Episode Transcript
Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.
Use Ctrl + F to search
0:00
It's It's better over here.
0:03
AT&T customers, switching to T-Mobile has never
0:05
been easier. We'll pay off your existing
0:07
phone and give you a new one
0:09
free, all on America's
0:12
largest 5G network. Visit t-mobile.com/carrier
0:14
freedom to switch today. Pay
0:17
off up to $650 via virtual prepaid MasterCard in 15
0:19
days. Free phone up to $830 via 24 monthly bill
0:21
credits plus tax qualifying port and trade and service on
0:24
Go 5G next and credit required. Contact us before canceling
0:26
entire account to continue bill credits. Your credit stop and
0:28
bounce and required finance agreement is now available. Hey,
0:30
it's Ryan Seacrest for Albertson's and Safeway! season,
0:32
make sure you set aside
0:35
time for self aside Now through
0:37
December 31st, shop in -store and
0:39
online for participating self -care products
0:41
and get four times points to
0:43
use for discounts on future
0:45
grocery and gas purchases. use Stock
0:47
up on self -care favorites grocery and gas
0:50
purchases, Fusion self-care favorites like Self-Care favorites, and four times
0:52
points to use for discounts on Offer ends
0:54
December 31st. Restrictions apply, promotions
0:56
may vary. Visit Albertsons
0:58
diapers'.com for more detail. vary.
1:01
Visit Albertsons
1:03
or safeway.com
1:05
for more
1:07
you're listening You're listening
1:10
to the Michael
1:12
Shermer Show. As
1:25
I I mentioned often on the show, show,
1:27
I'm constantly engaged in learning. when I'm when
1:29
I'm not recording new episodes or editing articles
1:31
or working on my next book. on my and
1:33
my favorite platform by
1:35
far is is brilliant because it helps
1:38
me build my critical thinking skills
1:40
through problem solving. problem solving. one on
1:42
data visualizing data and how to
1:44
analyze it. it. Anyway, the whole
1:46
idea is that you're solving problems,
1:48
not just memorizing, not just watching lecture
1:50
videos, and I and I love watching
1:52
lecture videos, but but this kind of
1:55
interactive learning is six times more effective.
1:57
than than just listening to somebody
1:59
speak. Plus all the content
2:01
on Brilliant is crafted by award
2:04
-winning team of teachers, researchers and
2:06
professionals at MIT, Caltech, Duke,
2:08
Microsoft, Google and more. To
2:10
try everything Brilliant has to offer
2:12
for free for a full 30
2:14
days, go to brilliant .org/skeptic. That's
2:16
brilliant .org/skeptic. Or you can click on
2:18
the link in the description of
2:20
the show notes for this episode.
2:22
And you'll also get 20 %
2:25
off an annual premium subscription. So
2:27
thanks for supporting the show. Thanks
2:29
for supporting brilliant .org/skeptic. Check it out.
2:31
My guest today is Dr. Amir
2:33
Raz, the world -renowned expert on
2:35
the science of suggestion and suggestibility
2:37
with recent positions as Canada Research
2:39
Chair, Professor of Psychiatry, Neurology and
2:41
Neurosurgery and Psychology at McGill University
2:43
and where I know him from
2:45
Chapman University's founding director of the
2:47
Institute for Interdisciplinary Brain and Behavioral
2:49
Sciences. We were colleagues there. I've
2:51
stopped teaching there a year and
2:54
a half ago. But we're still
2:56
colleagues. I'm not sure how that
2:58
works, but in any case, and
3:00
his son, his highly gifted son
3:02
was in my class. He took
3:04
my skepticism 101 class when he
3:06
was 15 years old and he
3:08
was the smartest kid in the
3:10
class. That was great. Formerly at
3:12
Columbia University and Cornell Medical Center,
3:14
his work has been covered widely
3:16
in the media, New York Times,
3:18
Scientific American Mind and other publications. has
3:20
over 200 peer -reviewed articles in
3:22
publications such as Nature and PNAS
3:25
and NeuroImage. And he won the
3:27
Young Investigator Award and Early Career
3:29
Award from the American Psychological Association.
3:31
He's a speaker in high demand.
3:33
He has a TEDx talk, for
3:35
example, When Can Deception Be Good
3:37
for You? I just watched that,
3:39
by the way. It was quite
3:41
good. And he's been featured in
3:43
documentaries for the BBC, National Geographic
3:45
and the CBC. And here's this
3:47
new book, The Suggestible Brain. The
3:49
Science and Magic of How We
3:51
Make Up Our Minds. Nice to
3:54
see you, how you doing? I'm
3:56
doing great, thank you very much
3:58
for having me on the show.
4:00
I love the book, you know,
4:02
you sent me early chapters, I
4:04
don't know, it must have been
4:06
two, three years ago when you
4:08
were working on it, and. wow, it's
4:10
so much better now, the stories you
4:12
tell, you open up. talking about your experiences
4:14
as a magician, which I love. There's
4:17
been a long time
4:19
connection between skeptics and magicians.
4:22
You know, Penn and Teller, The Amazing
4:24
Randy, and Banachek, and Jamie and
4:26
Swiss, and others that do this
4:28
for, I think a good reason,
4:31
but I'd like your explanation. What
4:33
do you think? scientists and
4:35
skeptics are so fascinated by magicians and
4:37
magic. Well, know, magicians, first
4:39
of all, being a magician really is
4:41
a privilege. And I started out
4:43
as a kid, not really knowing what
4:46
I'm getting myself into. It has
4:48
become, a life journey and very
4:50
meaningful one. I would say that
4:52
my magic actually informed my
4:54
decision to go into science.
4:56
I actually about it a little bit
4:58
in the book how that came about
5:00
but my interest and my research
5:02
into Hypnosis and into looking into altered
5:04
states of consciousness the effects of
5:06
suggestion and so on came primarily germinated,
5:08
marinated in my experiences
5:10
as a young magician.
5:13
interacting with audiences, and so on. Magicians.
5:16
are really clinicians of the public.
5:18
mean, they know how to
5:20
deceive, they know how to create
5:22
illusions, they know how to lie
5:25
with grace and elegance and flair. And
5:28
they do it for people's entertainment pleasure. And
5:31
that's why they're so
5:33
good also at detecting.
5:35
falsehoods and people who pretend that they
5:37
say all kinds of things that are
5:39
not true and so on. And
5:41
magicians are just very good at
5:43
guarding their secrets. They have been
5:45
very good at that. It's a
5:47
very private pottery of people who
5:49
are trying to be extremely careful
5:52
and discreet about what it is
5:54
that they know and the gizmos
5:56
and the props and the gimmicks
5:58
and the different tools that... they use
6:00
and many of and many of
6:02
the psychological methods. magicians use
6:04
are actually use are actually
6:07
not available to it it
6:09
or not I mean most psychologists
6:11
really know too much about
6:13
psychology of of magic. this And
6:16
this creates silos, it creates
6:18
silos of knowledge within the field
6:20
of psychology, within human behavior, behavior,
6:22
it comes to skepticism, when
6:24
it comes to identifying, it comes to
6:27
identifying... you know, certain things that
6:29
are not true or or they're or when it
6:31
comes to when it presentations, when it comes
6:33
to people saying things it comes they want to
6:35
direct you to a particular, uh, to place,
6:37
you to a have a lot of experience with
6:39
that. a So lot of experience with that so hence
6:42
nice. yeah Yeah, had nice yeah on
6:44
the show Swiss on of his books on of
6:46
his books were. magic and about this phenomenon, I've
6:48
long observed it. like to get your thoughts
6:50
on it. I'd like to get your thoughts there's
6:52
something about about that throws
6:54
people off. Also some
6:56
of the close up
6:58
press to digitation like Urgeller spoon bending. Most
7:00
people, especially Most people,
7:02
especially skeptics and scientists, have no
7:05
problem understanding that when makes makes
7:07
the Statue of Liberty disappear. they
7:09
don't think don't think for a moment that
7:11
moving atoms around or anything like that. They like
7:13
it's a magic trick. trick. And they
7:15
they seemed to accept big magic as, well,
7:17
of course, it's just magic. magic. But
7:19
with mentalism, I've heard a lot
7:22
of type and scientist type thinking, yeah, but how but how
7:24
did he know that I was
7:26
gonna pick this card or that he
7:28
knew the name of my grandmother, this
7:30
kind of stuff. Maybe there's there's
7:32
something else think I don't think it's or
7:34
or paranormal, but maybe they're able to
7:36
read my body language or how my
7:38
eyes are moving. Remember, Banacheck has this
7:40
great thing where he has the the trick
7:42
is, and then he has the person
7:45
look at his hand and then he he
7:47
his hand around around he stares at
7:49
their eyes. eyes. It is very And this is
7:51
very clever because people think tell me later. me later, I
7:53
think I must have been tracing out the
7:55
card with my eyes and he could read
7:57
the movement of my eyes. eyes. I'm just,
7:59
this is crap. but this, they always think there's
8:01
something else more complicated than it's just
8:03
a magic. Look, I was always attracted
8:05
to mentalism as a magician. I mean,
8:07
I could vanish handkerchiefs and coins and
8:09
that's fine, but I was really attracted
8:11
to mentalism from a very early age.
8:13
And I think mentalism does afford and
8:15
provide a very interesting trajectory into the
8:17
human mind in the sense that... People
8:19
feel a loss of agency when you
8:21
do a mental trick when you do
8:23
mentalism you begin to tinker Not with
8:26
the statue of liberty as you mentioned
8:28
and not with the stage illusion of
8:30
sorts when something you know when something
8:32
big disappears as impressive as that might
8:34
be You're actually tinkering you're messing with
8:36
their agency you're messing with their authorship.
8:38
They're no longer. Am I the author?
8:40
Is this my free will? Did I
8:42
choose this number or did he make
8:44
me choose this number? Did I choose
8:46
that card or did he lead me
8:48
to choosing that card? And these are
8:50
existential questions. I mean in people's minds,
8:52
they would like to know that they're
8:54
brushing their teeth because they want to
8:56
brush their teeth. They don't want, they're
8:58
not brushing their teeth because somebody's controlling
9:00
them to brush their teeth. The same
9:02
is true by the way in the
9:04
world of management. If you have a
9:06
supervisor or manager and they tell you
9:08
to do something, it's very different than
9:10
if you do it you do it
9:12
because you feel that this is your
9:14
idea because you know this is your
9:16
idea, and it's your prerogative and it's
9:18
your prerogative and it's your initiative and
9:20
it's your initiative and so on. So
9:22
there's something very elementary, very fundamental, very
9:24
rudimentary and very basic foundational about, you
9:26
know, tapping someone's ability to do something
9:28
because you want them to do it
9:30
versus if it comes from them. This
9:32
agency, this sense of free will, is
9:34
something that mentalism taps. Yeah, nice. I'll
9:36
tell you one last funny story about
9:38
this. I was on a scientific American
9:40
cruise where I was one of the
9:42
lecturers and so the dinners were... you
9:44
know, fun with all the people that
9:46
are mostly scientists and skeptics and so
9:48
on. Anyway, so at one of the
9:50
dinners, this guy tells me, look, I'm
9:53
a skeptic too, I don't believe me
9:55
this. And I know Ray Geller is,
9:57
he's kind of cheating when he bends.
9:59
But I saw this guy bend this.
10:01
spoon in a very peculiar way. It's
10:03
like it wasn't like twisted, you know,
10:05
where the bowl is just bent straight
10:07
down, but it was like twisted between
10:09
the handle and the bowl where the
10:11
neck had been twisted all the way
10:13
around. And that's just not, there's no
10:15
way he could just muscle that. And
10:17
as he's telling me this, I take
10:19
the spoon off the table and I
10:21
do the little move that Benacek taught
10:23
me. And then I, when he finished,
10:25
I held it up and said, did
10:27
it look anything like this? Oh, come
10:29
on, really? You know, I'll tell you
10:31
something about that particular move without getting
10:33
into the details, of course, but I
10:35
was once having a similar situation. I
10:37
was having dinner with a famous scientist
10:39
in Europe and he said to me,
10:41
I know you're a magician and I
10:43
know you can do like all these
10:45
things, but you know, bending metal, that's
10:47
a completely different thing. I mean, I've
10:49
seen people bend metal without really touching
10:51
it and I don't understand how they
10:53
do it. By the end of dinner,
10:55
his soup, you know, spoon was bent
10:57
with this particular twist, with this particular
10:59
twist that you just mentioned, which is
11:01
a variation on the technique. And he
11:03
said to me, wait a second, did
11:05
you do it? And I said, well,
11:07
who else is here? I mean, how
11:09
else would it happen? He said, but
11:11
you didn't touch it. I said, that's
11:13
right. I didn't touch it. And it
11:15
happened. So it means that, you know,
11:18
perhaps the soup did it or, you
11:20
know, or maybe the waiter and he
11:22
said, but wait a second, you have
11:24
to explain it to me. I said,
11:26
look, you don't understand. It's an illusion.
11:28
It's an illusion. It's an illusion. It's
11:30
not real. And he said, oh, so
11:32
the spoon is actually straight. It's just
11:34
an illusion. I was like, oh, this
11:36
can get so far, it can get
11:38
out of control so quickly when people
11:40
don't know or don't understand something. They
11:42
just can't wrap their hand around. They
11:44
cannot understand how to think about it.
11:46
They're so frustrated. They're so flustered. They
11:48
don't even know how to start thinking
11:50
about it. So even if you offer
11:52
them something that is patently false, like
11:54
I'm telling this is a visual...
11:56
illusion, although he can
11:58
hold it in
12:00
his hand and palpate
12:02
the distortion in
12:04
the the metal. It's so It's so
12:06
interesting for me to see how even
12:08
intelligent people and sometimes extremely intelligent people
12:10
fall into this trap time and again. this
12:13
trap time sometimes and sometimes do up
12:15
with the most unintelligent explanations for
12:17
it all. Yeah. for it
12:19
all. people have a hard time remembering
12:21
what the magician actually did. if I
12:23
recall I studies on this, they this, they the
12:26
story to make it seem it seem... even
12:28
less likely for you to explain why
12:30
it was a magic trick. He never
12:32
actually touched it. Well, touched it. Well, okay.
12:34
And Randy a funny story about going
12:36
on about show Barbara Walter's show, where already been
12:38
on and bent her and bent she swore
12:40
up and down he never touched
12:42
the key. never then they just played
12:44
the video back. they just played the
12:46
some moment he says, let
12:48
me just check to see if it's
12:50
see if it's course he touches it. of
12:53
And she's like, oh, it. but that's
12:55
not the way it, well, that's not the way,
12:57
well, well, oh. right there in the camera.
12:59
in her mind. her mind, it didn't happen
13:01
that way. it way. Sometimes, and people are not
13:03
trying to be manipulative about it.
13:05
mean, they really, truly, genuinely believe that
13:07
the performer did not touch the
13:09
physical And this is And this is also
13:11
important for them in order order to live
13:14
peacefully with the effect that they
13:16
just witnessed because convince you actually
13:18
convince yourself that this was not touched, it
13:20
makes it all the more miraculous. you then,
13:22
you know, you really need to resort to
13:24
other types of explanations. So I think
13:26
that sometimes people are sort of led into
13:28
it and sometimes they fall into it and
13:30
sometimes it's a combination of of both, but there's
13:33
no question that there's some very
13:35
effective techniques of through of through suggestion. you know
13:37
know forcing people or leading people down the
13:39
garden path of changing their memories and of
13:41
course this is this is well in
13:43
psychology through false memories memories work
13:45
of Elizabeth work of and other people
13:47
who are really showing how showing
13:50
how with narrative with with simple words
13:52
and words choices, you can actually
13:54
completely rewrite memories, including to very
13:56
important events in people's autobiographies. autobiographies.
13:59
Yeah. Another funny story from Ray Hyman
14:01
who blurbed your book about Erie Geller
14:04
that in the early days When
14:06
Erie would be tested by people like
14:08
Ray Hyman and nothing was working
14:10
He would then tell a story like
14:12
well, it's not working tonight But
14:14
last night you should have seen what
14:16
I did and then he repeats
14:18
everything he does And then Ray noticed
14:20
that a lot of the people
14:22
then later said this is what Geller
14:24
did as if they saw what
14:27
he Told them happened the night before
14:29
when they weren't even there Disincredible
14:31
people people like Ray Hyman are very
14:33
special and they're very few and
14:35
far between because He really is a
14:37
researcher scientist psychologist who spent most
14:39
of his life doing, you know research
14:41
in cognitive psychology and You know
14:43
lab work and and so on and
14:45
he understands quite a bit about
14:47
statistics and about the psychological literature on
14:50
the one hand On the other
14:52
hand, he's very well versed in magic
14:54
and in magic moves and in
14:56
magical techniques and and so on this
14:58
Marriage of magician scientist is not
15:00
very common. There are very few people
15:02
who are actually well versed in
15:04
both worlds at a high level It's
15:06
unfortunate, but it's true. Well, that's
15:08
why what makes what you're doing so
15:11
important even even more important and
15:13
entertaining It's really great Yeah, so you
15:15
talk a lot about suggestibility and
15:18
magic So
15:20
let's talk about the evolutionary origin
15:22
of this why why would we be
15:24
okay? First of all, let's define
15:26
what you mean by suggestibility because most
15:28
people think of it as gullibility
15:30
like it's a weakness, right? so suggestibility
15:33
really is the individual
15:35
ability to respond to suggestion
15:38
and You
15:40
know this maybe is you know begs
15:42
another question Which is you know, what
15:44
is your suggestion or how do you
15:46
define suggestion and and this is a
15:48
Conversation that we should probably have is
15:50
sort of the building blocks of what
15:52
we're talking about But I have to
15:54
say is a sort of a you
15:56
know preliminary comment that sometimes defining concepts
15:58
particularly concepts like art and love
16:00
and love and on. Sometimes the on
16:03
sometimes the finding concepts like
16:05
that more damage than good
16:07
sense that it the sense that
16:09
it creates some limitations on
16:11
the conversation creates creates all kinds
16:13
of in what people people
16:16
think because we do have in
16:18
practice kind of kind of an
16:20
intuitive understanding of what these things
16:22
mean. So we So we can deal with
16:24
art, we can deal with love, we we
16:26
can deal with consciousness even without
16:28
having a precise definition for it. And
16:30
the same is true for a term
16:32
like same is We understand a of what
16:35
suggestion is. We It's some kind of a
16:37
communication that is. gives us that talks
16:39
about, you know, somebody certain kind of
16:41
information you know, us without our knowledge
16:43
of like that. maybe without
16:45
do understand like that. And we
16:47
do understand intuitively that
16:49
we are exposed to to suggestions from
16:51
our spouses, from our our suggestions from
16:54
our our suggestions from our teachers, we
16:56
understand that in general. from
16:58
our teachers, we refers to that
17:00
in general. are we in the
17:02
sense that to how how susceptible, in
17:04
how vulnerable, how how acceptable
17:06
are we of a
17:08
particular suggestion? how So some
17:10
people, are we of a particular come
17:12
to them and you say, you look a little bit tired
17:14
to me today. you You You look to you know, you look
17:17
tired. you look a and they began to
17:19
feel sleepy. sleepy. This is a common thing. common
17:21
thing, out it's not we all know of the
17:23
quite, I mean, we all know
17:25
people like that. say, oh, oh, it's one uh, some
17:27
people say, oh. I must it's one
17:29
o 'clock, I'm really, I must
17:31
be very hungry now, it's and time
17:33
and they not and they're not anymore.
17:35
They're hungry, they're ready to ready to
17:37
horse. horse. Five five minutes earlier, 20 20
17:39
seconds earlier, were not they were not even
17:41
thinking about food. a particular, have a particular, there's
17:43
a particular suggestion, or if you want to
17:45
call it conditioning, if you want to, if
17:48
you want to call it, there's something
17:50
there that triggers them into a particular kind
17:52
of behavior, a particular kind of action. a
17:55
This is interesting of this has
17:57
been studied. this has been studied in
17:59
psychology. in different contexts. So for
18:01
example, we know that there's something called
18:03
hypnotic susceptibility or hypnotic suggestibility. These are
18:06
terms of art. There are very, you
18:08
know, some people make a career out
18:10
of defining and tweaking and working with
18:13
the semantics of what is the difference
18:15
between hypnotizability and hypnotic vulnerability and hypnotic
18:17
responsibility. I don't want to go there.
18:20
This is the kind of scholarship that
18:22
doesn't always, you know, help these kind
18:24
of conversations. But I've been in this
18:27
fold for many many years, for decades
18:29
actually, doing experiments and defining the terms
18:31
and refining them and changing them. And
18:34
I discovered some very interesting things. I
18:36
discovered that some people are extremely suggestible
18:38
in some domains of life, but not
18:41
in others. So some people can be
18:43
very hypnotizable, but very stubborn. For many
18:45
years, we thought that people who are
18:48
suggestible sort of our Star Trek fans.
18:50
They cry at the opera. These are
18:52
the people who miss the phone ringing
18:55
when they watch a movie. And so
18:57
on and so forth. And it turns
18:59
out that it's not as simple as
19:02
that. People who are highly suggestible are
19:04
not necessarily the best placebo responders in
19:06
the world. And the people who are
19:09
the best placebo responders in the world
19:11
are not necessarily highly suggestible in other
19:13
domains. So it's a very complicated field,
19:16
but all people are suggestible in some
19:18
domains. You just have to find the
19:20
domain for that particular person and you
19:23
can find the domain by getting to
19:25
know the person, by learning about their
19:27
hobbies, about their personality, sometimes about their
19:30
genetics, and you can find ways that
19:32
you can penetrate, you can tap that
19:34
suggestible fold that a person has. It
19:37
might be through a pet. It might
19:39
be through their kids, it might be
19:41
through a financial angle, it might be
19:44
through something that is near and dear
19:46
to their heart, it might be through
19:48
a trauma in their life, it might
19:51
be in different domains of life, but
19:53
we are all suggestible to some extent.
19:55
if we we can
19:58
just put our finger
20:00
on what it
20:02
is. Now and this is, I'm just gonna
20:04
make a just going to make a quick thing
20:06
here because I know that we have a lot
20:09
to talk about here. Suggestibility
20:11
is something we can actually
20:13
quantify. We can we can
20:15
actually do a suggestibility. test and the
20:17
psychometrics, psychometrics, or if you
20:20
want the test, retest, reliability of these
20:22
things is better than IQ tests. IQ
20:24
tests. the interesting point here is that that.
20:26
If you you were to give me an
20:28
IQ test, or I were to give you an
20:30
IQ test, we would both want our score
20:32
to be as high as possible. mean, it's human
20:34
I mean, I want my IQ score to be
20:36
high. Why? Because to don't know. It says something
20:38
about how intelligent I am how intelligent or smart
20:41
I am. how smart I I But if
20:43
I give you... a a suggestibility test? you
20:45
want your score to be your score to be
20:47
people high? I mean, some people to score low
20:49
on that would want you said before, low
20:51
maybe it's an indication or an
20:53
index of how said I am. Maybe
20:55
it's an index of how feeble -minded
20:58
I am, how an index of how gullible I
21:00
am, maybe it's an index
21:02
people who take a suggestibility
21:04
test, how spineless, how their
21:06
proclivity is to get as low
21:08
a score as possible because they feel
21:10
that that's an indication of how of
21:12
how... susceptible to manipulation they
21:14
be. And that's interesting thing because in
21:16
terms of the psychology of it... of
21:19
it, Suggestibility tests are probably better
21:21
than IQ tests in terms of
21:23
what they predict and how reliable
21:25
they predict it. it. Yeah. I'm going
21:27
to come back to come back to
21:29
that with hypnosis because I've seen seen
21:31
stage magicians or mentalists. whole you
21:34
know, get a whole group audience the audience and
21:36
they'll find like the six most hypnotizable people and
21:38
those are the ones that get up on get
21:40
up on sometimes on these TV shows, they start
21:42
off, the show starts the the people are
21:44
already on the stage. You don't see what
21:46
they did to weed out out on, so so on.
21:48
So I suspect there's something there. But But
21:50
I wanna drill down a little bit more
21:52
on the gullibility, susceptibility, suggestibility, whatever the right
21:54
word is because I've been having a debate
21:56
with myself, with it's in my next book with
21:58
a lot of my guests. of my guests. How rational
22:00
and animal are we? How gullible are
22:02
we to cults, to fake news, to,
22:04
you know, just bad ideas? And let
22:07
me just riff on this for a
22:09
second. So I tended, you know, is
22:11
a long career debunking nonsense to think
22:13
people are really gullible. And you could
22:15
succor almost anybody into believing some crazy
22:17
idea of flat earthers or UFOs or
22:19
whatever, but... I've now been rethinking it
22:22
because I think it is domain specific
22:24
like you said that is most people
22:26
most of the time are pretty rational.
22:28
You know they keep their jobs they
22:30
have families they got you know food
22:32
in the fridge gas in the tank
22:35
and so on and then and then
22:37
something happens. And we know who these
22:39
people are and otherwise they seem like
22:41
they're married they got kids they got
22:43
jobs. They're like normal people you know
22:45
this is one little domain right. And
22:48
so a lot of it comes from
22:50
I think social proof like. Most of
22:52
the things that I say I believe
22:54
I don't really understand. Like I'm not
22:56
a climate scientist. People send me these
22:58
papers. I can't really understand the modeling.
23:01
It's not what I do. I mostly
23:03
trust the scientists get it right most
23:05
of the time. So when I say
23:07
I accept climate science, I'm really just
23:09
signaling I trust that the experts are
23:11
probably right about this. And there's a
23:14
lot of research on this. on like
23:16
yeah students do you accept the theory
23:18
of evolution and they go yeah yeah
23:20
explain it and they can't explain it
23:22
they don't you know they give some
23:24
Lamarckian thing the draft stretches its neck
23:27
or whatever you know they don't really
23:29
know so in a way you know
23:31
when we say I believe it there
23:33
it's not gullible it's just like I
23:35
can't back check everything and it gets
23:37
even and it gets even more complicated
23:40
than that Michael because when somebody says
23:42
to you equal E equals E equals
23:44
MC squared What do you do with
23:46
that? And you know, and do you
23:48
just believe Einstein blindly? I mean, how
23:50
do you know it's not equals MC
23:52
to the power of 1.995? I mean,
23:55
there's all kinds of interesting questions and
23:57
debates that you have to ask yourself
23:59
about. How do we know that that's do
24:01
we know that that's actually
24:03
true? science where the comes into
24:06
the picture. come that's
24:08
where scientists come into the
24:10
picture and experimental science and evidence
24:12
and statistics and replicability and all
24:14
kinds of things that of Most
24:16
people know very little about,
24:19
and even if they know about
24:21
it, even if not about it, it's centerpiece
24:23
of their life, and they And
24:25
they don't... invest a a lot
24:27
of time into thinking about nuances
24:29
ways of presenting ways of presenting information
24:31
and about something is it mean
24:33
if something is statistically significant and
24:35
what kind of a correction
24:37
do you need? you need? It's just
24:39
not part of the lingo. It's not
24:41
part of what people do. It's
24:43
not. do. It's not. And when you're talking about fact
24:45
-checking. There's a very big
24:47
difference from fact -checking. If somebody said
24:50
that there were 10 ,000 people at
24:52
a rally a rally or were actually just
24:54
10 there, that's one kind of of
24:56
of information. It's It's completely different if
24:58
you say that that did a study
25:00
and we showed that this is an
25:02
effective treatment because there are many
25:04
ways to come to that conclusion and
25:06
there are many ways to rebut
25:08
that conclusion. And And when people are
25:10
not familiar with it, they're just looking
25:12
at the bottom line. at the where
25:14
suggestion becomes critical. Now it's about
25:17
about saying saying it. about
25:19
does about a he have a Harvard
25:21
degree or a a Bergen community Is this person
25:23
a doctor this is this person only
25:25
has a master's degree? person only wealth of
25:27
this person? Is he written up in
25:29
Forbes? the wealth of What kind of a suit
25:31
is he wearing? What kind of a
25:33
car is he driving? Does he have
25:35
white teeth of a suit is he teeth What kind
25:37
How long, how much does he charge
25:39
per hour? have white teeth? Are to see this
25:41
person? Let's maybe check his website. Is
25:43
he famous? How many followers? per hour? These are
25:46
all questions that influence people in
25:48
a tremendous way. a tremendous are
25:50
questions that also influence
25:52
not just people. just people, influence
25:54
neurophysiology. And this is
25:56
really what I'm trying to
25:58
say here. here. not a
26:00
theoretical, ethereal, cerebral thing that
26:03
we're talking about, about, know, philosophizing
26:05
about in some kind of
26:07
a grand scheme of abstract
26:09
scheme of This This
26:11
is palpable. This changes
26:13
neurobiology. This creates This creates
26:16
neurochemicals in the in the
26:18
brain. This This creates electrical currents
26:20
in the brain. This changes
26:23
the weights that we associate with
26:25
certain things. in very,
26:27
very real ways. ways. And as a
26:29
result of that, that, our physiology changes
26:31
are thinking. changes. This is what
26:33
This is what people don't always appreciate. They
26:36
think that this is all ethereal. They think
26:38
that this is all all in some kind of
26:40
a space that is completely abstract. it
26:42
is not abstract. not abstract. is
26:45
concrete. This This has to do
26:47
with changing molecules in your brain, forming
26:49
connections in your brain, and as
26:51
a result of of that, you can actually
26:53
change people, not just how they think. how they but
26:55
how they behave. behave. Well that astonishing factoid
26:57
in there your book about the blood
26:59
sugar level changing based on whether
27:02
you think. you think How sweet you
27:04
think the thing is you're eating. How
27:06
is that even possible? What's the mechanism?
27:08
the mechanism for that? In general, mechanisms that
27:10
are based on that are based on are
27:12
based on you do you expect do
27:14
what do you perceive, if you
27:17
perceive time passing a certain way,
27:19
you you think that you you a
27:21
certain amount, certain you burned a a certain
27:23
number of calories. If you you
27:25
think that something is gonna hurt you, if you think
27:27
that you be very painful going to All these
27:29
things have to do with to do with
27:32
neural mechanisms that we sometimes understand
27:34
and we sometimes we think that
27:36
we understand. we think that we matter
27:38
how you look at it, you look beginning
27:41
to unravel to things. And the way that
27:43
we do we do it... is by by
27:45
experimenting usually with small groups when
27:47
you you do with, when you do with
27:50
hypnosis or or with you don't
27:52
do it with with people. You don't do it
27:54
with a million people. You do it with 17
27:56
people it with 17 64. or 64, and you do you do
27:58
it with hypnotic suggestions and and you would...
28:00
suggestible people versus low suggestible people you do
28:02
it with people with a certain neuropsychiatric
28:04
or psychiatric problem and it gives you
28:06
evidence it gives you data and you
28:09
can do brain imaging and you can
28:11
see what changes in the brain but
28:13
you have to take it with a
28:15
grain of salt. These are small studies
28:17
they are revealing but they're not revealing
28:19
to the point of concluding in a
28:21
sweeping way about the population. Maybe it's
28:23
only relevant for people who have a
28:25
particular... mental condition, maybe it's only for
28:27
people who are highly suggestible, maybe it's
28:29
only for people who are susceptible to
28:31
hypnosis and so on. What we're beginning
28:33
to see though, is that we are
28:35
able to target, with suggestion, we're able
28:37
to target in some situations, particularly when
28:39
it comes to visual attention and experiments
28:41
that have to do with the senses,
28:43
let's say with the sense of taste,
28:45
hearing visual information, we are able to
28:47
target areas in the brain that are
28:49
known. to be allocating attention to specific
28:52
domains like the anterior singlet cortex or
28:54
other areas that have very specific anatomy,
28:56
very specific physiology, and we can show
28:58
that people who have the gift, I
29:00
call it the gift, of focusing in
29:02
such a tremendous, you know, laser beam
29:04
fashion on something, are able to activate
29:06
these areas in ways that other people
29:08
cannot. And this can happen momentarily. Sorry?
29:10
Yes, sometimes they're the open energetic areas,
29:12
absolutely. And they're able as a result
29:14
of that to sometimes see patterns that
29:16
do not exist. Because I mean, I
29:18
don't want to say that, you know,
29:20
these people are psychotic or anything of,
29:22
you know, of that nature. That's not
29:24
the direction that I'm going. I'm just
29:26
trying to say that the connection between
29:28
things is sometimes a function of how
29:30
much attention do we... invest in those
29:32
things. So if I sit here and
29:35
every time I sneeze the door opens,
29:37
at some point I'm going to think
29:39
that my sneezing is responsible for the
29:41
door opening. Of course this is
29:43
correlation, more than causation for
29:45
the most part, but most
29:47
part, would it happen many
29:49
times would it happen
29:51
that I come to
29:53
the conclusion that doing
29:55
sneezing is doing
29:57
that? And I can
29:59
lead people an think
30:01
that. I can
30:03
create an experimental situation
30:05
that would lead
30:07
people to do that.
30:09
And that's what
30:11
we often do. what we
30:13
how many repetitions
30:15
that they take for
30:17
somebody to start
30:20
believing. take for that they're actually
30:22
responsible for this. that they're How many
30:24
kind of, How many much information
30:26
does a person need need they
30:28
say, actually, I am the author
30:30
of this. I am causing
30:32
this. Although, you know, quite clearly, are they
30:34
are not the causal reason for
30:36
this to happen. Yeah, interesting.
30:39
I used as an I use this
30:41
example comparing Richard Feynman with John Nash in
30:43
one of my books. that, you know,
30:45
John Nash won Nash won the
30:47
Nobel for his discovery, game theory game
30:49
theory, mathematics. of course,
30:51
for of course, for quantum
30:53
electrodynamics and the Feynman diagrams.
30:56
So they they both saw patterns that no
30:58
one else had seen and they were amply
31:00
rewarded with Nobel Prizes. But but John patterns
31:02
that that didn't see, you know, because he's
31:04
schizophrenic. So there, you know, you're finding
31:06
patterns that are not real. not real. So it's a
31:08
a signal detection problem. And so Now
31:17
at T-Mobile, get four 5G phones on us
31:19
and four lines for $25 a line per
31:21
month when you switch with the
31:23
eligible trade-ins. All on America's largest
31:26
5G network. All on America's
31:28
largest 5G line per month without a pay discount
31:30
using debit or bank account. we per line
31:32
without $25 per line fees and $10 device connection charge.
31:34
Phones via 24 monthly bill credits for well
31:36
-qualified customers contact us before canceling entire accounts
31:38
to continue bill credits or credit stop and
31:41
balance on a required finance agreement to bill
31:43
credits and if you pay off devices early
31:45
ctmobile.com. You're seeing patterns aren't really there. aren't really
31:47
there. know, know, in my book, actually, I
31:49
talk about Feynman, who was who was hypnotized
31:51
at and, you know, the hypnotist, I mean, he know,
31:53
the hypnotist, I mean, he was hypnotized
31:56
at least three times in his
31:58
life kind kind of a public domain. wrote about
32:00
it. And it's really interesting
32:02
because you would think that somebody of
32:05
the caliber of Richard Feynman is not
32:07
just a critical thinker and, you know,
32:09
one of the marvels of, you know,
32:11
the human intellect. But you would think
32:13
that, you know, it would be difficult
32:15
to, you know, to pull a fast
32:18
one on him because he's just too
32:20
intelligent and too skeptical and so on.
32:22
And he was, but he was also
32:24
suggestible. And this is what most people...
32:26
miss in this equation. They think that
32:28
suggestibility means that you are inferior. They
32:31
take the word suggestibility or they take
32:33
this notion of suggestibility and they say
32:35
if I'm suggestible it means that I'm
32:37
weak. And that's not at all the
32:39
case. As a matter of fact, and
32:41
you know we were talking or you
32:44
mentioned it at the beginning, there are
32:46
evolutionary advantages to being suggestible allows you
32:48
to learn much faster. being suggestible allows
32:50
you to navigate the world much more
32:52
effectively and to be more adaptable because
32:54
you can learn from the experience of
32:57
others you can learn by what others
32:59
tell you you don't have to experience
33:01
it yourself if they suggest certain things
33:03
to you you say oh I can
33:05
learn from their experience and so on
33:07
it's a very important mechanism for humans
33:10
to learn from other people's insights from
33:12
their life experience from their even I
33:14
would say calculations from their observations. So
33:16
if somebody tells me something and I
33:18
trust that person and that person ranks
33:20
highly in my hierarchy of individuals that
33:23
I should listen to, why should I
33:25
question that? Why, I mean, why not
33:27
let this guide me in my life
33:29
and make some shortcuts? And in that
33:31
regard, suggestibility and being suggestible is very
33:33
important. The question is... Just like when
33:36
we are skeptical, you know, should we
33:38
skeptical of our belly buttons? I mean,
33:40
you cannot be skeptical of everything because
33:42
when you do that, you become paralyzed.
33:44
You never get out of bed. You
33:46
cannot be suggestible to the point that
33:49
you believe everything. Yeah. And that's
33:51
where it gets complicated.
33:53
So, all those So
33:55
all those degrees and
33:57
credentials and where you
33:59
went to school
34:02
and who you know
34:04
and those sorts
34:06
of things, those are
34:08
proxies those are proxies trust
34:10
and social proof proof that I
34:12
can trust I can trust what this
34:14
person tells me because look at all
34:16
these other characteristics that I already that I already
34:19
acknowledge are worthy. For some people, yes.
34:21
yes. For For some people, no. no. I
34:23
mean, we, I don't don't have to tell
34:25
you. We live at a time
34:27
where there's a backlash against a
34:29
certain elite kind of strata. elite kind
34:31
of, a lot, strata. And I don't
34:33
know, to be a PhD or
34:35
to be a DSC, to be
34:37
an MD I don't know, to be, to be a
34:39
it's a disadvantage to be a DSC, to be an
34:41
MD, And to be honest with
34:43
you, a Um, some people
34:46
are extremely impressed with, uh, physical
34:48
stature. Some people are extremely impressed
34:50
with wealth. wealth. Some people are extremely impressed
34:52
with with some people are extremely impressed
34:54
with humor, are so on and so forth,
34:56
impressed with also a cultural thing. and so
34:58
In different cultures, a cultural attribute
35:00
different meaning to different
35:02
aspects of human personality. meaning to
35:05
different aspects of human so on.
35:07
It's very important to understand.
35:09
so on. It's very very important to
35:11
explain. understand and it's very important to
35:13
changes over the lifespan.
35:16
changes over the to when you're young is
35:18
not necessarily what you're suggestable to when you're older older.
35:21
because you you change, of and
35:23
some of the that that you
35:25
have about certain topics changes. changes but
35:27
we're all we're all suggestible, and this is
35:29
the key. doesn't magician doesn't ask you
35:31
when they start doing a trick. They
35:33
don't ask you, you oh let me just
35:35
interview you first you see how suggestible
35:38
you are before I do you are trick.
35:40
do my trick but a magician would
35:42
sort of say before the person Can
35:44
you tell me a little bit about
35:46
the crowd? mean, is it children? mean, is
35:48
it it, Are they at workers at IBM?
35:50
Are at they Google employees? Are they you know,
35:52
educated people? What are What Are they are they? Are
35:54
they mostly in more in the the 40s? It's
35:57
very important to know these things you you
35:59
can actually tailor particular tricks to a particular
36:01
audience with higher statistical outcome like you
36:04
can you can tailor it in a
36:06
general way but a magician really if
36:08
if they're if they're good it's gonna
36:10
work I mean they know how to
36:12
work the audience regardless of all these
36:14
parameters that's funny yeah Mandy like to
36:16
say how easy it was to fool
36:18
a group of scientists particularly really smart
36:20
educated and and you know high status
36:22
scientists because they think they know what
36:25
he's going to do or they can
36:27
figure it out and it's always something
36:29
super simple. That's true and there's another
36:31
reason and I'm going to tell you
36:33
this this is what I found as
36:35
a scientist I mean I can tell
36:37
you so what Randy said is exactly
36:39
right I mean they think that they
36:41
know they think that you know it's
36:43
going to be difficult to trick them
36:46
and so on but there's another thing
36:48
that is actually a hindrance to being
36:50
a scientist. Let's go back to bending
36:52
a spoon like I, like I, you
36:54
know, we talked about before. If I
36:56
bend a spoon at a scientific conference,
36:58
you know, while we're, you know, having
37:00
a coffee break or while we're, you
37:02
know, over dinner or whatever, I'll bend
37:05
a spoon for their, you know, for
37:07
the entertainment pleasure of my colleagues. That's
37:09
great. And the first thing that they'll
37:11
say is something like, hey, can you
37:13
do it again with your sleeves rolled
37:15
up? So now I roll up my
37:17
sleeves and I do it again. And
37:19
they say, okay, so he can do
37:21
it with his sleeves rolled up. All
37:23
right, can you do it again? And
37:26
now I'll say, okay, but I'm going
37:28
to roll down my sleeves because it's
37:30
uncomfortable for me to have my sleeves
37:32
up. I'm going to roll down my
37:34
sleeves. We've already determined that the sleeves
37:36
have nothing to do with it. And
37:38
I do it again. And in the
37:40
scientists' mind, I'm doing it the same
37:42
way every time. And when I do
37:44
it with my sleeves up, I do
37:47
it using technique X. And when the
37:49
sleeves come down, I do it using
37:51
technique Y. So now they have no
37:53
control and they have no... No
37:55
idea how I'm
37:57
doing it because they're
37:59
only going by
38:01
the effect. Now, magicians
38:04
are sneaky that way. They can achieve
38:06
the same effect using different methods. And
38:08
what they do is they let you,
38:10
they deceive you with the methods So
38:12
what you're controlling for is irrelevant because
38:14
nature, when you're doing things in nature,
38:16
nature doesn't trick you. They, nature operates
38:18
in the same way. And that's why
38:20
scientists are used to thinking about things
38:22
in a certain way. they you.
38:25
roll your sleeves down, and that's
38:27
a big mistake. And it and
38:29
good magicians, smart magicians take advantage of
38:32
that. Yeah, that's a great story.
38:34
yeah I've hung around magicians for so
38:36
long, I've learned how a lot
38:38
of the tricks are done, and I'm
38:40
always disappointed that I know. I'd
38:42
really rather not know. because the explanation
38:44
is always something like That's
38:46
how they do it, really? That's all
38:48
it is? Oh no i I really
38:50
really the super complicated know that I
38:52
thought was going on. I have to
38:54
tell you that um think, and I
38:56
don't know if I'm speaking for other
38:58
magicians, but I definitely am speaking for
39:01
myself. um My sense of
39:03
wonder is a result of learning
39:05
a lot about magic and about,
39:07
you know, the stupid explanations and
39:09
the actual mundane things that
39:11
are going on in order
39:13
to achieve these magnificent effects.
39:15
um my door of wonder has
39:18
closed quite a bit. i mean, it's difficult
39:20
for me as somebody who's been in this
39:22
you know for such a long time. I
39:24
don't, I still I yearn, I really miss
39:26
that feeling that I had when I was
39:28
in first grade and I saw a magician
39:30
perform something and I was like, wow, you
39:32
know, and I would get like pilar erection
39:34
on my skin, you know, my hair would
39:37
go. And I would like, you know, I
39:39
would like to experience this again. It's very
39:41
difficult for me because I too much. And
39:43
as a result of that, when somebody's just
39:45
beginning to perform a trick, I'm already controlling
39:47
for certain things, and I'm looking at certain
39:49
things. uh a result of that,
39:51
I don't have the same enjoyment.
39:53
I can give it to others,
39:55
but I cannot experience it myself
39:57
because I'm sort of contaminated I'm
39:59
jaded. I'm tainted already, And I think I
40:01
think that it happens to
40:04
other magicians in the field
40:06
as well. Yeah, I've learned to I've
40:08
learned to just appreciate the
40:10
presentation and presentation. that goes into it. all
40:12
the practice that goes into it.
40:15
The example, the cups and the cups and
40:17
balls with clear plastic cups. see So
40:19
you can see exactly how it's done I I still
40:21
couldn't do it. it, because it takes
40:23
a lot of practice. It's a It's smooth
40:25
moves. Magicians are very interesting performers. They're
40:27
not just actors who are playing
40:29
the role of wizards. the They
40:32
also They also their own shows. own shows.
40:34
They decide They decide on the
40:36
tricks, they decide on the themes,
40:38
they decide When you connect things. a
40:40
comedy show, when you are doing
40:42
a comedy show, sometimes you're telling somebody else's
40:44
jokes. you're know, you you're doing, you know,
40:47
somebody else writes the text for you and
40:49
so on. on. Most magicians have
40:51
to perform. mean, they have
40:53
to perform. And, you know,
40:55
if know, if you... If a comedy show
40:57
kind of actor you bomb out with a
40:59
bad joke or something that is inappropriate
41:01
joke know something dud inappropriate audience doesn't a dud you
41:03
the audience doesn't take it. You pick
41:05
it up with the next line and
41:08
it's complete. If you're a a magician
41:10
and bombed out, you didn't do well
41:12
on a trick. This is is going
41:14
to be a serious scar on the I
41:16
mean, it's very difficult to recover from
41:18
that. recover from that. And are on their toes
41:20
all the time. the time. They have
41:23
to be. And And it creates some pressure.
41:25
It creates you know, on them to perform
41:27
and they need to be at the
41:29
top of their game. at the top of
41:31
their for calls for a
41:33
particular alertness and and
41:36
vigilance that sometimes is
41:38
not there for other other performers,
41:40
stage performers, and the
41:42
showmanship is tremendous. tremendous. Yeah. Yeah, on
41:44
the last point this,
41:46
this rationality, gullibility point. you
41:49
You know, there's a selection bias there of who
41:51
we pay attention to, the people that actually
41:53
fell for it. Like, these are are the people that
41:55
joined the cult and they committed suicide or you
41:57
know these are know, these are the people that turned
41:59
over more to Scientology or or whatever. But
42:01
there's a a base rate neglect. That is,
42:03
how many people did they try this on?
42:05
How many people have taken the Scientology Scientology or
42:08
held the little cans down at Hollywood and
42:10
Vine at where they set up their little
42:12
desk? where they You know, probably millions, you know? know,
42:14
probably of winnows down to just a handful
42:16
of people that fall for it. just a
42:18
handful other example is fall for it.
42:20
Or my other example is the Netflix documentary, I I
42:22
don't know if you've seen this, but
42:24
it features four women in their seen this, to
42:26
late know, it never married, looking for love, wanna
42:28
get married, have kids, start a family, the
42:30
whole thing, successful careers. the whole thing, successful careers. the
42:32
tender with his airplane guy who's a
42:35
good looking guy and he's looking
42:37
for love and so on. And
42:39
he's not just looking for sex. for love
42:41
and so on. And a long process looking takes
42:43
months. He actually gets them to wire
42:46
him tens of thousands of dollars, this
42:48
is the key. them to So him this handsome
42:50
international man of mystery at the key. So he's
42:52
got the plane. But they actually go
42:54
out on dates with him where they're
42:56
on the plane. They go to some
42:58
resort and island and all these romantic
43:01
things and they hold up and they have thousands
43:03
of texts from this guy. To
43:05
me, there's a lot of social proof. guy. To me,
43:07
that went into, before the moment where, you
43:09
know, he's in Switzerland on some trip on the
43:11
weekend and he calls the woman and says,
43:13
oh my God, the bank doesn't open on Monday
43:16
and I gotta give these guys the ,000 bucks.
43:18
Can you wire it? the I'm gonna close the
43:20
deal and make $10 million. Monday gonna, it's gonna
43:22
be great. We're gonna get married and all this.
43:24
can you do it! I'm going to course
43:26
what we don't know we don't know
43:28
How many many this on? he tried this
43:31
on, right? We always know the right,
43:33
that's right. Look, right. Look,
43:35
this is unfortunately people are, I
43:37
mean, are, I mean,
43:39
fortunately and unfortunately, people
43:42
are very, partial to
43:44
narratives. People love narratives if if
43:46
you have a compelling a
43:49
A fascinating narrative, Alice in know,
43:51
Alice in Wonderland of of
43:53
narrative, something that is
43:55
fantastic and and people go for
43:57
it. People like to think that.
44:00
their movie stars. People like to think
44:02
that to see themselves, you know,
44:04
doing things that you can only
44:06
see in the movies. And people
44:08
love legends and they love fairy
44:10
tales. And if you sell them
44:12
a good narrative and if they
44:14
can see themselves as being successful
44:16
and loved and so on, they'll
44:18
go for it. Yeah. Well, along
44:20
these lines, you discuss in your
44:22
book, Milgram Shock experiments in Zebardo's,
44:24
oh, Prison experiments, now most famous,
44:26
and all of psychology. Obedience to
44:28
authority, are these people just being
44:30
suggestible because of the authority of
44:32
Milgram and the White Coat or
44:34
Zimbardo, you know, at Stanford University.
44:36
What else might be going on
44:38
there in terms of social proof
44:40
or, you know, well, I'm at
44:43
Yale University, here's this guy with
44:45
a white coat. I can't really
44:47
be shocking these people to death,
44:49
can I? I mean, there must
44:51
be some other rationalization going on
44:53
there. Well, you know, succumbing to
44:55
authority is a huge thing. And
44:57
there's a big, there's a big
44:59
difference, really, a remarkable difference between
45:01
the Zimbabwe experiment and the Milgram
45:03
experiment. By the way, they were
45:05
contemporaries. They even went to the
45:07
same high school. The interesting thing
45:09
there is that Zimbabwe actually gave
45:11
his participants costumes to wear. And
45:13
they knew that what they were
45:15
doing was an act. From the
45:17
get-go, it was an act. They
45:19
just got very deeply into it.
45:21
They got so deep into the
45:23
act that they sort of forgot
45:25
or they turned off this particular
45:27
monitoring system that tells them they're
45:29
just acting and they're just wearing
45:31
clothes that, you know, of a
45:33
prisoner or sunglasses of a police
45:35
officer and so on. Zimbardo did
45:37
not, I'm sorry, Milgram did not
45:39
do this in the same way.
45:42
Milgram was a son of Holocaust
45:44
survivors and he was really interested
45:46
after having heard you know, certain
45:48
things at home from his parents
45:50
and so on. He was interested
45:52
in a second Holocaust. can we
45:54
we have this
45:56
happen again of
45:58
millions of people
46:00
will just go
46:02
with a particularly
46:04
enthusiastic leader or
46:06
a very dominant
46:08
figure? And he wanted to ask
46:10
to ask the question from that standpoint, So
46:12
he was the dominant figure, a Yale a
46:14
Yale a coat in a people what to
46:16
do and so on. to do and had a
46:18
slightly different question. a slightly different
46:21
question. the question that Zimbardo asked,
46:23
was also new for psychology because
46:25
he was actually one of the
46:27
first. one of the first got, got what we
46:29
what we call IRB, you know, institutional
46:31
review board approval. At Stanford, it was
46:33
early days, it was the was the so
46:35
it was not very sophisticated or very
46:37
refined, but he got it. refined,
46:40
but he got did something that today would
46:42
be very difficult to do and a
46:44
lot of people will get and a upset will
46:46
get, you know, somebody tried to do it. but
46:48
the effects are the same. It just shows you. same.
46:50
It just vulnerable we are to
46:53
social pressure, to authority, and
46:55
to figures. And I I think
46:57
that really there's no difference
46:59
today. today. You know, know, the dresses changed,
47:01
the the the
47:03
platforms change, the
47:05
techniques change. changed, it's the
47:07
same. the same. Yeah. I don't know if you know
47:09
if you know I did a
47:11
replication of the Milgram shock experiment for
47:13
experiment for Dateline Hansen was
47:15
the Hansen, was the built a
47:18
little toggle box toggle box
47:20
with the signals and so
47:22
forth and the lights
47:24
and and the lights, and we
47:26
people. seven people. The setup was were
47:28
trying out for a reality TV
47:30
show called What a Pain. And they And they
47:32
wanted to see, far know, how far they would
47:34
go and an shocks, right? right? So, and it
47:36
was, you know, under the guise of
47:38
NBC, so right? six of the seven to got
47:40
six of the seven to participate. what this
47:42
is, I'm woman said, this. know what this
47:44
is. I'm not doing this. that hadn't heard
47:46
amazed we got anybody shock heard of
47:48
the Milgram But a couple of the couple of
47:50
the guys were pretty enthusiastic about
47:52
it. Most went all the way, the way.
47:54
but it was clear they were
47:56
not comfortable doing this. I mean, they
47:58
were squirming and sweating. and groaning and
48:01
looking at the our actor playing
48:03
our direct director. director and you
48:05
sure? I I I don't really feel comfortable. You
48:07
must go on. must go on I go
48:09
on. all right you know and and You
48:11
know, and the same found the same
48:13
thing. So they're not mean, they're not
48:16
just blindly obeying some authority some I'm
48:18
an idiot an idiot a You
48:20
know, there's a lot of social
48:22
proof there. Yale you know PhD or NBC.
48:24
NBC I mean, NBC they can't possibly
48:26
be having me kill somebody on
48:28
film. kill mean, come on. That's right.
48:31
on film. must actually know what's going
48:33
on here. Somebody and Milgram reports
48:35
quite openly that people were squirming
48:37
and sweating reports quite signs of discomfort
48:39
and resistance and so on. and
48:42
But at the end of the day, they were pressing
48:44
the button. the end of the day, they were, they
48:46
were, you know, gaging up. and so on, but at the end
48:48
of the were doing it you know, I
48:50
have to say. and they were have
48:52
to say I have to say, I have to
48:54
say that today, We are dealing with
48:56
not the Milgram experiment, not the Zimbardo
48:58
experiment, but other social experiments are going
49:00
on in our society right now and
49:02
they're going on all the time. right
49:05
sometimes the experiment is about, when
49:07
do you not keep silent anymore
49:09
about something? when do you not keep I
49:11
mean these are about something. I
49:13
mean, these are experiments that are not
49:15
necessarily declared as such, but
49:17
it's our life. It's the fabric
49:19
of life. life today. of our
49:21
life today. All All right, let's talk about hypnosis. I'll
49:23
tee this up for you, because
49:26
we've all seen we've all seen the they give
49:28
a post they give suggestion. You won't
49:30
remember the number four. And then
49:32
they wake them up and say, then they you
49:34
know, count and say, okay, And you know, count to know,
49:36
end up at 11 fingers because, you because, know,
49:38
you know, the three... like they go one two five six two, three,
49:40
five, six. And they end up. Yeah, I
49:42
did a demonstration like that for National Geographic that
49:44
some point. that story. in Vegas was that in
49:47
Vegas? in Vegas to Vegas? a Yes, yes, in it
49:49
was in Vegas. a great story. is All
49:51
right, what is going on there? I mean,
49:53
where is the happened What happened? thing first thing
49:55
that we have to understand here is that a
49:57
a huge difference between stage hypnosis or what
49:59
what we usually you know refer to a
50:01
stage hypnosis which is a performance in
50:03
front of an audience and you invite
50:06
people to come to the stage and
50:08
you do some kind of a screening
50:10
on them and then you choose the
50:12
ones who are who seem to be
50:14
very susceptible and then you perform it
50:16
for the entertainment of the crowd because
50:18
you're going to do something really remarkable
50:20
or really funny or really ridiculous. So
50:22
people do all kinds of things. They
50:25
play, you know, double O7 maneuvers on
50:27
stage. They have sex with a chair.
50:29
They do all kinds of things that
50:31
would get the audience wild and, you
50:33
know, and entertained. But stage hypnosis is
50:35
actually different from medical hypnosis or from
50:37
psychological hypnosis that we do in the
50:39
clinic. Hypnosis, first and foremost, is an
50:41
acute intervention. that we are offering to
50:43
people that is based on suggestion, usually
50:46
for a specific problem. It could be
50:48
an open management, smoke cessation, you name
50:50
it. I mean, there's a whole bunch
50:52
of things. And there's a certain medical
50:54
literature, scientific literature associated with it. Sometimes
50:56
not very good, by the way. If
50:58
you go back in time, sometimes the
51:00
experiments were not very well done. But
51:02
with time, it has gotten a lot
51:05
better, and as a result, we know
51:07
a lot more about it. Stage hypnosis.
51:09
is a form of entertainment. I mean,
51:11
magicians do stage hypnosis without really knowing
51:13
anything about hypnosis. I mean, and it
51:15
just goes by the same name. So
51:17
they will, somebody would call themselves, I'm
51:19
a hypnotist, and actually they are just
51:21
a stage hypnotist doing stuff for college
51:24
kids or something like that, not to
51:26
be confused with an American board, you
51:28
know, certified individual who has the right
51:30
credentials. I have to say something about
51:32
not knowing the number four, like you
51:34
mentioned. In order not to know the
51:36
number four, you need to know it
51:38
so that you wouldn't know it. It's
51:40
really, it really is a very tricky
51:43
situation. In other words, you need to
51:45
know that you don't know the number
51:47
four. So you need to have sort
51:49
of a meta-cognitive process going on when
51:51
you count. You need to go one,
51:53
two, three, oh. Now it's going to
51:55
be four, but I don't know it.
51:57
Five, six, seven. So something needs to
51:59
be there to tell you, and this
52:01
is the one that you don't know.
52:04
And this is an interesting concept, because
52:06
metacognition is something that we do know
52:08
a little bit about. And what do
52:10
we do when we think about our
52:12
thinking? And you can actually think if
52:14
you go back to the experiment, the
52:16
Stanford Jail experiment. These people knew. that
52:18
they were acting. They knew that they
52:20
were actors playing the roles of prisoners.
52:23
They were actors playing the roles of
52:25
inmates. They knew it. But at some
52:27
point, it disappeared. At some point, you
52:29
know, it just dissipated or it was
52:31
so weak that they could ignore it.
52:33
The same thing happens here. People know
52:35
that they're not supposed to know the
52:37
number four, but they put it aside
52:39
in such a way that they are
52:42
sort of it doesn't bother them. It
52:44
doesn't bother them that they know it,
52:46
but it doesn't bother them that they
52:48
know that they know it. It's like
52:50
almost like ignoring something in your visual
52:52
field. It's a little bit like daydreaming.
52:54
Like when you daydream, your eyes are
52:56
open, but you're not processing the information.
52:58
You are completely absorbed in your internal
53:01
mental state. So you sit there, eyes
53:03
open, but you are invested internally in
53:05
your own thoughts. So something might be
53:07
happening in your visual field. Your brain
53:09
is registering it, but you are not
53:11
attentive. to it. We can create these
53:13
situations. We know how to create these
53:15
situations in the lab and ecologically in
53:17
the world. And we're getting better and
53:20
better at understanding what is happening at
53:22
the level of the brain. What is
53:24
happening at the level of brain states
53:26
that allows these situations to happen? How
53:28
long can we prolong them? And who
53:30
are the people who are more likely
53:32
to experience them? We know for example
53:34
that you know sometimes psychedelic drugs can
53:36
induce some of these states that are
53:38
very very similar. We know for example
53:41
that meditation, contemplative practice are also experiences
53:43
that that can help
53:45
get people to
53:47
these places and the
53:49
activate the same brain
53:51
structures. this is a So
53:53
structures. this is a
53:55
fascinating field of
53:57
study within the neuro
54:00
sciences within the psychological
54:02
sciences that we're
54:04
getting to know more
54:06
and more about. more
54:08
and it teaches us
54:10
a great deal
54:12
about mental health, about
54:14
about resilience, emotional resilience.
54:16
It resilience, teaches us
54:19
also about It know,
54:21
us also about, you know, How how
54:23
to how how to be a
54:25
better athlete to how to
54:27
use this mindset in order
54:29
to get more performance, physical
54:31
performance and so on. It's
54:33
a fascinating field that we
54:35
are getting more and more
54:37
knowledge on on and about it
54:39
not just experimentally, but with
54:41
applied techniques. but with applied techniques.
54:44
for a second. I know
54:46
the a second. I'm suppressing
54:48
it or whatever. suppressing it or
54:51
whatever. actors playing guards with the
54:53
mirrored with the mirrored sunglasses
54:55
and all that. shouldn't be They know they
54:57
shouldn't be doing this, but... it they They
54:59
just and then it, they ignore it. one
55:02
more step then let me just take
55:04
one more step. at Auschwitz know, the guards
55:06
at Auschwitz or whatever, I know this
55:08
is wrong. I know I shouldn't be
55:10
doing this. Or just take the slaveholders at
55:12
some level, know know, the 1800s America they had to know
55:14
these They had to know want people don't want
55:16
to be slaves. we That's why we have to chain
55:18
them up and beat them They don't want it. I
55:20
wouldn't want to do this. want to do this,
55:22
did it anyway. they did it anyway.
55:24
How did did they do that if they know,
55:26
or maybe they didn't know know, was a different
55:28
time and they didn't know was wrong time and
55:30
they know. You know, wrong. I don't know.
55:33
You know, role playing and role enactment
55:35
can sometimes become quite
55:37
real. can Um, if you
55:39
are, um, If you are an actor.
55:41
and you are you are playing a particular
55:43
role. you can can actually
55:45
get quite absorbed to the the point that
55:47
you are actually living you are playing. you are
55:49
playing. Now, playing. some people are better
55:51
at it at others, a but a lot
55:53
of it, a lot of it is
55:56
a function of the kind of feedback,
55:58
social feedback that you're getting from your...
56:00
peers and the kind of social fabric
56:02
that you are within, the milieu
56:04
that you're set in. These are
56:06
things that we are beginning to
56:08
understand more and more because in
56:10
science, when we do scientific experiments,
56:12
we usually do them in a
56:14
lab. We usually do them in
56:16
settings that are very clinical, very
56:18
anal, very, I would say, not
56:20
ecological. They don't necessarily reflect what's
56:22
happening or the way that we
56:24
behave in the outside world. We
56:26
process information on computer screens, we
56:28
do things in very controlled environments.
56:30
This is not necessarily what is
56:32
happening when we behave in the
56:34
world and when we're exposed to
56:36
all kinds of parameters that we
56:38
often not measure in a controlled
56:40
lab experiment. This is beginning to
56:42
change. Interesting. Yeah, I would recommend
56:44
people read Christopher Browning's book, Ordinary
56:46
Men. There's now a Netflix documentary
56:48
based on that, where they follow
56:50
the lives, he follows the lives
56:53
of this, a police battalion that
56:55
was part of the Einstein scrupen,
56:57
that followed the Vermont, the German
56:59
army, into Ukraine, for example, to
57:01
clean up after the army, kill
57:03
all the Jews, for example, in
57:05
others, but mostly Jews. And, you
57:07
know, these were ordinary men. You
57:09
know, they were slightly older than
57:11
the young men joining the army.
57:13
They were like late 20s, early
57:15
30s. And so the question is,
57:17
is, did they enjoy it? You
57:19
know, how did they do it?
57:21
How do they do it? How
57:23
do you get people to kill
57:25
people? So at first, you know,
57:27
it was the Holocaust by bullets.
57:29
It's called. Yeah. You know, they
57:31
just put the gun right up
57:33
to the back of the head.
57:35
Bam. Blood all over the place.
57:37
It's horrible. So then they moved
57:39
to having them shoot them further
57:41
away with rifles in the pit.
57:43
You can see pictures of this
57:45
and so on. And then ultimately
57:47
that, you know, Himmler went and
57:49
saw one of these and said,
57:51
we got to do something different
57:53
here. That's when they introduced gas
57:55
and that sort of thing in
57:57
the gas chambers. But, you know,
57:59
if you look at their lives,
58:01
you know, in the process of
58:03
doing this, there was some obedience
58:05
to authority. They weren't threatened. Like,
58:07
if you don't do this, you
58:09
know, we're going to shoot you
58:11
or whatever. It was nothing like
58:13
that. But there was a lot
58:15
of social pressure. Like, you know,
58:17
don't be a pussy and, you
58:20
know, we're all doing this, you
58:22
know, and then they all got
58:24
shit-faced afterwards, and there was a
58:26
lot of alcohol and other things
58:28
involved. You know, yes, I could,
58:30
I could, you know, in front
58:32
of everybody back out and say,
58:34
I'm not going to do it.
58:36
But, well, you know, that, that's
58:38
hard to do in front of
58:40
everybody. So, that is hard to
58:42
do. It's hard to do. And
58:44
it's not just a question of
58:46
peer pressure. It's also a question
58:48
of narrative. And, you know, what,
58:50
what is the overarching narrative that
58:52
you're selling to people? race, if
58:54
it's got like race overtones, if
58:56
it has to do with cleansing
58:58
or purging, if it has to,
59:00
whatever it is that you're selling,
59:02
whatever it is that you know
59:04
the overarching agenda is, people love
59:06
a good story. People embrace good
59:08
stories uncritically, uncritically. People are not
59:10
very critical when it comes to
59:12
these things. Again, the skepticism element
59:14
comes again into this into this
59:16
picture. People are not very good
59:18
at questioning. at asking, you know,
59:20
is this true, trying to verify,
59:22
trying to go and see if
59:24
something is falsifiable? It's not part
59:26
of the lingo of most people.
59:28
You need to be educated about
59:30
that. You need to acquire this
59:32
through education, through listening to people
59:34
who tell you about this, through
59:36
learning, really how to think critically.
59:38
This is something that is missing
59:40
in our society and... you are
59:42
trying to do the best you
59:44
can to remedy that. I'm trying
59:47
to do the best I can
59:49
to remedy that, but from the
59:51
scientific point of view, it's very
59:53
clear that we're vulnerable to it.
59:55
Yeah. Yeah, one more recommendation is
59:57
a book called The good
59:59
old days. days. And it's
1:00:01
a it's a collection of letters
1:00:03
from the Zysos and soldiers soldiers their wives
1:00:05
and families back home. And
1:00:07
the narrative was, this is really
1:00:09
brutal. It is nasty, but. but... If
1:00:11
we we weren't doing it, they'd be
1:00:13
doing it to us. to us. If this
1:00:16
war turns around, the Russians are gonna
1:00:18
treat treat my family, you guys, you would would
1:00:20
not believe. so So we need to win
1:00:22
this war. And besides that, the the
1:00:24
Jews started the whole thing, thing, they repeat
1:00:26
the stab in the back conspiracy theory
1:00:28
theory all that stuff. and all Okay, back
1:00:30
to Okay, back to hypnosis. So, the last time I
1:00:32
looked at this this was like the Ernst Hilgaard's theory
1:00:34
of the hidden observer. observer. which which is
1:00:36
sort of a metaphor, I don't know.
1:00:38
I There's somebody in there that knows
1:00:40
the there that knows the four, right? And that's somebody or a neural
1:00:42
a neural network or whatever it is.
1:00:44
There's some other module of the brain
1:00:46
that that keeps of stuff, even though the
1:00:48
conscious self is saying, I don't know
1:00:50
what it is, it is. Right? Is, but that's, that's that's
1:00:52
decades old now, that theory. What's the
1:00:54
latest we know on the neuroscience of
1:00:56
hypnosis and - And so, uh, what Hilkard did was
1:00:58
very interesting. uh, the notion of the notion of
1:01:01
the hidden observer, he basically told people
1:01:03
something like, you know, you're not gonna know, know,
1:01:05
I don't know, know, scissors or It's better over
1:01:07
here. AT&T customers, switching
1:01:09
to T-Mobile has never been easier. We'll
1:01:11
pay off your existing phone and give
1:01:13
you a new one free, all
1:01:16
on America's largest 5G network.
1:01:18
Visit t-mobile.com/carrier freedom to
1:01:20
switch today. Pay
1:01:22
off up to $650 via virtual prepaid MasterCard in 15
1:01:24
days. Free phone up to $830 via 24 monthly bill
1:01:26
credits plus tax. Qualifying port
1:01:28
and trade and service on Go5G Next and credit required.
1:01:31
Contact us before canceling entire account to continue bill credits
1:01:33
or credit stop and balance and required finance agreement is
1:01:35
due. some objects. you know, and he would put a would
1:01:37
put a whole bunch of objects, and including scissors, what's
1:01:39
this, what's this, what's this, what's this, what's this.
1:01:41
And to the scissors, people what he pointed to don't know what, I don't
1:01:43
know what, I had some know this. I don't know what, I don't know, a
1:01:45
musical instrument. I don't know, I've never seen anything like that. of the
1:01:48
But he told he told them, of the hypnotic
1:01:50
induction. will know, he would say, what is, what is, what is, what I
1:01:52
touch your right shoulder, what is, what you will
1:01:54
know. is, what is, what is, what then what he did is, what is,
1:01:56
would say, what is this? what would say, I don't know. I
1:01:58
think it's a musical instrument. I think is, or that. And then
1:02:00
he would touch their right shoulder and
1:02:02
they would say, oh, it's scissors. I
1:02:04
can see that now that it's a
1:02:06
scissors. What we added to this original
1:02:08
pilgrid plot, which has to do with
1:02:11
metacognition and hypnosis and all these things,
1:02:13
is brain imaging. Today we have imaging
1:02:15
of the living human brain, which allows
1:02:17
us to, with sophisticated technology, see what
1:02:19
brain areas light up and what do
1:02:21
they do and what happens when you
1:02:23
have a hypnotic suggestion like that? what
1:02:25
happens when you're just hypnotized with no
1:02:27
suggestion, what's the difference between relaxation and
1:02:29
so on, and we're beginning to identify
1:02:32
different areas and different structures in the
1:02:34
brain, and we're beginning to see what's
1:02:36
the difference. And one of the things
1:02:38
that we notice is that people really
1:02:40
allocate attention very differently. The brain mechanisms
1:02:42
that have to do with attentional allocation
1:02:44
in the brain are being resourced very
1:02:46
differently when you are given. a hypnotic
1:02:48
suggestion to do something, let's say to
1:02:50
pay attention to something visually. For example,
1:02:52
if I tell you you don't know
1:02:55
scissors, basically what happens is they are
1:02:57
blocking that part of the table where
1:02:59
the scissors are. It's as if they
1:03:01
see something hazy there. They just can't
1:03:03
see what the object is. A little
1:03:05
bit like the daydreaming that I described
1:03:07
to you before. It's a little bit
1:03:09
like it's masked. This is like sort
1:03:11
of the metaphor or the representation of
1:03:13
what's going on. They can see that
1:03:16
there as an object there. but it's
1:03:18
too hazy to observe or it's too,
1:03:20
it's masked, it's behind a veil of
1:03:22
some sort. So it's not that they
1:03:24
see it and they say, oh this
1:03:26
is scissors and I'm not supposed to
1:03:28
know it, maybe they do it once
1:03:30
or twice at the beginning and then
1:03:32
they say, oh this is a part
1:03:34
of the table that I'm not supposed
1:03:36
to look at, I'm not supposed to
1:03:39
have access to. This is interesting because
1:03:41
when we do these experiments time and
1:03:43
again, time and again, we begin to
1:03:45
see that different people have different strategies
1:03:47
as to how to achieve it, but
1:03:49
they all use their attentional resources.
1:03:51
in different ways. ways. Sometimes
1:03:53
try to avert
1:03:55
their gaze. gaze. They They
1:03:57
try not to
1:04:00
look at the the object
1:04:02
directly. Sometimes they
1:04:04
try to look at
1:04:06
it obliquely it covert
1:04:08
attention. covert We can
1:04:10
see these attentional mechanisms working either in overdrive
1:04:12
or underdrive, depending on what the instruction
1:04:14
is and so on. So that's one
1:04:16
way. mean, there are many ways to
1:04:18
achieve this. I Now we know many ways
1:04:20
to attention Now we know that really
1:04:22
is a very powerful filter to also
1:04:25
regulating our emotions. It's not just regulating
1:04:27
our senses our our perceptions. It's also
1:04:29
a very strong filter of regulating
1:04:31
our emotions, what we allow ourselves to
1:04:33
get excited about, what we allow to
1:04:35
to get, you know, aggressive about. to
1:04:38
get, you know, attention is a
1:04:40
very strong mediator of behavior. If you
1:04:42
I I mean, you can create
1:04:44
a lot of self -regulation. It's a
1:04:46
function of temperament, of course. Some
1:04:48
people are more of course. Some mean, are
1:04:50
more trigger them very, very easily. you will
1:04:52
trigger them very, very easily. So element of personality
1:04:54
an and temperament, and things like
1:04:56
here and But it's also a
1:04:58
function of like that. You can train.
1:05:00
a your attention. You can
1:05:02
train your attentional system your attention, there
1:05:04
are ways to train the
1:05:06
brain. There are ways to train
1:05:08
the brain through cognitive training training
1:05:10
and certain exercises. How effective
1:05:13
they are to to be seen
1:05:15
but some people benefit from it
1:05:17
more than others and that's just
1:05:19
a fact of life there's no question
1:05:21
no question about these
1:05:23
people of your of your
1:05:25
experiments participants in in the
1:05:27
hypnotists show. They're not They're not just
1:05:30
going along and faking to not embarrass the
1:05:32
the or the magician. they
1:05:34
really are in some kind of
1:05:36
altered state of of consciousness. Right, and I
1:05:38
would say, Twilight Zone, between is
1:05:41
I would say, um, Are you just
1:05:43
doing it because of this role Are Are you just
1:05:45
doing it because of social compliance? you just trying
1:05:47
to be nice to me? Are you just
1:05:49
trying to know, for me not to lose face?
1:05:51
And that's why you're doing all these things. things, versus
1:05:54
you're doing this because you're
1:05:56
genuinely. genuinely entrenched in a
1:05:58
brain state that makes makes you... feel that
1:06:00
this is real, we can show a
1:06:02
whole spectrum. We can show a whole
1:06:05
spectrum of some people who are towards
1:06:07
this end and some people who are
1:06:09
towards this end. And we're trying to
1:06:12
understand what is it about some people
1:06:14
that are closer to this particular distribution
1:06:16
and people are a little bit close
1:06:18
to the other part of the distribution.
1:06:21
And we're beginning to see that this
1:06:23
has to do with certain genetic polymorphisms,
1:06:25
certain brain structures, certain connections between brain
1:06:27
areas. but we are identifying again and
1:06:30
again the same brain areas of interest.
1:06:32
We can name them, we can measure
1:06:34
them, we can see the connections, and
1:06:36
more importantly, we can see adjacent behaviors.
1:06:39
So for example, when people are, as
1:06:41
I mentioned before, psychedelic drugs, people who
1:06:43
are doing meditation, people are, you know,
1:06:46
Tibetan monks, people are doing, you know,
1:06:48
contemplative practice and so on, we can
1:06:50
see that some of the same areas
1:06:52
are involved. in these altered states of
1:06:55
consciousness, in these pattern recognition kind of
1:06:57
activities, in the ability to regulate pain,
1:06:59
in the ability to demonstrate resilience, to
1:07:01
recover from trauma and so on. And
1:07:04
this gives us the clues that we
1:07:06
are on some kind of an interesting
1:07:08
path where we can actually distill the
1:07:11
neurobiological substrates of these things. Yeah, one
1:07:13
of my favorite magicians on that is
1:07:15
Darren Brown, I'm sure you know him.
1:07:17
Of course. And, you know, he does
1:07:20
this hypnosis, he has this whole show
1:07:22
about faith healers, and which he kind
1:07:24
of makes the point indirectly that faith
1:07:26
healers are themselves kind of hypnotizing their
1:07:29
audience in a way, and putting them
1:07:31
in a state in which they're more
1:07:33
susceptible to their particular message, and then
1:07:36
he does it himself, using hypnosis. But
1:07:38
again, you know, we don't see where
1:07:40
those people came from. Are there ways
1:07:42
to test, you know, like if you
1:07:45
had 100 people in the room, could
1:07:47
you give me the, you know, through
1:07:49
however many hours you need to do
1:07:51
this, these are the 10 most susceptible
1:07:54
people? Yes, yes. I mean, so when
1:07:56
it comes to hypnotic susceptibility or hypnotic
1:07:58
suggestibility, I mean, we have very good
1:08:01
tools. We have very good instruments, psychological
1:08:03
instruments, where we can screen people and
1:08:05
sort of identify how good they are.
1:08:07
Remember, suggestion, hypnotic suggestion is just one
1:08:10
type of suggestion, but suggestion in general
1:08:12
is an uncritical acceptance of an opinion
1:08:14
or an idea or a behavior, which
1:08:16
really arises from either within us or...
1:08:19
the influence of others. It could be
1:08:21
me, it could be you. So there
1:08:23
are ways to test that. There really
1:08:25
are ways to test that and you
1:08:28
don't need to sort of take it
1:08:30
into a psychoanalytical domain or, you know,
1:08:32
or, you know, Zigmund Freud asking you
1:08:35
to lie on the couch. You can
1:08:37
do it in very practical ways, and
1:08:39
we have tests that do that. But
1:08:41
the take-home message is not just about
1:08:44
identifying the people who are very suggestible,
1:08:46
because they could be vice presidents of
1:08:48
companies, CEOs, or they could be the
1:08:50
janitor, or somebody else. The most important
1:08:53
thing is to understand that everyone is
1:08:55
highly susceptible to some form of suggestion.
1:08:57
This is something that a lot of
1:09:00
people miss. Often without realizing it, we
1:09:02
are suggestible and these suggestions shape our
1:09:04
perceptions. They color our behaviors, even our
1:09:06
physiological experiences in profound ways. And that's
1:09:09
really what I'm trying to drive home
1:09:11
in this book because the examples that
1:09:13
I give are real life examples. They're
1:09:15
not, I mean, they're from decades of
1:09:18
studies that I've done throughout my life,
1:09:20
also from my experience as a magician.
1:09:22
I mean, I remember... being a teenager
1:09:25
performing theatrical mind reading for you know
1:09:27
all kinds of audiences and people will
1:09:29
come to me people my parents age
1:09:31
will come to me after and consult
1:09:34
with me on life matters they will
1:09:36
say you know my aunt is dying
1:09:38
you know what do you recommend that
1:09:40
we do this or that or I'd
1:09:43
like to invest in this company I
1:09:45
was 16 you know I mean I
1:09:47
had no idea what and and it
1:09:49
was amazing to me that no matter
1:09:52
what disclaimers I would use no matter
1:09:54
how many times I will say this
1:09:56
is a show You know, these are
1:09:59
tricks. The mental part is so strong,
1:10:01
the effect is so compelling, that they
1:10:03
just forget about all these disclaimers. It's
1:10:05
what they want to hear, it's what
1:10:08
they want to see. It's not what
1:10:10
you say is what they think you
1:10:12
say. It's not what they see, it's
1:10:14
what they think you see. Yeah, we
1:10:17
did another TV show with James Van
1:10:19
Prague, who was a big psychic. Talked
1:10:21
into the dead in the 90s late
1:10:24
90s. So I was part of a
1:10:26
history chat history mysteries I think it
1:10:28
was so they filmed him for hours
1:10:30
and You could see how he got
1:10:33
information from people he would chit chat
1:10:35
with them on the brakes and things
1:10:37
like that and then come back to
1:10:39
the information he got because we've filmed
1:10:42
him Well, he didn't know he was
1:10:44
being filmed during the breaks So we
1:10:46
figured out how he was doing that
1:10:49
but the people themselves forgot you know
1:10:51
they'd say he I never told him
1:10:53
my grandfather's name was George It's like
1:10:55
actually you did and we'll show you
1:10:58
how you did it, you know, because
1:11:00
he's like I'm getting this name and
1:11:02
this initial and then he goes, yeah,
1:11:04
that is George, yeah, letter G, yeah,
1:11:07
George is my uncle, whatever it was,
1:11:09
you know, and then they'd forget that,
1:11:11
you know, back to the forgetting thing.
1:11:14
Yeah, okay, so. If you have some
1:11:16
theory of altered states of consciousness, you
1:11:18
must have a theory of consciousness. Do
1:11:20
you want to wade into the hard
1:11:23
problem of consciousness and give us your
1:11:25
explanation? What do you think is? Well,
1:11:27
the hard problem of consciousness. So, you
1:11:29
know, it's really interesting for me. So
1:11:32
I've been dealing with aspects of consciousness,
1:11:34
you know, most of my adult life
1:11:36
through experiments, through philosophical discussions, through debates,
1:11:38
through intellectual, you know, scientific conferences and
1:11:41
so on. I have to tell you,
1:11:43
and I'm being super honest, that I
1:11:45
have learned as much from my magic
1:11:48
experience, and being a magician in the
1:11:50
trenches, performing in front of audiences, making
1:11:52
people believe or remember certain things that
1:11:54
didn't happen, like you said, or making
1:11:57
people see. certain things
1:11:59
that were never there.
1:12:01
never there, or them forget things that
1:12:03
were there, I learned a lot
1:12:05
more, or as much. from my
1:12:08
magician role as I did as
1:12:10
a scientist, doing I did
1:12:12
as a scientist doing with
1:12:14
experiments with hypnosis you know, masking
1:12:16
and and reverse masking and
1:12:18
back back masking and stimuli and
1:12:21
all these things. and all these
1:12:23
And the reason I say this I say
1:12:25
this is because when When it comes to
1:12:27
consciousness, We have a
1:12:29
have a fairly of
1:12:32
what consciousness understanding of what consciousness
1:12:34
is, just like we have an intuitive
1:12:36
understanding of would like We would like to
1:12:38
think that we have free will. not
1:12:40
confusing free will with free speech or things
1:12:42
like that, just free will. just free will.
1:12:44
And ability to go and
1:12:46
tweak, to rattle that full free will, which
1:12:49
magicians, mentalist in mentalists in particular,
1:12:51
but magicians can do
1:12:53
so easily. so so easily going
1:12:55
going to college, without spending years and
1:12:57
years and years studying statistics and
1:12:59
research methods and methods to seminars and
1:13:01
all these things, you can do it
1:13:03
when you're 14. when You can do
1:13:06
it when you're 15. you're 15 and you
1:13:08
can do you can do it can make you can make
1:13:10
a living a good Being a good magician spend
1:13:12
most spend most of their life doing these
1:13:14
things and they can't make a good
1:13:16
living and this this is not about a living.
1:13:18
This is about, you know, what can
1:13:20
you you unravel Now, the the methods of a scientist
1:13:22
and the method of a magician are completely
1:13:24
different, but they can sort of converge in some
1:13:26
of the same questions. And what I'm trying
1:13:28
to say... And what I'm that to say sure
1:13:30
there are other professionals out there. They
1:13:32
could be marketers, could they could be they could be
1:13:34
barbers, they could be accountants, they could be
1:13:36
lawyers, they could be anything. they could be who
1:13:39
are also illuminating questions related to consciousness through
1:13:41
their own tools that I'm not familiar with.
1:13:43
I'm not, you know, I don't have... with.
1:13:45
I'm I'm not privy to what they're
1:13:47
able to do and I'm not
1:13:49
part of of their professional realm of techniques I
1:13:51
so on. the kind I think these
1:13:53
are the are of questions, these are the kind
1:13:56
of questions that can illuminate consciousness in many
1:13:58
interesting ways. It doesn't have to come. from
1:14:00
neuroscientists. It doesn't have to come from
1:14:02
psychologists. It doesn't have to come from
1:14:04
mathematicians or from computational cognitive scientists. It
1:14:07
could come from magicians. It could come
1:14:09
from people who are doing some human
1:14:11
interaction of some marketers. It could come
1:14:13
from other people. It's just a question
1:14:16
of refining the questions and trying to
1:14:18
systematically study them. And the reason I
1:14:20
say that I learned as much about
1:14:22
consciousness as a magician as I did
1:14:25
from being a scientist is because for
1:14:27
me magic. is not a one-trick thing.
1:14:29
It's not that I, you know, read
1:14:31
what's written on the cereal box and
1:14:33
I tried it and that made me
1:14:36
a minute. It's a lifelong pursuit. It's
1:14:38
a serious systematic study of a particular
1:14:40
body of knowledge and trying things and
1:14:42
being a member of a society, going
1:14:45
to conferences, associating with colleagues and so
1:14:47
on. The same thing is true as
1:14:49
a scientist. I don't know that I'm
1:14:51
the best magician within the scientist or
1:14:54
the best scientist within the magicians. But
1:14:56
it's very important to understand to understand.
1:14:58
that both of these worlds illuminated my
1:15:00
understanding of concepts like free will, conscious
1:15:03
and so on, in a very, very
1:15:05
big way. So I don't expect magicians
1:15:07
to know too much about neuropsychology. I
1:15:09
don't expect them to know too much
1:15:12
about brain structures, about things that are
1:15:14
related to the technical jargon and the
1:15:16
experiments that are related to neuroscience. I
1:15:18
don't think it's even necessary. But I
1:15:21
also have to appreciate that most neuroscientists,
1:15:23
most psychologists know very little, if anything
1:15:25
about magic and about the powerful techniques
1:15:27
that these performers, you know, actors playing
1:15:30
the role of wizards, can illuminate questions
1:15:32
that are related to consciousness in no,
1:15:34
you know, less fundamental and practical a
1:15:36
way. So to answer your question, I'm
1:15:39
blessed by having both these perspectives, and
1:15:41
I think that that gives me perhaps
1:15:43
something that most of my colleagues don't
1:15:45
have. One of my favorite conspiracy theories
1:15:48
that turned out to be a real
1:15:50
conspiracy when I was writing my conspiracy
1:15:52
book was MK. The CIA's program for
1:15:54
mind control, one of which was hypnosis.
1:15:56
You're not worried that the North Korea...
1:15:59
and the Chinese and the Russians are
1:16:01
using these techniques to, you know, create
1:16:03
super soldiers and so on. You know,
1:16:05
a lot of that didn't pan out.
1:16:08
You know, what are the limitations on,
1:16:10
let's say, government or terrorists using these
1:16:12
techniques to, I don't know, create an
1:16:14
assassin by hypnotizing them or teenage boys
1:16:17
that want to know if they can
1:16:19
hypnotize girls to get them to take
1:16:21
their clothes off, that kind of stuff.
1:16:23
I mean, how much can we really
1:16:26
control somebody else's mind? Or and then
1:16:28
we can move to the techniques you
1:16:30
can actually use to you know stop
1:16:32
smoking or lose weight or whatever These
1:16:35
are these are really hard question to
1:16:37
answers, but we have some leads that
1:16:39
can that can help us answer them
1:16:41
So we are all familiar with stories
1:16:44
about cults. They're well documented We know
1:16:46
that the energetic dominant enthusiastic, you know
1:16:48
aggressive cult leaders can actually fascinate crowds.
1:16:50
They can, you know, if you want
1:16:53
to call it mesmerize, if you want
1:16:55
to call it hypnotize, whatever you want
1:16:57
to call it, that's fine with me,
1:16:59
but they can captivate the imagination of
1:17:02
people to the point that they would
1:17:04
follow what the cult leader says uncritically.
1:17:06
Again, this word, uncritically. They will believe
1:17:08
things that are, you know, certainly false.
1:17:11
They would discount things when things don't
1:17:13
go right. They just say, oh, you
1:17:15
know, you didn't understand, we misunderstood, he
1:17:17
misspoke, whatever it is. So that's, we
1:17:19
know that, that's well documented. We also
1:17:22
know about mass hysteria. We know that,
1:17:24
you know, there's this phenomenon that is
1:17:26
again well documented throughout the ages going
1:17:28
back to medieval times and even before
1:17:31
that, you know, people can demonstrate these
1:17:33
mass hysteria kind of situations as a
1:17:35
result of particular rumor a particular suggestion
1:17:37
they think that there's a toxin in
1:17:40
the water they think that they're breathing
1:17:42
something in the air they begin to
1:17:44
you know have all kinds of things
1:17:46
we know about the Salem witch trials
1:17:49
we know these things these are things
1:17:51
that are part you know don't
1:17:53
need to go
1:17:55
to college and do a
1:17:58
a in in the
1:18:00
paranormal know order to
1:18:02
know that people
1:18:04
are complex. We are
1:18:07
very complex organisms. and
1:18:10
we have higher brain functions
1:18:12
that are very susceptible to stories,
1:18:14
narratives, ideas, beliefs. We know know
1:18:16
that we know how powerful that
1:18:18
can be. We can, We with
1:18:20
techniques like hypnosis and
1:18:23
with things like psychedelic drugs and
1:18:25
with digital platforms and with
1:18:27
information and by repeating information by
1:18:29
with artificial intelligence, with deep fakes
1:18:31
and so on, we can
1:18:33
really deep fakes and so on. We the way
1:18:35
people think in unprecedented ways. It's
1:18:37
not just a function of hypnosis. It's
1:18:39
not just a function of taking
1:18:41
people and hypnotizing them with a pendulum
1:18:43
or with other techniques that we
1:18:46
have. with other techniques that we have.
1:18:48
It's the convergence of what
1:18:50
we're beginning to have. have. We're beginning
1:18:52
to have information, we are
1:18:54
bombarded with information like never
1:18:56
before. like never we
1:18:58
are exposed to to explosive
1:19:01
amounts of information without being able to
1:19:03
screen it, without being able to
1:19:05
vet it. able to are beginning
1:19:07
are to believe things
1:19:09
based on our cultural upbringing.
1:19:12
based on on our religious
1:19:14
beliefs lack thereof. based on on
1:19:16
our education, on based on our social who
1:19:18
who are the people that we
1:19:20
talk to. to. Where do we do
1:19:22
we live, our geography? And this is this
1:19:25
is beginning to create
1:19:27
situations where people are only
1:19:29
people echo living in echo chambers or
1:19:31
in bubbles and they only speak to people
1:19:33
who think like them or behave like
1:19:35
them or so on, like but we
1:19:37
are beginning to we are beginning to certain things
1:19:39
that are completely untrue but are
1:19:41
liberating for us know we know we feel to
1:19:44
we feel very badly about the
1:19:46
white rhinos we just go into wikipedia
1:19:48
and we just rewrite the numbers
1:19:50
and there we save them rhinos out more
1:19:52
white rhinos out there you know they
1:19:54
were now they're they're not don't don't we
1:19:56
don't feel comfortable with the george
1:19:59
washington you know a slaveholder, we just go
1:20:01
and we change the Wikipedia entry or
1:20:03
we say something about that. This is
1:20:06
a new thing. It's a new thing
1:20:08
in our society. It's not just a
1:20:10
question of alternative facts. It's a question
1:20:12
of information. We have so much information.
1:20:15
We don't know what to do with
1:20:17
it. It comes from multiple sources. We
1:20:19
don't have the time. We spend our
1:20:22
days. scrolling up and down screens if
1:20:24
it's your smartphone if it's your laptop
1:20:26
if it's your tablet you know whatever
1:20:29
and it's just information addiction some people
1:20:31
are addicted to alcohol some people are
1:20:33
addicted to drugs we are getting to
1:20:36
be as a society we're addicted to
1:20:38
information we are bombarded with information and
1:20:40
we don't have the tools to deal
1:20:43
with that information onslaught as a result
1:20:45
suggestion becomes all the more powerful in
1:20:47
our lives it becomes all the more
1:20:49
concrete and elemental, it becomes rudimentary because
1:20:52
we are beginning to get more and
1:20:54
more. We make decisions based on suggestions,
1:20:56
not based on real information, because we
1:20:59
don't have the time to process the
1:21:01
information. We don't have the tools to
1:21:03
process the information. When you talk about
1:21:06
the Holocaust, you have become a scholar
1:21:08
of the Holocaust. You have read so
1:21:10
much about it. You have interviewed so
1:21:13
many people. You have spent time going
1:21:15
through historical records. And this is one
1:21:17
of the events in history that actually
1:21:20
has a mountain of evidence about it.
1:21:22
Compare it to other atrocities, other situations
1:21:24
where we don't have a lot of
1:21:26
evidence. People don't see that. They don't
1:21:29
understand. To them, it's all the same.
1:21:31
And they put things in a very
1:21:33
superficial plateau. The problem is that the
1:21:36
suggestions of an expert, the information of
1:21:38
an expert or somebody who invests. a
1:21:40
tremendous amount of their intellectual prowess into
1:21:43
a particular question becomes as valuable as
1:21:45
as you know the ruminations of somebody
1:21:47
who just learned about it five minutes
1:21:50
ago. And this is the problem that
1:21:52
we have and that's where suggestion becomes
1:21:54
so powerful because now it's about how
1:21:57
white your teeth are and how straight
1:21:59
they are. And that's very dangerous. Yeah.
1:22:01
All right last question. Is somebody dealing
1:22:03
with depression or anxiety or ADHD or
1:22:06
they just want to stop smoking or
1:22:08
drink less or lose some weight or
1:22:10
exercise? What can you recommend that they
1:22:13
do? So, you know, I have a
1:22:15
whole chapter on whether, you know, antidepressants,
1:22:17
for example, are working through the chemical
1:22:20
ingredients in them or through some kind
1:22:22
of a, you know, complex, suggestive process.
1:22:24
And when we look at the data
1:22:27
very critically and very carefully and very
1:22:29
carefully, and it's not, I'm not speaking
1:22:31
on behalf of Amir Raz, I'm speaking
1:22:34
here on behalf of, you know, a
1:22:36
large group of scientists who are looking
1:22:38
at these data for many many years
1:22:40
and doing all kinds of very sophisticated
1:22:43
statistical analysis, we see that the effects
1:22:45
are very very small. So when it
1:22:47
comes to, for example, depression and millions
1:22:50
of people, this is a very real
1:22:52
people, this is a very real disease
1:22:54
and a very serious disease and a
1:22:57
disorder that is not just debilitating in
1:22:59
terms of life quality and things like
1:23:01
that, but also in terms of suicidality
1:23:04
who's inflicted and so on. But we
1:23:06
are demonstrating time and again that some
1:23:08
drugs, antidepressants in particular, are not as
1:23:11
effective as we thought that they once
1:23:13
were. And we can demonstrate that there
1:23:15
are other techniques that are as good,
1:23:17
sometimes better, that antidepressants. And yet in
1:23:20
our society, that's the first thing that
1:23:22
we get. I mean, when we show
1:23:24
signs of depression and we go to
1:23:27
a psychiatrist, they usually give us antidepressants,
1:23:29
and that's a backbone drug in modern
1:23:31
psychiatry despite... in spite of all the
1:23:34
science that we know and you know
1:23:36
you gotta ask yourself why is this
1:23:38
happening why are we not doing something
1:23:41
about this this is scientific knowledge It's
1:23:43
not well It's not
1:23:45
well known from five
1:23:48
minutes ago. well
1:23:50
known for a while.
1:23:52
that Why is it
1:23:54
that it's happening? a
1:23:57
very is a very
1:23:59
complex answer to answer,
1:24:01
because it's not
1:24:04
just about knowledge. It's
1:24:06
not just about
1:24:08
what we know. It's
1:24:11
about about regulation and big
1:24:13
pharma and politics and insurance policies and
1:24:15
regulation. It's about all kinds of things
1:24:17
that have nothing to do with science
1:24:19
and have nothing to do with it.
1:24:21
And and we're looking at the data,
1:24:23
it's clear at plays a huge role a huge
1:24:26
role in getting into depression, getting out of
1:24:28
depression. And And, you know, if we understand that, we
1:24:30
can maybe do something about it in
1:24:32
a different way that we're doing today. Now,
1:24:34
I'm not Now, I'm not poopooing I'm
1:24:36
just saying there's something there that
1:24:38
suggests to us that antidepressants are
1:24:40
far, far away. the from the effectiveness
1:24:42
that we once attributed to them. And
1:24:44
they're probably on par with placebos for
1:24:46
most of of depression. And that's well documented.
1:24:48
in the the mental health literature. Now, what
1:24:50
about the stuff that is not documented
1:24:52
well in the mental health literature? in
1:24:54
Because we haven't even gotten there because
1:24:56
science takes time. Research takes time. It
1:24:58
takes effort. takes, know. takes time, When
1:25:01
it comes to these things, if you
1:25:03
can demonstrate this with it comes to these things,
1:25:05
if you for example, in antidepressants, what
1:25:07
else can you demonstrate? example, an What
1:25:09
I am what else can you say
1:25:12
what I am is that to
1:25:14
say here key the key about about that
1:25:16
it's not just a tool. a
1:25:18
tool. for magicians or but
1:25:21
a fundamental aspect of human nurse psychology. of
1:25:24
human It operates in
1:25:26
all areas of life. in
1:25:28
we understand how we works. how
1:25:30
allows us to works, it allows us
1:25:32
to harness its power improve
1:25:34
well well-being and and resist
1:25:37
manipulation, better navigate
1:25:39
our personal personal realities.
1:25:41
Ultimately, is
1:25:43
is not a sign of weakness, just like
1:25:45
we talked about before, but a complex. deeply
1:25:48
rooted aspect of the human mind that mind
1:25:50
that can leverage in all
1:25:52
kinds of ways. ways to our advantage to
1:25:55
our advantage from mental health,
1:25:57
like the depression that we talked
1:25:59
about talked things like performance in sports
1:26:01
all the way to self-regulation,
1:26:03
to resilience training, for example.
1:26:05
Our mindsets and the functionality
1:26:07
of our brains, our brain
1:26:10
states, are as trainable as
1:26:12
our anatomy. A lot of
1:26:14
people don't understand this concept.
1:26:16
Our physical bodies are controlled
1:26:18
by our minds in addition
1:26:20
to the muscles and the
1:26:22
glands that they're connected to.
1:26:24
And we should take advantage.
1:26:26
of the science and the
1:26:28
research findings that unravel how
1:26:30
we can better tap this
1:26:32
mind-body domain. And that's really
1:26:34
the point that I'm trying
1:26:36
to make. Yep. Well, you
1:26:39
make it brilliantly and very
1:26:41
readable. Here it is again,
1:26:43
the suggestible brain, the science
1:26:45
and magic of how we
1:26:47
make up our minds. It's
1:26:49
a fun read. Thanks, Amir.
1:26:51
Thanks for the book. Thanks
1:26:53
for your work. Thanks for
1:26:55
talking to me for so
1:26:57
long. And yeah, good, good,
1:26:59
good, good show. Thank you
1:27:01
Michael. I love your show
1:27:03
and it's an honor to
1:27:06
be speaking with you. I
1:27:08
really appreciate the opportunity and
1:27:10
I hope that my contribution
1:27:12
would actually tickle some of
1:27:14
the people out there. Oh,
1:27:16
I think it will for
1:27:18
sure. After investing billions to
1:27:20
light up our network, T-Mobile
1:27:22
is America's largest 5G network.
1:27:24
Plus, right now, you can
1:27:26
switch, keep your phone, and
1:27:28
we'll pay it off up
1:27:30
to $800. See how you
1:27:32
can save on every plan
1:27:35
versus Verizon and AT&T at
1:27:37
t-mobile.com/keep and switch. Up to
1:27:39
four lines via virtual prepaid,
1:27:41
virtual prepaid card, a left
1:27:43
15 days qualifying unlocked device
1:27:45
credit service, credit, service, and
1:27:47
90 plus days with device
1:27:49
and eligible carrier. When President
1:27:51
Trump needed someone to fix
1:27:53
our military, he turned to
1:27:55
Pete Hexeth, the man who
1:27:57
literally wrote the book. book
1:27:59
it. Veteran, Christian,
1:28:02
and Ivy League graduate, Hexith wrote
1:28:04
the number one York Times Times
1:28:06
The War The Warriors, revealing
1:28:08
his career, his concerns, and
1:28:10
his solutions. solutions. Available now
1:28:12
in hardcover and digital audio
1:28:14
from Fox Fox News books.
Podchaser is the ultimate destination for podcast data, search, and discovery. Learn More