Evolving flu, and the desert decomposition conundrum

Evolving flu, and the desert decomposition conundrum

Released Friday, 20th December 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Evolving flu, and the desert decomposition conundrum

Evolving flu, and the desert decomposition conundrum

Evolving flu, and the desert decomposition conundrum

Evolving flu, and the desert decomposition conundrum

Friday, 20th December 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

The eLife podcast from

0:02

eLife, the open access journal

0:04

for outstanding Research in

0:06

the Life and

0:08

Biomedical Sciences online at eLife

0:10

Sciences.org. .org. Hello

0:13

This is Merry Christmas, 96

0:15

this is episode 96 of

0:17

Chris Smith from the Chris Smith from

0:19

The Naked In this In this

0:21

episode, the desert decomposition conundrum,

0:23

how things can break down

0:25

so quickly. so quickly in the

0:28

dry. What can reveal about a

0:30

gene linked to autism and

0:32

how mice decode the smell

0:34

of first, every year influenza Every

0:37

year influenza circulates globally,

0:39

evolving and adapting as it

0:41

goes most its arrival in

0:43

most given geographies usually

0:45

coincides seasonally with winter this steady

0:47

Now this changes the changes the

0:49

face that the virus presents to

0:51

our immune systems and it's enables it

0:53

to stay one step ahead of

0:55

us ahead keep on coming back. coming

0:57

back. So for a a vaccine strategy to

0:59

work, scientists have to try to

1:01

anticipate sufficiently far ahead far forms

1:03

of the virus are going to

1:06

emerge in any forthcoming season to

1:08

give healthcare systems enough time to

1:10

mass enough then administer an appropriate vaccine

1:12

cocktail that can protect a population.

1:14

cocktail that can get it right, a about

1:16

60 We of the time. 60% of the

1:18

time. is that number not number not 100%? Where

1:20

are the are the shortcomings in the

1:22

system and what's it going to take

1:24

to push up up, we can can? our

1:26

hit rate. This is what is what Richard

1:29

Near at the University of he where

1:31

he works on predictive models of flu,

1:33

has been wondering He's He's hoping

1:35

to shed some light on where the

1:37

weaknesses are and how how surmountable they may

1:39

or may not may not

1:41

be. influenza viruses adapt very rapidly

1:43

to human immunity, meaning they mutate

1:46

such that antibodies don't recognise the

1:48

virus as well anymore as they

1:50

used to, and these variants that...

1:53

have these immunoscape mutations, these adaptive

1:55

mutations that tend to increase in

1:57

frequency, and our primary means

1:59

of predict... future population would be that you

2:01

look at the ones that grow in

2:03

frequency and extrapolate frequency grows

2:06

further into the future. the

2:08

it turned out that many things

2:10

that grow at some point

2:12

stop growing point stop just and about.

2:14

meander about of curious curious is

2:16

what triggers this work. work. Is

2:18

that that because it grows and

2:20

turns itself into a a disaster disaster?

2:22

is it because it grows grows? Flourishes,

2:24

but then we catch up our immune

2:27

response catches up and heads it

2:29

off. So then is kind of stuck yeah

2:31

we believe it we believe it

2:33

is so as these viruses these viruses grow

2:35

in frequency and circulate. gets Whoever

2:38

gets infected by them generates

2:40

immunity against them, so in a

2:42

a way are using up the

2:44

susceptible populations that allows them to

2:46

circulate and thereby their gross ends

2:48

up being diminished as their frequency

2:50

increases. Why do they

2:52

not though not though just stay one step ahead

2:55

of us continuously and just keep

2:57

on changing? keep on that they

2:59

can continue to grow continue always

3:01

playing catch we're always playing

3:03

some extent some do. they

3:05

do but... Sometimes you have a mutation that a

3:07

particular the virus with a particular advantage. really

3:09

go further, but But from there, it's stuck

3:11

in a place where to can't really go

3:13

further, but some other viral lineages that

3:15

also circulate, way in which they tend to

3:18

have gone a different and while they have a space

3:20

then they're offers in more future opportunities to

3:22

further adapt. then overtakes that overtakes and of a way

3:24

in which viruses can back themselves overtakes,

3:26

a corner then overtakes, then overtakes, they have a

3:28

transient advantage, then of stuck in a

3:30

place overtakes, and overtakes, and overtakes, ones that were initially

3:32

not as successful overtakes, and them. overtakes, and

3:34

overtakes, and overt There are obviously lots of different

3:36

families and types of virus. Many of

3:38

them share some characteristics with flu, but they

3:40

are different from the flu. We've just

3:43

been through a pandemic caused by a different

3:45

group of viruses, the group of but they

3:47

seem to show a very similar pattern to

3:49

the one you're describing, where we saw

3:51

jumps of the virus to adopt a new

3:53

configuration. It thrived in that form for

3:55

a bit, and then a new one came

3:57

along, came and then it got as far

3:59

as as far as the... micron variant, and it seems

4:01

to have stalled there. And we're getting

4:03

lots of offshoots of that, but we haven't

4:05

seen these dramatic shifts again. So is

4:07

COVID showing a very similar pattern to the

4:09

mechanism you think is at play with

4:11

flu? Yeah,

4:13

COVID is a very, very interesting

4:15

example of, you know, the main difference

4:17

with COVID is none of us

4:19

had seen COVID five years ago. Now

4:22

we have all gone through multiple

4:24

exposures, multiple times being vaccinated

4:26

or infected, while flu has been

4:28

around as long as we've

4:30

been living and people that have

4:32

been born in different decades

4:34

have been exposed to different flu

4:36

histories in a way. They've

4:38

seen different flu viruses throughout their

4:40

life and thereby have more

4:42

diversity in their immunological makeup. We

4:45

believe that these differences in this

4:47

immunological makeup affect the virus

4:49

host core evolution. And that for

4:51

if you have a very

4:53

heterogeneous immunological makeup, there's lots of

4:55

sort of different niches that

4:57

some viruses can exploit and others

5:00

can't. While in the more

5:02

homogeneous case with COVID, it's more

5:04

likely that you have one

5:06

variant that escapes a larger fraction

5:08

of the population and thereby

5:10

has a gross advantage that carries

5:12

it through the entire population

5:14

And it ends up wiping out all

5:16

other variants. With things like the

5:19

flu then, if you can now work

5:21

out mechanistically what is going on,

5:23

does this mean we are in a

5:25

strong position to make predictions about

5:27

the future and therefore make better judgments

5:29

about the sorts of vaccines that

5:31

we're compiling to try and head off

5:33

what the virus is going to

5:35

do as it goes through the population?

5:38

I certainly hope so. I think

5:40

the main insight from this work

5:42

is actually exploring the limits

5:44

of predictability. What aspects of a

5:46

future population are predictable, which

5:48

ones aren't, and how much phase

5:50

should be put into this

5:52

kind of predictions versus another kind

5:54

of prediction. So this -evolutionary dynamics

5:56

that we see inherently limits

5:58

the prediction horizon. horizon if you wish.

6:01

the amount of time we can

6:03

run these predictions forward is limited by

6:05

the sort of intricate interaction between

6:07

hosts and virus. How far ahead do

6:09

you think we can look then?

6:11

Because obviously people are lots of other

6:13

laboratories and rival groups to your

6:15

own, they're spending a fortune in trying

6:17

to answer this very question. They're

6:19

bringing AI in to look at past

6:21

flu behavior, future flu behavior and

6:23

so on. Are you saying then that

6:25

in fact we're going to have

6:27

to narrow our horizon? Well,

6:29

there is certainly a lot of

6:32

And the molecular features that

6:34

we've discovered over the last

6:36

decades, so mutations in particular

6:38

places, changes in the viral

6:40

protein, these are features that

6:43

often make a virus successful. And

6:46

I think these predictions are going to get better and better. This

6:48

is absolutely crucial for this

6:50

vaccine optimization vaccine selection process. And

6:53

then there is sort of

6:55

another. question as to how you know

6:57

how confidently can we predict the

6:59

relative proportions of different variants, three

7:01

months out, six months out, 12

7:03

months out. And that sort of

7:05

where the main insight from our

7:07

work here is that prediction of

7:09

these explicit measures of population makeup

7:12

of the future, these are going

7:14

to be much, much harder. Currently,

7:16

it's sort of one season that

7:18

over which these predictions can be

7:20

meaningfully made, but making now sort

7:22

of actually frequency predictions for the

7:24

next winter season the 20, 25,

7:26

26 winter that currently I don't

7:28

think is in reach and sort

7:30

of our paper here highlights some

7:32

sort of intrinsic limitations that predictions

7:35

of these kind face. If we

7:37

know what the limitations are though.

7:39

We understand something about the mechanism of those

7:42

limitations, which means we might be in a

7:44

position to do something about it. So does

7:46

your paper also shed some light on where

7:48

we need to direct our efforts to try

7:50

to become better at doing this in the

7:52

future? Yes. So,

7:54

you know, my take home from

7:57

this work is that to

7:59

actually those predictions better we

8:01

need need a much more. a

8:04

fine of understanding of what

8:06

of systems, of what part

8:08

of the population recognize which

8:11

well. how well. would want to

8:13

be able to measure measure

8:15

of immunological of immunological makeup for

8:17

children and teenagers and adults

8:19

and all the adults older adults

8:21

elderly. and the in different parts

8:23

of the world, so that we we

8:25

we really kind of understand if there's

8:27

a new virus coming in sort

8:29

of. in, sort of is susceptible

8:31

to this new virus and

8:34

who is protected from this

8:36

virus that will allow us us,

8:38

A, to understand the future the future

8:40

frequency trajectory of the virus

8:42

for some time. time, also might

8:44

help us to identify variants that

8:47

are of particular concern because

8:49

the vulnerable are are less well

8:51

protected than they should be. be.

8:53

So, a little little way to go, but we

8:55

do at least now have a hand on

8:57

most of the gaps are are we need to

8:59

address. to address. Richard, near there. there.

9:02

Planet Earth is a giant nutrient

9:04

a giant nutrient Energy

9:06

machine. sun captured from the sun

9:08

captured by photosynthesis of a food

9:10

chain of consumers which, when

9:12

they die, army are recycled by

9:14

an army of decomposers that release

9:16

the nutrients and embodied energy

9:18

back into the environment for for reuse.

9:20

And hitherto, much of

9:22

the attention around these decomposers

9:24

has quite appropriately focused

9:26

on microbes focused on and fungi.

9:28

bacteria thrive in warm, wet

9:30

environments. So, unsurprisingly, the wetter

9:32

it is, the faster things

9:34

tend to be broken

9:36

down. But there's a a catch. in

9:39

extremely dry places where the

9:41

normal assemblage of microbial recyclers ought

9:44

to struggle. decomposition

9:46

nevertheless at the same expected rate,

9:48

rate. So something must be stepping

9:50

in to fill that gap, but

9:52

no one knew what. Now, Nevosagi,

9:55

working originally at the Hebrew at the

9:57

Hebrew in in Jerusalem and

9:59

now the... University of Texas

10:01

at Austin thinks he may

10:03

have solved what has

10:05

been dubbed the desert Decomposition

10:08

Conundrum. This is a

10:10

question that exists for about

10:12

50 years already. What

10:14

we know is that decomposition

10:17

or the breakdown of

10:19

plant litter is determined by

10:21

the activity of microorganisms

10:23

such as bacteria or fungi

10:26

and they are very

10:28

much moisture dependent. So

10:30

expect that the composition

10:32

should increase with moisture

10:34

availability. But the problem

10:36

is that in dry

10:39

lands, which is like

10:41

the dry part of

10:43

the world, it is

10:45

more than 40 %

10:47

of the global land

10:49

area, this model doesn't

10:51

work. The composition doesn't

10:53

increase with annual precipitation. So

10:56

if I drew a graph of

10:58

the rate of decomposition on the

11:00

y -axis and the amount of

11:03

moisture in the soil on the

11:05

x -axis, what you're saying is we

11:07

should see an upward sloping line

11:09

as it gets wetter. The decomposition

11:11

should go faster, but what we

11:13

actually see is almost a straight

11:16

line between the dry and the

11:18

wet. So it doesn't follow the

11:20

prediction of wetter equals faster breakdown.

11:23

Yeah, I would just comment

11:25

here that this is

11:27

the case in dry

11:29

So basically most of

11:32

the research is done

11:34

in the most wetter

11:36

ecosystems, like temperate ecosystems,

11:38

but drylands which are

11:40

less studied just show

11:42

very different trends. So

11:44

something is changing. When

11:46

it's dry, something must

11:48

be stepping in to

11:50

compensate to keep up

11:52

the rate of decomposition.

11:54

because the normal mechanism is not doing

11:56

what it would normally do. There has to

11:58

be something filling the gap. Exactly,

12:01

and this is what was

12:03

termed the dryland decomposition conundrum.

12:05

And it stimulated much research

12:07

that basically what people asked

12:09

is what is different between

12:12

drylands and these other water

12:14

ecosystems. And there are several

12:16

mechanisms that were tested, but

12:18

all of them were mostly...

12:20

focused on the role of

12:23

microbes or the other things

12:25

that facilitate the activity of

12:27

microbes. And what we offer

12:29

here is a different hypothesis.

12:31

Basically, some other organisms, larger

12:34

organisms, are more adapted or

12:36

activity under very dry conditions.

12:38

And maybe they... kind of

12:40

compensate for the lack of

12:43

microbial decompositions and this is

12:45

why we see same decomposition

12:47

rate across different precipitation levels.

12:49

How did you test that

12:51

hypothesis then? What we did

12:54

is a multi-site decomposition experiment.

12:56

We went to seven different

12:58

sites across a grandiant of

13:00

precipitation. We did this litter

13:02

box experiment. We have this

13:05

box or cage where we

13:07

keep plant litter and we

13:09

allow different sizes of organism

13:11

to access this litter. So

13:13

we have three different treatments.

13:16

One was just allowing microorganisms

13:18

to access the litter, the

13:20

other allowed also misofauna, and

13:22

the third treatment allowed the

13:24

whole decomposer community, which also

13:27

includes the macrofauna, which we

13:29

believe has the more important...

13:31

in drier

13:33

sites. Right so

13:36

basically what

13:38

what you're able

13:40

to say

13:42

is well if

13:44

we allow

13:47

different categories of

13:49

decomposer in

13:51

and they vary

13:53

by size

13:55

if they are

13:58

filling the

14:00

gap in the

14:02

dry place

14:04

and we exclude

14:06

them we

14:09

should see that

14:11

desert decomposition

14:13

gap come back

14:15

if we

14:17

exclude the big

14:20

guys and

14:22

they're responsible. exactly

14:24

exactly. you just right

14:27

to look if

14:29

the mechanism we suggest

14:31

is right. We

14:33

also use default just

14:35

to capture ground -active

14:37

insects, and we

14:39

use this to really

14:41

characterize the macro

14:43

decomposer community or assemblage

14:45

in these different

14:47

sites. And we repeated

14:49

this experiment in

14:51

the winter, which is

14:53

cool and wet, then in

14:55

the summer which is a and

14:57

very dry. What did

15:00

it show? Have you got to

15:02

the bottom of the desert decomposition

15:04

conundrum? Do we think what you

15:06

have done shows that it is

15:08

the bigger consumers that move in

15:10

and fill the gap yes so what

15:12

we have found is very beautiful

15:14

because in the winter, we saw

15:16

that microbes dominated

15:18

decomposition in general

15:20

and that decomposition

15:22

increased with precipitation

15:24

but in the

15:26

summer what we saw

15:28

is that macro decomposers

15:30

were the dominant decomposers

15:32

and they decomposition in

15:34

general picked in the

15:36

we call it the

15:38

arid sites and when

15:40

you look at the

15:42

two seasons combined and

15:44

the total decomposition you

15:46

really see comparable decomposition

15:48

rates across the gradient. one

15:51

of the arid sites

15:54

decomposition was higher than

15:56

the wetter sites Well,

15:59

congrats Congratulations on solving

16:01

a 50-year-old mystery and apart

16:04

from being extremely satisfying academically,

16:06

why does this matter? Drylands

16:08

cover more than 40% of

16:10

the global land area. So

16:12

this is kind of a

16:15

big deal. It means that

16:17

we should look at different

16:19

sizes of decomposers and treat

16:21

them separately when we do,

16:23

for example, carbon cycle models,

16:25

when we want to understand

16:28

how trajectories of carbon cycling

16:30

and then... of ecosystem in

16:32

the world in general are

16:34

going to change in the

16:36

future because we do see

16:39

the certification. We see this

16:41

trend and therefore I think

16:43

this is very crucial. Novosagi

16:45

there. This is the e-life

16:47

podcast from the Naked Scientists.

16:50

I'm Chris Smith. In a

16:52

moment how mice detect and

16:54

respond to the smell of

16:56

cats. But first, a gene

16:58

called P10 has been linked

17:01

to a number of neurodevelopmental

17:03

and behavioral disorders, including autism.

17:05

In some cases, it seems

17:07

to affect the relative proportion

17:09

of inhibitory and excitatory nerve

17:12

cell populations. How exactly changes

17:14

to P10 cause these presentations

17:16

and whether we can use

17:18

this knowledge to ameliorate them,

17:20

we don't know. But now,

17:22

working with C. elegans worms,

17:25

which also use this same

17:27

gene in their nervous systems,

17:29

Argentina's UNS researcher Diego Rashes

17:31

has made an intriguing observation.

17:33

Mutating the worm equivalent to

17:36

P10 produces an imbalance of

17:38

inhibitory and excitatory neural numbers.

17:40

But giving the animals the

17:42

substance beta-hydroxy butyrate, which is

17:44

a chemical known as a

17:47

ketone body and produced naturally

17:49

by our own metabolisms when

17:51

we burn fat, can reverse

17:53

the effect of the mutation.

17:55

In recent years there is

17:58

several reports that link mutations

18:00

in a specific she... called

18:02

peten with neurodevelopmental defects,

18:04

instance instance autism. This gene is

18:06

is present in only in

18:08

humans but throughout the animal

18:11

even in very simple multicellular

18:14

animals like worms. In

18:16

autism there is

18:18

an imbalance between excitation

18:20

and inhibition. So we wanted

18:22

to to see what is

18:25

the relationship between this this

18:27

gene. and the imbalance between

18:29

excitatory and and in theory. So given that

18:31

given that you've said that this

18:33

gene is present throughout the

18:35

animal kingdom, even in worms. in Does

18:38

that mean then you could study a worm? study

18:40

a look at some of the same

18:42

changes of the that are in the

18:44

genes in humans with things like autism.

18:46

Put those changes into a worm. into

18:49

a worm and the worms nervous

18:51

system change as well? well?

18:53

Yes, that was the idea. Given

18:55

that the activity of is is

18:58

very well and it has it has more

19:00

or less the same function. throughout

19:02

the animal kingdom. I mean, what

19:04

we can manipulate very

19:07

easily in the laboratory,

19:09

we we wanted to

19:11

study these relationships between P10 and and

19:13

excitatory and inhibitory

19:16

neurons. tried to to understand what happened.

19:18

And when you do you do this,

19:20

if you look at the manipulations

19:22

of that gene, it is

19:24

it reflected in changes in the

19:26

balance of excitatory and inhibitory neurons

19:28

in the the worm it is in

19:30

the human brain? human what

19:32

we observe is that is

19:34

that animals that not working. working

19:36

has specific impairment in the

19:39

development and in the and

19:41

in the morphology of the GABA -L6

19:43

The the GABA -L6 units are the

19:45

inhibitory ones. ones. while the the

19:47

excitatory neurons are quite normal,

19:49

they are not affected. So So

19:51

is too much excitation in in this.

19:53

And is there any way to is there any

19:55

way to revert that? Is there any

19:57

intervention we've got got to have an an

19:59

on if we we to to

20:01

apply this knowledge.

20:03

neurodevelopmental disorders, is there anything

20:05

we could potentially do to

20:08

rebalance things? Yes, the first the

20:10

first achievement of this work is

20:12

that have now a kind of kind of where

20:14

to study the study. between

20:16

between this imbalance between existential

20:19

innovation and can

20:21

try. now different interracial to

20:23

try to ameliorate that. We

20:25

try one of these interventions when

20:27

we expose the animal to our ketone

20:30

body. Ketone body is a

20:32

metabolite that we produce when

20:34

we are And we found

20:36

And we found that the of this ketone

20:38

body. are majorized, the specific effects

20:40

on the bary signals are majorizing this this

20:43

imbalance the animals are more normal,

20:45

even though they have mutation in potent.

20:47

Just to to clarify then, are are

20:49

you saying that when you

20:51

feed or expose the animals

20:53

to this ketone body that

20:56

we naturally make when we're

20:58

starving? when we're starving? it resets

21:00

the balance of activation and

21:02

inhibition in the nervous system?

21:04

Is that because it changes

21:06

the it populations or does it

21:09

just change just change nerve And does

21:11

it only do that when the ketone

21:13

body is there, or does it change

21:15

it is there? Or does it change it we

21:17

found that there is

21:20

a specific time window when the

21:22

the body is efficient. is

21:24

important It is important that the

21:26

is is present when the neurons

21:29

are being developed. If we just

21:31

expose the animal, when they are

21:33

adults, who keto body, the

21:35

nothing. body do what we

21:37

found is found body acts

21:39

when the neurons are

21:41

developing developing and somehow ketone body

21:43

acts very early in development.

21:47

preventing the developmental defects that

21:49

we do see in

21:51

pit music. Do you have any Do

21:53

you have any insights into how

21:55

it might be doing that? it's quite a

21:57

quite a bizarre observation that this

21:59

naturally present... chemical, could shift brain development

22:01

so dramatically? Is it signalling in

22:03

some way? it triggering some other

22:05

genes to get switched on to

22:08

compensate in these animals? Do you

22:10

know how it might be doing

22:12

this? In mutations

22:14

in p10 There is

22:16

an inhibition of a transcription

22:18

factor. So there are a lot

22:20

of genes that are not

22:22

being expressed. and this probably arise for

22:25

the defects that we do see

22:27

in p10 mutants. the exposure

22:29

of this animal to the

22:31

ketone body enhance the activity. of

22:34

these transcription factors. Therefore,

22:36

we think that rescue the

22:38

expression of some important

22:40

scenes for the

22:42

inhibitory neuron development. I

22:46

know this is a very

22:48

speculative question. and you haven't

22:50

looked at this per se, and

22:52

certainly not in anything bigger

22:54

than a worm. But there will

22:56

be circumstances where a pregnant

22:58

animal, including a pregnant human, may

23:01

well be exposed to chemicals

23:03

like ketone bodies. Women will be

23:05

starving at certain points. They

23:07

may have really serious morning sickness,

23:09

so they just can't eat,

23:11

for example, and that could do

23:13

this. Do you think, then,

23:15

that this is happening during development

23:17

in, say, mammals and could

23:19

be affecting the development of the

23:22

nervous system? for

23:24

sure, during pregnancy, there is

23:26

an increased levels of ketone body,

23:28

even in mothers that are

23:30

eating normally, right? So maybe endogenous

23:33

ketone bodies are playing a

23:35

role in neurodevelopment, and maybe enhancing

23:37

this effect of the ketone

23:39

body in neurodevelopment, let's put it

23:41

this way, it would be

23:43

good to evaluate. in mammals is

23:45

increasing the levels of ketone body

23:47

to see if there

23:49

is an improvement in the

23:52

neurodevelopment of p10 mutants in mice,

23:54

for instance. It's an

23:56

intriguing finding, isn't it? Diego Rashes there.

24:00

Mice are are genuinely fearful

24:02

of cats encounter they encounter

24:04

the aroma of a feline

24:06

they freeze scarper. This happens

24:08

thanks to an happens thanks to

24:10

an accessory smell system called

24:12

the or VMO. organ roles is to detect

24:15

One of its roles its

24:17

to detect pheromones and its

24:19

wiring into the limbic system

24:21

enables it to coordinate mating behaviour,

24:23

but but it can also,

24:25

as it turns out, detect

24:27

predator smells, including cats, and

24:29

projections to the brain's hypothalamus

24:32

can then initiate defensive responses. responses.

24:34

Interestingly, the the extent

24:36

of these responses isn't binary.

24:38

They're they're proportional to the intensity of

24:40

the stimulus. Suchiko Haga-Yamanaka from

24:42

the University of California California

24:44

used used cat tears, which

24:47

contain odourance the the mouse is

24:49

sensitive to to, to unpick

24:51

the neurological pathways responsible.

24:54

What we are

24:56

looking at at is

24:58

interspecies communications. The organ or

25:01

VNO can detect B&O

25:03

from predator species in

25:05

prey animals. predator species in

25:07

looking at So what we

25:09

are looking at is can

25:12

control can behavior. the

25:14

behavior of the prey animals.

25:17

is the vomeronase logon located? located?

25:20

the marrow nasal organ is

25:22

situated within the nasal

25:24

cavity. between the nasal

25:26

cavity and oral cavity. Is it

25:29

it, therefore, anatomically distinct from

25:31

the bits of the... olfactory

25:34

system smell other smells? For

25:36

smells. if I For instance, if nose my nose

25:38

in a bunch of flowers, I were

25:40

a mouse and I were sniffing flowers,

25:42

I would I would smell the with with

25:44

one part of my system, but I

25:46

would smell other things with this foam

25:48

or a nasal organ. It's a separate

25:50

entity from the rest of the olfactory

25:52

system. old factory exactly. exactly. The

25:54

organ is organ organ

25:57

of the of the accessory

25:59

old factory. system And then

26:01

the main smell is detected

26:03

through the main olfactory

26:05

system. And how is it wired

26:07

up? What's it connected to? and

26:10

what supplies the innovation to carry

26:12

signals from it and to where.

26:14

The vomeronasal organ contains

26:16

sensory neurons and

26:18

then the axons of

26:20

the sensory neurons.

26:22

travel to the frontal part

26:24

of the brain called the

26:26

accessory olfactory bulb and then

26:28

from there send the

26:31

signal to the amygdala

26:33

or the hyposalamus

26:35

that controls innate behaviors

26:37

as mating or defensive

26:39

behavior. How did

26:41

you... use that understanding

26:44

then to test whether or

26:46

not it could detect for

26:48

instance the and presence of

26:50

a potential predator. First

26:52

of all, we looked at the

26:54

behavior to the source of

26:56

the creator queue. In our

26:58

case, cat was used as

27:01

a creator queue source and

27:03

then we looked at the

27:05

behavior changes. At the

27:07

same time, we also looked at

27:09

whether the sensory neurons in the

27:11

bone marrow are organ was

27:13

activated or not using a

27:15

specific neural activity marker called

27:18

the C -Pos. Right.

27:20

So in other words, you

27:22

can present that the cat saliva,

27:24

which you, which you hypothesize

27:26

the mouse can smell and be

27:28

alarmed by and you're looking

27:30

to see if when that is

27:32

present, the nerves connect

27:34

between the vomeronasal organ and relevant

27:36

brain target structures whether they

27:38

change their activity, which would argue

27:40

they are sensing the smell

27:42

of the cat saliva. Yes.

27:46

So that tells you they're sensitive to

27:48

it, but how do you probe what

27:50

it does to their behavior? What

27:52

we looked at was freezing

27:54

behavior. The freezing is one type

27:56

of the defensive behavior response. So

27:59

if If I present some of

28:01

the the particular the to

28:03

the mouse, it will it. when it

28:05

smells it. That's what you're saying, isn't

28:07

it? It suddenly lock up. lock up.

28:10

Lock up, yes. Once you establish that

28:12

that scent is detected it's going

28:14

to these different brain areas, how

28:16

did you then try to

28:18

establish how the response is programmed?

28:20

response is at the specific At the

28:23

of the neurons in the

28:25

neurons in the are activated

28:27

by by cat saliva

28:29

exposure. and then the

28:32

activation controls the freezing behaviour

28:34

output. And is it a

28:36

proportional response? So if I had

28:38

a big dollop of dollop of would

28:40

I get a bigger response a is

28:42

it a binary thing? It's all

28:44

or nothing. thing? It's all we Yeah,

28:47

we tested that amount of

28:49

of cat saliva the diluted

28:51

cat survivor. cat saliva.

28:53

The non-diluted cat saliva

28:55

in this robust but

28:58

but... So it does mean that the

29:00

animals can make a proportionate it

29:02

does mean that the animals

29:04

can make a proportionate response

29:07

to to a concentration of the

29:09

potential threat stimulus from the

29:11

environment. from the

29:13

environment? Exactly. What If you

29:15

could learned then? up if you were

29:17

to sort of give us a summary

29:19

of what this study adds or

29:21

how this changes our understanding, what

29:23

do you think you've done to move

29:25

the field forward with this? with this?

29:28

What What we

29:30

found was that to

29:32

contain the time-sensitive component

29:34

that influenced the intensity

29:36

of the defensive behavior

29:39

output, and then the VNO can

29:41

can detect. such

29:43

time-sensitive chemical cues, and

29:45

then the and then

29:47

the intensity of the behavior

29:50

through the VNO mediated neurocircuitry.

29:52

The circuitry. medial

29:54

hyposalamus is involved

29:56

in in this circuitry and

29:59

a regular it's defensive behaviour

30:01

out of it. Such a go hagga yamanaka there.

30:03

Well that's where we're that's to gonna have

30:05

leave it for this episode, and in

30:07

fact, this year. listening been listening

30:09

to podcast which is which is produced

30:12

by Scientists. Previous Previous editions, the the

30:14

references and full transcripts for these

30:16

for well as details on how

30:18

you can subscribe to this

30:20

podcast subscribe to this .com forward slash. forward

30:23

slash eLife. The Naked also publish

30:25

a published a weekly covers the

30:27

latest leading science stories the

30:30

you can find that science stories

30:32

and .com that slash podcast. I'll

30:35

be back in 2025 with another look

30:37

inside with Until then, from me Chris

30:39

Smith, thank you for listening, have a

30:41

wonderful Christmas Smith, a very you new year. Have

30:44

see you on the other side. Goodbye. and a very

30:46

happy new year and I'll see you on the

30:48

other side. Goodbye. The Open Access Journal

30:50

for Research in the

30:52

Life and Biomedical

30:54

Sciences Sciences,

30:56

online at .org

Rate

Join Podchaser to...

  • Rate podcasts and episodes
  • Follow podcasts and creators
  • Create podcast and episode lists
  • & much more

Episode Tags

Do you host or manage this podcast?
Claim and edit this page to your liking.
,

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features