty,
54:11
if life is very simple, if everything's
54:13
simple, you're not going to learn anything.
54:15
But if it's super complicated, if it's
54:17
the, you know, the dictionary in Norwegian,
54:20
you're also not going to learn anything
54:22
because you can't make head or tail
54:24
of it. So you're looking for intermediate
54:26
uncertainty. But you kind of have a
54:29
sense of the patterns and the possibilities.
54:31
And, you know, this is kind of
54:33
actually a segue to rule number three.
54:35
Rule number three is play more. And
54:38
the reason I say play more is
54:40
that the sense of play, the sense
54:42
of play, the sense of fun, the
54:44
sense that you want to do something
54:46
because it's interesting. That is probably the
54:49
most reliable guide we have to this
54:51
uncertainty sweet spot. So how do you
54:53
find this intermediate zone of uncertainty? You
54:55
listen to that internal voice that says
54:58
this is interesting. this is worth pursuing.
55:00
I'm curious about this because you're not
55:02
going to be curious about something that's
55:04
too complicated and you're not going to
55:06
be curious about something that's too easy.
55:09
So it's easy to say play more
55:11
and you know for our kids we
55:13
can say all right I'm gonna you
55:15
guys can play all afternoon but for
55:18
adults what play more means you can't
55:20
just force yourself to play you don't
55:22
like just buy a model train set
55:24
and sit on the floor and hope
55:26
that that works but you have to
55:29
give yourself time and space away from
55:31
the responsibilities that we all have, whether
55:33
it's with family or career or whatever,
55:35
give yourself some room consciously to just
55:38
sort of do something because it's fun.
55:40
The fourth rule is minimize regret. So
55:42
regret in this context is actually a
55:44
mathematical concept. The difference between the outcome
55:47
you got and the best outcome you
55:49
could have gotten if you'd made all
55:51
the right decisions. So you can think
55:53
about this in gambling terms. This is
55:55
one of the ways they study exploration
55:58
decision making is you're. choosing different slot
56:00
machines or whatever. And if you magically
56:02
knew always which bet was the right
56:04
one to make, you would make a
56:07
certain amount of money. And regret is,
56:09
you know, with the choices you made
56:11
in real life, not being clairvoyant, and
56:13
you're always going to be clairvoyant. And
56:15
you're always going to have regret. As
56:18
you go through life, you're going to
56:20
make decisions, and sometimes you're going to
56:22
be like, man, I kind of wish
56:24
I'd made that other decision. One of
56:27
the best ways to minimize regret is
56:29
to embrace what what scientists actually call
56:31
optimism in the face of uncertainty, which
56:33
is to be To look for the
56:35
best realistic upside. So Whenever we're facing
56:38
choices, we kind of have a range
56:40
of like, yeah, is this one I
56:42
might let's say I have a 40%
56:44
somewhere between a 40 to 60% chance
56:47
of getting a good outcome. And this
56:49
other one I have, let's say a
56:51
20% to 70% chance of a good
56:53
outcome. We don't, I know we don't
56:56
think about this, but just for the
56:58
point of illustration, there's these kind of
57:00
error bars where we don't know how
57:02
things are going to turn out. And
57:04
what this, what the math of optimal
57:07
decision-making tells us is that you should
57:09
take the one that has the best,
57:11
that the top of their error bar,
57:13
that the one that has a chance
57:16
of turning out well. So this is
57:18
a little abstract. So let me say
57:20
you're. considering two different job opportunities. One
57:22
is pretty stable, decent starting salary, not
57:24
a lot of opportunity for advancement, but
57:27
it's a good solid job and it'll
57:29
pay the bills. The other is maybe
57:31
less stable, less starting salary, but there's
57:33
a plausible path of advancement that will
57:36
lead you to the job you actually
57:38
really want, your dream job. And what
57:40
optimism in the face of uncertainty means
57:42
is take the second job, take the
57:44
one that gives you a chance of
57:47
you know, not panning out because that'll
57:49
minimize your regret. You'll all... wonder whether
57:51
you could have made it if you
57:53
take the safe but job that doesn't
57:56
have that upside. Sorry that's a little
57:58
bit of a complicated one to explain.
58:00
It's super helpful and I know many
58:02
in our audience have had that exact
58:05
choice between jobs and might be sitting
58:07
with it right now. It's worth thinking
58:09
about and just pursuing and the last
58:11
the last rule is embrace the struggle
58:13
which is really going back to this
58:16
effort paradox we were talking about before
58:18
that understand that when things are hard
58:20
let's say you take the the uncertain
58:22
job and it you know it doesn't
58:25
pan out in your back on the
58:27
you know on the sun for job
58:29
when things are hard that's part of
58:31
what gives the pursuit meaning so so
58:33
and I think that that mental framing
58:36
is important because the the the feeling
58:38
we get or the meeting we ascribe
58:40
to our experiences depends a lot on
58:42
the framing we give to it. And
58:45
so if you're telling yourself, this is
58:47
hard, it sucks, that's kind of, those
58:49
words help create your reality. Whereas if
58:51
you're telling yourself, this is hard, this
58:53
is good, you know, like, this is
58:56
the struggle I want to be on,
58:58
that makes it a much more positive
59:00
experience and helps maybe motivate you to
59:02
keep pushing. Well, thank you for stopping
59:05
by to motivate our audience and share
59:07
these great insights with them. Where can
59:09
they find out more about you in
59:11
the book? First of all, thanks so
59:14
much for having me. This has been
59:16
a fun conversation that has pushed me
59:18
in some interesting directions. I have a
59:20
website that is Alex hutchinson.net and that
59:22
has links to basically social media and
59:25
things that I write things that I
59:27
write things that I write as a
59:29
journalist. I write a lot for I
59:31
write once a week for outside magazines,
59:34
so I have columns there too. But
59:36
yeah, Alex Hutchinson. We'll get you all
59:38
your Alex Hutchinson needs. What
59:40
a great interview. And now comes
59:43
to part of the show, where
59:45
we showcase one of our X-Factor
59:47
Accelerator members. Take it away, Boris.
59:50
My name is Boris. I've seen...
59:52
software engineer. I was born in
59:54
Europe and moved to the United
59:57
States 10 years ago. There was
59:59
a time when I was lacking
1:00:01
sense of community and belonging. I
1:00:04
chose the art of charm program
1:00:06
because I wanted to advance my
1:00:08
career and build a personal circle
1:00:11
of people with similar values. The
1:00:13
program made me view people around
1:00:15
me as doors to new opportunities.
1:00:18
That person at a conference can
1:00:20
refer you to your next job.
1:00:22
That stranger at the coffee shop
1:00:25
is a gateway to your new
1:00:27
hobby. That passenger on a train
1:00:29
is a significant other if you
1:00:32
make the move. I recommend to
1:00:34
take a step and sign up
1:00:36
for the Art of Charm program.
1:00:39
I will see you as the
1:00:41
AUC family reunion. Thank you, Boris.
1:00:43
It was a pleasure and an
1:00:45
honor working with you too, and
1:00:48
good luck to all your future
1:00:50
endeavors. If you've gotten value out
1:00:52
of this, or any of our
1:00:55
podcasts, head on over to your
1:00:57
favorite podcast player. and review the
1:00:59
show, it will mean the world
1:01:02
to us and it helps others
1:01:04
find the show. All right, before
1:01:06
we head out, a huge thank
1:01:09
you to our producers Michael Harold
1:01:11
and Eric Montgomery. Until next week,
1:01:13
go out there and make it
1:01:16
epic. You
1:01:26
saw the
Podchaser is the ultimate destination for podcast data, search, and discovery. Learn More