Longevity roundtable — the science of aging, geroprotective molecules, lifestyle interventions, challenges in research, and more | Steven Austad, Matt Kaeberlein, Richard Miller

Longevity roundtable — the science of aging, geroprotective molecules, lifestyle interventions, challenges in research, and more | Steven Austad, Matt Kaeberlein, Richard Miller

Released Monday, 27th January 2025
Good episode? Give it some love!
Longevity roundtable — the science of aging, geroprotective molecules, lifestyle interventions, challenges in research, and more | Steven Austad, Matt Kaeberlein, Richard Miller

Longevity roundtable — the science of aging, geroprotective molecules, lifestyle interventions, challenges in research, and more | Steven Austad, Matt Kaeberlein, Richard Miller

Longevity roundtable — the science of aging, geroprotective molecules, lifestyle interventions, challenges in research, and more | Steven Austad, Matt Kaeberlein, Richard Miller

Longevity roundtable — the science of aging, geroprotective molecules, lifestyle interventions, challenges in research, and more | Steven Austad, Matt Kaeberlein, Richard Miller

Monday, 27th January 2025
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:09

Hey everyone, welcome to the Drive

0:12

podcast. I'm your host Peter Atia. This

0:14

podcast, my website, and my weekly newsletter

0:16

all focus on the goal of translating

0:18

the science of longevity into something accessible

0:21

for everyone. Our goal is to provide

0:23

the best content in health and wellness

0:25

and we've established a great team of

0:28

analysts to make this happen. It is

0:30

extremely important to me to provide all

0:32

of this content without relying on paid

0:34

ads. To do this, our work is

0:37

made entirely possible by our members, and

0:39

in return, we offer exclusive, member-only

0:41

content and benefits above and beyond

0:43

what is available for free. If

0:45

you want to take your knowledge

0:47

of this space to the next

0:49

level, it's our goal to ensure

0:52

members get back much more than

0:54

the price of the subscription. If

0:56

you want to learn more about

0:58

the benefits of our premium membership,

1:00

head over to Peter atiamd.com forward

1:02

slash subscribe. Welcome to

1:05

a special episode of The

1:07

Drive. Today we're introducing a

1:09

new format to the podcast.

1:11

It's our inaugural roundtable conversation.

1:14

For this one, we have

1:16

gathered three brilliant minds, all

1:18

former guests of the podcast,

1:21

to sit down and have

1:23

a... nuanced, funny, sometimes a

1:25

little heated discussion about one

1:27

of the most fascinating and

1:29

rapidly evolving areas of medicine

1:31

today. Geoscience, also known, I

1:34

guess, as longevity science. So

1:36

joining me for this episode,

1:38

are doctors Steve Osted, an

1:40

expert in aging biology and

1:42

author of groundbreaking research on

1:44

extending health span. Richard Miller,

1:47

pioneer of the study of

1:49

anti-aging interventions, through the interventions

1:51

testing program, or ITP, which

1:53

you hear me reference a

1:55

lot, and Matt Caberlin, whose

1:57

expertise explores the intersection of

1:59

genetics, aging, and translational research, and

2:01

Matt, of course, is famous for

2:03

his work in the Dog Aging

2:05

Project. So in today's roundtable,

2:07

we discuss a number of things,

2:09

such as the relationship between health

2:12

span and lifespan. And what does

2:14

health span actually mean? Is it

2:16

something we should try to define?

2:18

Can you improve one without improving

2:20

the other? What has caused a surge

2:22

in the public interest in longevity science

2:25

and what major barriers are preventing longevity

2:27

research from reaching its full potential? This

2:29

actually was one of my favorite parts

2:32

of the discussion. How do we evaluate

2:34

the effectiveness of interventions like rapomycin,

2:36

synalytics, or calorie restriction in humans,

2:38

where it's very difficult to study

2:41

them for obvious reasons? Are there reliable biomarkers

2:43

or aging rate indicators that can measure

2:45

biologic aging, which of course is a

2:47

very hot topic? What role do

2:49

epigenetic changes play in aging?

2:51

Specifically, are they causal? Are

2:53

senescent cells a valid target

2:55

for longevity interventions? Or has

2:57

their role in aging been

2:59

overstated? Are GLP1 receptor agonists,

3:02

for example, drugs like

3:04

terzepotide and semaglutide, potentially

3:06

geroprotective beyond just their

3:08

weight loss effects? How

3:10

do we overcome the funding

3:13

and political challenges that prioritize

3:15

disease-specific research over foundational aging

3:18

science? What would it take to

3:20

make longevity research more mainstream and

3:22

gain broader support from the public

3:24

and policy makers? Anyway, this is

3:26

a new format, this idea of

3:29

doing a roundtable, so we really

3:31

want to hear from you. Is

3:33

it something you like? If so,

3:35

what are other topics you would

3:37

like to see for roundtables? So,

3:39

without further delay, please enjoy this

3:42

roundtable discussion with Steve Osted,

3:44

Rich Miller, and Matt Kaverlin.

3:46

This is a lot of fun. I am

3:48

excited to be sitting down with you guys

3:50

today. Where do we want to begin? Let

3:53

me start by saying the following. The

3:55

term longevity, someone sent me something the

3:57

other day that was like list of,

3:59

I don't know. whether it was how many

4:01

times the word longevity was searched on

4:03

Google or something like that, but it

4:05

literally looks like Bitcoin. So we are

4:07

clearly at peak longevity in terms of

4:09

public interest, which for all of you

4:12

who have kind of devoted decades plural to

4:14

this, I just want to kind of get

4:16

a reaction from you, each of you, on

4:18

what that means, why you think it's happening

4:20

and maybe even extending the metaphor a little

4:22

bit. Is there a bubble going on? We'll

4:25

start with you Steve. It's a surprise to me

4:27

that longevity has become so big because for

4:29

a long time we try to move away

4:31

from that in the aging field because we

4:33

were worried that people were thinking of

4:35

longevity as well. We're going to keep

4:38

frail, feeble, old people alive longer. That's

4:40

what longevity meant. When really what we're

4:42

trying to do is extend health. So I'm

4:44

kind of surprised, but I think it's

4:46

because there are certain people of a

4:49

certain age who've started to think about

4:51

their own longevity. And then I think

4:53

there's a whole new generation of tech

4:55

entrepreneurs that really feel like this is

4:58

a problem that will allow them to

5:00

live healthily for several decades at

5:02

least longer than they are now.

5:04

So I think it's a combination.

5:06

It's a multi-generational thing. That kind

5:08

of surprises me. And you haven't seen

5:10

this before to be clear. So 30

5:12

years ago you didn't see glimmers of

5:14

this? No, 30 years ago I would

5:17

have said, let's not even say the

5:19

word longevity. Let's say health span. That's

5:21

changed quite clearly as more and more

5:23

people have been from the outside. They're

5:25

sort of peaking in at the field.

5:27

I don't think the people in the

5:29

field itself have changed the way they

5:31

talk that much, but the people eavesdropping

5:33

on the field certainly have. Rich, is

5:35

that your experience? Well, I think there

5:38

are two aspects that I would want

5:40

to emphasize in response to your question, and

5:42

to what Steve said. People have always

5:44

been fascinated for millennia on things they

5:46

could do to stay alive and healthy

5:49

as long as possible. But there were

5:51

actually scientific discoveries in the 90s

5:53

that showed that it could be done.

5:55

And then in the last 20 years there's

5:57

evidence that it can be done at

5:59

least. in mice with pills. So that

6:01

naturally should lead to

6:04

speculation that there could be pills

6:06

you could give to people that

6:08

would postpone poor health for

6:10

a substantial amount of time.

6:12

20 to 30% is what we're seeing

6:15

in mice and 20 to 30% would

6:17

be very important for people. So

6:19

I think that is a part of it.

6:21

The other part is that... They're

6:23

now people who are making a lot of

6:26

money by selling stuff that is untested to

6:28

be polite about it or is useless to

6:30

be less polite about it to gullible

6:32

customers. And so people who want

6:34

to make a lot of money

6:36

have finally found that there's an

6:38

impetus that will allow them to

6:40

sell stuff even if there's no

6:42

evidence that it works, that they

6:44

control an enormous amount of advertising

6:46

dollars, both formal and informal. That's

6:48

a big part of the difference.

6:50

The one comment I wanted to make

6:53

with regard to something Steve said

6:55

has to do with the alleged

6:57

balance between health span and

6:59

life span. It's become fashionable

7:01

for the last 20 or 30 years

7:03

to imagine that you get one or

7:05

the other, that you have to make

7:07

a choice. It's a decision, and that

7:09

if you give up on life span,

7:11

that allows you to extend health

7:14

span. I think that's ridiculous

7:16

and controverted by all

7:19

the available evidence. That is, all

7:21

of the drugs at least that extend

7:23

lifespan in mice and could potentially

7:25

do so in people, do so

7:27

by postponing diseases. Both the diseases

7:29

that will kill you, that's why

7:31

they extend lifespan, and the diseases

7:33

that won't kill you, but which

7:35

will annoy you, and make you

7:37

very unhappy to be old. Which

7:39

is true, by the way, of

7:41

non-molecular tools as well. Yes, absolutely.

7:43

Yeah, absolutely. That's a good point.

7:45

Not being insulin resistant. I agree

7:47

with you. So the notion it's time to put

7:49

behind us and to make fun of the

7:51

notion that I'm not interested in

7:54

lifespan, don't put me on that boat.

7:56

I am interested in health spend because

7:58

they are linked together. and they

8:00

go up and down together, getting

8:02

people disabused of that false metaphor,

8:05

the seesaw metaphor is

8:07

probably an important goal

8:09

for the public interface

8:11

between longevity scientists, aging

8:13

scientists. Now I just want to

8:15

push on one thing though, you

8:17

talked about obviously the discoveries of

8:19

molecules, you've been personally

8:21

central to that work, but there

8:23

was still a lag, rich. I mean it was...

8:26

15 years ago, the first

8:28

ITP was published showing the

8:30

overwhelmingly surprising and positive results

8:33

of rapomycin. Those results were

8:35

repeated. Why a decade, let's be

8:37

generous and charitable and call it

8:39

a still decade-long lag from that.

8:41

And by the way, I'll throw

8:43

one more thing in there. If

8:45

you go back to Cynthia Kenyon's

8:47

work, which may have been... the

8:49

thin end of the wedge into

8:51

the idea that lifespan was malleable,

8:54

albeit through a genetic manipulation in

8:56

a less relevant model. Yeah. There's

8:58

still a lag. Do you buy

9:00

Steve's argument that it's a confluence

9:02

of technology, tech entrepreneurs? Let

9:04

me answer your question first.

9:06

Why the lag? I think there's

9:08

a whole batch of reasons and

9:10

they're important and they're easy to spell

9:12

out. One is the prevailing attitude is

9:14

that aging is there. There's nothing

9:16

you can do about it. I'm gonna... not

9:19

be able to outwit aiding, though I may

9:21

be able to be maybe healthier in

9:23

my older years. The notion that aging

9:25

is not malleable, though wrong and

9:28

provably wrong, is still

9:30

the overwhelming opinion even

9:32

of reasonably educated scientists

9:34

and certainly of the lay public.

9:36

Then commercially, there are companies

9:38

that make a ton of money selling

9:41

stuff that doesn't work by pretending with

9:43

a wink and a nod and a

9:45

lawyer that it might... slow the aging

9:47

process down. And since they can make

9:50

a lot of money, they don't actually

9:52

have to spend valuable marketing

9:54

dollars on doing research and

9:56

stuff to prove that it works.

9:58

Some of the drugs... that at least

10:00

in the hands of our mouse

10:03

group, the ITP interventions testing program,

10:05

some of the drugs are the patent

10:07

is owned by another company or they're

10:09

out of patent or it's a natural

10:11

product, none of that says take me

10:13

to whoever owns a big pharmaceutical firm.

10:16

And also even if you do it right

10:18

and you really want to do it and you've

10:20

got a very large budget, it's not

10:22

an overnight kind of thing. Any one

10:24

drug, a leading agent that like rapidism

10:26

which you mentioned in the... half a

10:28

dozen others that we've shown work, at

10:31

least in mice, finding something in

10:33

that same family that works really

10:35

well, that is safe for people, that's

10:37

the member of the 20 congeners

10:39

of that drug that's best and

10:41

most potent and safest, that's not

10:44

at all trivial. That takes a

10:46

long time and it takes a

10:48

commitment of money and time and

10:50

effort and intellectual resources, where the

10:52

place where we can start to make

10:55

an argument that that's a good idea.

10:57

But make a good argument that that's

10:59

a good idea to people who

11:01

actually have the resources to carry

11:03

it out as not so far

11:05

been enormously successful, unfortunately. Can I

11:08

push back a little on

11:10

what Rich said about health

11:12

span versus lifespan? Several papers

11:14

have come out recently showing that

11:16

the gap between health span and

11:18

lifespan in people is actually

11:20

increasing, and it's increasing faster

11:22

among women than men. So in humans,

11:25

this is a very real gap, and it's

11:27

a growing gap, and I think one of

11:29

the advantages of the kind of geroscience, the

11:31

stuff that we do, is that Rich is

11:33

right. We don't see this in our experimental

11:36

systems. So this to me emphasizes the

11:38

fact that we need to change the focus.

11:40

I think one of the reasons that the

11:42

gap exists is we're getting better and better

11:44

and better and better at treating heart

11:46

disease and cancer and all these things

11:48

and keeping people alive. when they wouldn't have

11:51

been alive 10 years ago. But this is

11:53

a really important factor, I think, about thinking

11:55

of public health globally. But I think you're

11:57

both right. I think you're looking at it

11:59

from different... So Steve, you're pointing out

12:01

that you can make people live longer when

12:03

they're sick. I think what Rich is

12:05

saying, which I agree with, hopefully I'm

12:07

going to paraphrase you correctly, which is if

12:09

we target the biology of aging, I

12:11

haven't seen anything to make me believe

12:14

that you can separate health span and

12:16

lifespan, meaning that I haven't seen things

12:18

that slow aging, increase lifespan, don't increase

12:20

health span. I don't actually think that's

12:22

plausible. And I think that's an important

12:24

point, that if we're doing something different.

12:27

then with the way that medicine is

12:29

operating now, which is targeting individual diseases,

12:31

after they occur. This is a very

12:33

important point. It came up in a

12:35

recent podcast that I did with Sam

12:38

Sutaria, talking about health care costs. And

12:40

in that discussion, one of the

12:42

things that emerged, which I think most

12:44

people are sadly familiar with this statistic

12:46

today, is that among the OECD nations,

12:48

the United States has the lowest

12:50

life expectancy. which is ironic given that

12:52

we are spending on average about 80%

12:55

more in some cases double what most

12:57

other developed nations spend on health care.

12:59

So how do you reconcile this?

13:01

Well some made a very interesting

13:03

point which is that's aggregate life

13:06

expectancy, but why is that the case?

13:08

That's because the United States has by

13:10

far the greatest rate of death in

13:12

middle age. So when you look at

13:14

maternal and infant mortality, we're horrible.

13:17

When you look at gun violence...

13:19

and suicide and homicide were horrible,

13:21

and most of all when you

13:23

look at overdoses were horrible. When

13:25

you kill a whole bunch of people

13:27

in their 40s and 50s, you cannot

13:29

have a very high life expectancy. Understood.

13:32

But what Sam pointed out was, once

13:34

an American reaches the age of, and I

13:36

forget the exact age, I think it was

13:38

about 65, all of a sudden they jumped to

13:40

the top of the list. That was

13:42

very interesting to me. In other words,

13:45

if you look at the blended... life expectancy,

13:47

we're not doing very well. But if you

13:49

look at life expectancy, just inmeasured as years

13:51

alive, once you escape those big causes of

13:53

death in middle age, we actually do quite

13:56

well. And it comes down to what you're

13:58

saying, which is we get very... good at

14:00

delaying death in chronic disease. That's what

14:02

I call the Medicine 2.0 machine at

14:05

its absolute finest. We are going to

14:07

keep you along an extra six months

14:09

once you have cancer. We are going

14:11

to get you through that third revascularization

14:14

procedure. And so now the question is,

14:16

because my intuition is where yours is

14:18

Steve, I don't think we're getting any

14:20

healthier. Even if we're incrementally figuring out

14:23

ways to extend life in the face

14:25

of chronic disease, I don't see it

14:27

being a quality of life. Part of

14:29

this might be, how do we define

14:31

health span? Yeah. I agree with you.

14:34

And I think it's even worse, though,

14:36

than the way you laid it out.

14:38

So if you look at the statistics,

14:40

if you accept that 60% of Americans

14:43

have at least one chronic disease, and

14:45

the median age in the United States,

14:47

and the 60% of Americans have at

14:49

least one chronic disease, and the median

14:52

age in the definition of health span,

14:54

I would not define health span as

14:56

ending once you have your first chronic

14:58

disease, but that's... The definition most people

15:01

would use, if you use that definition,

15:03

most people are spending three decades or

15:05

more in the absence of health span,

15:07

or in six span. So the situation

15:10

is, even in the United States, where

15:12

life expectancy is relatively short compared to

15:14

other nations, a big chunk of that

15:16

life expectancy is not spent in good

15:19

health. And it's exactly for this reason.

15:21

But the two different issues that are

15:23

being confused here in the discussion... One

15:25

is the issue of whether you can

15:28

help middle-aged people live longer. And everybody's

15:30

agreed that we're getting better at that,

15:32

we're pretty good at it. And that

15:34

certainly contributes to whatever you think health

15:37

span might mean. That's an issue, however,

15:39

that is quite different from a concoction

15:41

that slows aging, do so by extending

15:43

health span. Those both have the word

15:45

health span in them. But they're different

15:48

and shouldn't ever be confused with one

15:50

another. The other point in this question

15:52

you asked us what is... health span.

15:54

My own personal answer to that is

15:57

it's a useless term. That is because

15:59

no one can define it. It's not

16:01

because no one is smart, it's because

16:03

the term itself is vacuous and nebulous.

16:06

If you have somebody that gets a

16:08

certain chronic disease here and then another

16:10

one and then they fall down and

16:12

bump their head and by the way

16:15

they go to the hospital and with

16:17

COVID etc. etc. etc. defining when in

16:19

that 20 to 30 year period They

16:21

flicked the switch, now they have gotten

16:24

to the end of the health spend,

16:26

is impossible. And of no interest. The

16:28

general notion that people are interested in

16:30

is whether you can do stuff to

16:33

keep people healthy for a long time,

16:35

either without changing their life expectancy or

16:37

by changing their life. Those are interesting,

16:39

but you don't have to assign a

16:42

number, a health spend digit. I don't

16:44

like the medical definition of health span,

16:46

which I believe is, quote, the period

16:48

of time in which an individual is

16:51

free of disability and disease. I find

16:53

that to be a very unhelpful definition.

16:55

Yeah. But part of the reason it's

16:57

awful is it's binary. Yeah, exactly. You

16:59

got it. But if we made it

17:02

analog instead of digital, I'm not saying

17:04

that makes it easy. It's still very

17:06

challenging. But now it allows us to

17:08

start talking about things. Except it's a

17:11

concept. It's a qualitative. to where we

17:13

can actually come up with a way

17:15

to measure, whether we call it health

17:17

span or not, that doesn't really matter.

17:20

I kind of agree with Rich, like

17:22

I agree with what you're saying, except

17:24

I think it's a really useful term

17:26

as a concept. I think it's a

17:29

really useful way to communicate to a

17:31

broader audience what one of the goals

17:33

is, which is to increase the healthy

17:35

period of life. I kind of like

17:38

the term health for health. And you

17:40

don't have to pretend you can define

17:42

it as a number. But I think

17:44

we all could agree. There's a period

17:47

of life where you are in relatively

17:49

good health. And then there's a period

17:51

of life where you aren't. And so

17:53

I think the idea that we're trying

17:56

to increase that component of life is

17:58

really important. So I don't think we're

18:00

actually. disagreeing on much other than whether

18:02

we like the word. Well I also

18:05

think there's an individualization of this that

18:07

we're missing. To me health is a

18:09

state of your physical being that you

18:11

can do the things you like to

18:14

do. Therefore if you like to climb

18:16

mountains your health span is going to

18:18

be different than if you like to

18:20

play golf for instance and a lot

18:22

of this is personal. If you can't

18:25

run a marathon anymore some people will

18:27

say oh my health is... And we

18:29

never pay attention to the mental health

18:31

piece, at least the biologists. So I

18:34

have a question for you, Steve. What

18:36

is my health span? I would only

18:38

be able to ask you that. So

18:40

we do this exercise, guys, because I

18:43

completely agree with you, Steve. We call

18:45

it the marginal decade exercise. So we

18:47

say to every one of our patients,

18:49

and I write about this a lot

18:52

in the book, everyone will have a

18:54

marginal decade, which I define as the

18:56

last decade of your life. So obviously

18:58

by definition, everyone has a marginal decade.

19:01

Everyone has a marginal decade. But most

19:03

people have a pretty good sense when

19:05

they're in it. Okay, so the exercise

19:07

we do is we go through with

19:10

the patient and we say, what are

19:12

the things that are most important to

19:14

you to be able to do in

19:16

your marginal decade? And they generally fall

19:19

into three buckets with a sub bucket.

19:21

Physical, cognitive, emotional, social. The physical bucket

19:23

we kind of divide into activities of

19:25

daily living and recreational activities. So that's

19:28

where, again, most people obviously into it

19:30

into it that... Boy, I would really

19:32

not be happy if I couldn't take

19:34

care of myself. If I couldn't get

19:36

out of bed, get dressed, shave, cook,

19:39

that would be disappointing to me. But

19:41

then, of course, you have different levels

19:43

of ambition within the recreational side. I've

19:45

got patients who say, when the day

19:48

comes that I can't helly ski, I'm

19:50

going to be devastated. And other people

19:52

are like, I just want to be

19:54

able to garden. That's going to create

19:57

a very different standard. On the cognitive

19:59

side, you have people who say, I

20:01

want to be able to run my

20:03

hedge fund and still make money and

20:06

make really important investment decisions. And other

20:08

people are like, I want to be

20:10

able to do crossword puzzles and read

20:12

the newspaper. I agree with you. You

20:15

can't define it, but it doesn't mean...

20:17

we shouldn't try to personalize it. Okay,

20:19

but I want to come back to

20:21

you Matt with the original question. Why

20:24

are we at a point where? Why

20:26

is longevity gone mainstream? Yeah, for a

20:28

lot better way. Yeah, so I mean

20:30

I think both of the points that

20:33

Stephen Rich raised are part of the

20:35

equation. I mean I think it's a

20:37

convergence of all of these factors and

20:39

maybe a few others. I do think

20:42

the science has matured to the point

20:44

where more people are believing that we

20:46

can actually modulate. the biology of aging.

20:48

I think the concept of biological aging

20:50

has become popularized through a variety of

20:53

mechanisms, including some influencers, individuals who I

20:55

personally think often err on the side

20:57

of being a little bit less scientific

20:59

than they should be, but I think

21:02

they've helped popularize the concept. So I

21:04

think it's been a combination of these

21:06

factors. And why it has taken so

21:08

long? I mean, I just think that's

21:11

the pace that science moves and the

21:13

rate at which these concepts can sort

21:15

of permeate the public... So it's frustrating

21:17

in a sense that it's moved so

21:20

slowly. I also wonder, because you sort

21:22

of said, are we at a longevity

21:24

bubble? I don't know. I think maybe

21:26

we're still kind of in the early

21:29

days of this hockey stick moment, where

21:31

you're getting this exponential increase in attention.

21:33

My hope is, as we go forward,

21:35

it will become more scientific and less

21:38

snake oily, and it's a spectrum. There's

21:40

this huge gray area in the field

21:42

right now of what's real and what's

21:44

not real, and I think none of

21:47

us at this at this table, actually.

21:49

can really define exactly where in that

21:51

gray area that line is or is

21:53

there a line? To that point, Matt,

21:56

what is the collective wisdom of the

21:58

group on the funding appetite for that?

22:00

Because I agree with you completely, like

22:02

if we could channel this exuberance away

22:05

from kind of the highly commercial speculative

22:07

grifting towards the budget increasing legitimate investigative,

22:09

that would be awesome. What is the

22:11

appetite right now of NIA with respect?

22:13

back to this? I think it's hard

22:16

to say it. I mean, NIH is

22:18

a moving target, and as we all

22:20

know, there's going to be a lot

22:22

of change coming in the near future.

22:25

So, cautiously optimistic, I would say, if

22:27

you look historically, it's been really pretty

22:29

terrible. The percent of NIH budget that

22:31

goes to biology of aging, I think

22:34

is still probably around half of 1%.

22:36

Sorry, just to put numbers in perspective,

22:38

fraction that goes to biology of aging.

22:40

Yes, yes, yes, yes. Yeah. But I'm

22:43

saying there are 17 groups of NIH.

22:45

NIA being one of them gets what

22:47

fraction of NIAH budget roughly? I think

22:49

it's roughly 3%. 3% of NIA budget

22:52

is NIA. Within NIA, how much goes

22:54

to this type of research? It was

22:56

about 350 million a few years ago.

22:58

It might be a little higher than

23:01

that, but I don't think it's ticked

23:03

up any more proportional to the increase

23:05

in the increase in NIA budget since

23:07

then since then. What's your level of

23:10

optimism, Rich? You're obviously very close to

23:12

this. That NIH will wake up and

23:14

start to pay attention to aging research

23:16

the way they should? It's near zero.

23:19

It's been near zero for 30 years

23:21

now. Even with this outside attention? Well,

23:23

it's gone up. I mean, they funded

23:25

the ITP, the interventions testing program, 20

23:27

years ago, and they liked it, and

23:30

they doubled our budget about 15 years

23:32

ago. So that's something. And I'm very,

23:34

very grateful to them for that, but

23:36

there's still an enormous untapped potential for

23:39

making progress in the basic biology of

23:41

aging. And the reason is, again, a

23:43

matter of defending turf. If you are

23:45

a cardiologist researcher or an oncologist researcher

23:48

or an AIDS researcher or an Alzheimer's

23:50

researcher, any time somebody says, the smart

23:52

play is to reduce your budget by

23:54

10 percent or your institute's budget by

23:57

10 percent. We're going to go there

23:59

faster if we spend money on aging

24:01

and its relationship to the disease you

24:03

care about. You get the porcupine defense.

24:06

You don't take any of my money

24:08

because Alzheimer's is important. little kids with

24:10

leukemia are important, breast cancer is important,

24:12

you go away. And that is the

24:15

predominant feeling. Most of the people making

24:17

those decisions were not trained in aging

24:19

research, they view it as something interesting,

24:21

I read something about that in Time

24:24

Magazine the other day, but they don't

24:26

understand that to actually conquer or slow

24:28

down or effect or protect against the

24:30

disease they care about, the smart play

24:33

is to do aging research. And so

24:35

they view your suggestion, which I of

24:37

course agree with 100% as an imposition,

24:39

an invasion to be repelled at any

24:41

cost. No one in a position of

24:44

power has had whatever it takes to

24:46

reverse that. And if he or she

24:48

tried to do that, Congress would even

24:50

a good Congress would smack them down.

24:53

The Alzheimer's group has 100 lobbyists, the

24:55

cancer group has 100 lobbyists, the AIDS

24:57

group has 100 lobbyists. The aging group

24:59

has two lobbyists, one who's a lawyer

25:02

and one who takes the calls, and

25:04

that's not enough to do it. Can

25:06

I just add something real quick? I

25:08

agree completely. And I think, as well,

25:11

the reputation of the field has hindered

25:13

that transition as well. So historically, the

25:15

field was viewed as not very mechanistic,

25:17

kind of phenomenological, became much more mechanistic,

25:20

starting around the time of Cynthia Canyon's

25:22

work, and since then. but has continued

25:24

to have a reputation problem as not

25:26

being as rigorous as other areas of

25:29

research. So I think it is absolutely

25:31

a turf for, and there's this overcoming

25:33

the reputational problem which makes it harder

25:35

for serious people in funding and policy

25:38

circles to give it the attention it

25:40

deserves, in my opinion. So I've got

25:42

a different take on this. I actually

25:44

think that this is a very good

25:47

time for aging research funding, and that's

25:49

not because of what's going on at

25:51

the NIA. but it's what's going on

25:53

in the private sector. There's more and

25:55

more money. There's even interest now in

25:58

big pharma that was very spotty in

26:00

the past. So I think if We

26:02

focused entirely on the Nationalist and on

26:04

aging. We would get a false impression

26:07

of what the funding climate is in

26:09

the field now. And I think we

26:11

need to take advantage of that. Got

26:13

to make sure that it doesn't get

26:16

captured by the people who are doing

26:18

the fleshy, but bad science. You're saying,

26:20

look, Calico, Altos, other private companies, especially

26:22

within biotech and pharma that are looking

26:25

at geoprot protective molecules building on the

26:27

work of the ITP. Yeah, I think

26:29

it's safe to say the amount of

26:31

money that's being spent privately probably out

26:34

does public spending, I mean in a

26:36

given year two to one easily. It

26:38

could, although how much of that is

26:40

actually going to biology of age? I

26:43

think it's still an open question. You

26:45

mentioned Calico and Alton's right? Exactly. I

26:47

actually agree with Steve. I don't think

26:49

what Rich and I were communicating as

26:52

opposed to what Steve was communicating. There

26:54

are a lot of this hockey stick

26:56

moment. And I think Steve's right, there

26:58

are real opportunities for more resources to

27:01

be focused on the scientific side and

27:03

hopefully less focused on the non-scientific aspects

27:05

of what are going on. And you

27:07

asked the question of can we shift

27:10

resources from the more consumer-facing, maybe not

27:12

as rigorous stuff, and into the more

27:14

rigorous stuff. I'm not a fan of

27:16

that stuff at all. But maybe you

27:18

need that stuff to kind of move

27:21

the needle and get people's attention, and

27:23

at least people are talking about longevity

27:25

now. Now you've questioned I'm embarrassed I

27:27

don't know the answer because I spent

27:30

more than two years working there What's

27:32

the mission statement of the NIH? It's

27:34

to preserve and enhance human health I

27:36

mean it's basically the same thing that

27:39

we do that we're supposed to enhance

27:41

human health I mean it's basically the

27:43

same thing that we do that we're

27:45

supposed to be doing Yeah and I

27:48

didn't actually get to give you my

27:50

spiel here but what I started to

27:52

say about the NIA budget is if

27:54

you look at the United States Nine

27:57

of them have biological aging as their

27:59

greatest risk. factor and it's not even

28:01

close. Yet half of one percent of

28:03

the research budget that's supposed to

28:05

be focused on improving human health

28:07

goes to study that risk factor. I

28:10

mean I think it is extremely frustrating to

28:12

all of us sitting at this

28:14

table that that hasn't changed but

28:16

there's reason to be optimistic that

28:19

maybe it will change in the near future.

28:21

Let's state that again because it is

28:23

so profound I want to make sure

28:25

not a single person missed that statement.

28:27

The top 10 causes of death

28:30

in the United States are

28:32

well enumerated and incredibly predictable,

28:35

and they increase by category,

28:37

by decade, 3 to 8

28:39

percent, monotonically, with no exception.

28:42

Point being, 90 percent of, and

28:44

more than 90 percent on an

28:46

adjusted basis of what causes death,

28:49

goes up with age. And yet, a

28:51

few basis points of federal

28:53

R&D goes to addressing that.

28:55

Let me give you an example of what

28:57

the sort of point that Matthew and

28:59

you have been making. About once every

29:01

five years, I give a talk and

29:04

invite a talk at the University of

29:06

Michigan Cancer Center, and I point out

29:08

that we have drugs now, anti-aging

29:10

drugs, in mice, and they extend mouse

29:12

lifespan, and they do it mostly

29:14

by postponing cancer, because most of

29:16

our mice die of cancer. And if you

29:19

look at age-adjusted cancer incidence rates,

29:21

our drugs reduce these by a

29:23

factor of 10. Wouldn't they like

29:25

to know why? As cancer scientists,

29:27

we now have a batch of

29:29

drugs that postpone cancer. Wouldn't they

29:31

like to study them? Invariably, I get

29:34

one call back from somebody who says,

29:36

that's interesting, maybe we should talk

29:38

about that, and then it dies, and

29:40

then five years later, I'm asked to give

29:42

the same talk or related talk. So they

29:45

know how to do cancer research, they are

29:47

cancer scientists, that's how they know how

29:49

to do cancer research, and you certainly

29:51

don't do it by... diverting your

29:54

lab's attention to aging. That's

29:56

insane, but that insanity is

29:58

how medical research is organized

30:00

and breaking that addiction to

30:02

the kinds of models you grew up on

30:04

because there are a better idea, it's not

30:07

an easy thing. It may not even be

30:09

a possible thing to do. That's a

30:11

major hassle. I think this is because

30:13

we think about health all wrong. We

30:15

think, let's wait to get cancer and

30:17

see what we can do about it.

30:19

That's what cancer biologists do. You have

30:21

cancer, okay, how can we better treat

30:24

that? Or could we have diagnosed it

30:26

earlier? What Rich is saying and what

30:28

we can know how to do in

30:30

lots of model organ, it prevents you

30:32

from getting cancer, delay it for a

30:34

considerable amount of time, that's a little

30:36

bit harder to study if you're a

30:38

cancer biologist, because you want to see

30:41

the cancer before you can study it.

30:43

I think that's why we need aging

30:45

biologists, rather than people focused on certain

30:47

disease to come and try to use

30:49

what we do. If we prevented the

30:51

cancers, they'd be out of a job.

30:53

people or mice will get cancer. They'll just

30:56

have 10 extra years of life if they're

30:58

a person or 10 extra months of life.

31:00

They'll get cancer. They'll need specialists. It'll be

31:02

all right. Yeah, I think that's important. I

31:04

mean, I think the reactive disease care component

31:06

is still going to be there. Even if we're

31:08

insanely successful at slowing agent, people are still

31:10

going to get sick. But I think Steve's

31:13

point is really important. Like Peter, you've been

31:15

a leader in helping people recognize the need

31:17

to shift the need to shift the medical.

31:19

approach from reactive to proactive. I

31:21

think what a lot of people

31:23

don't realize is that mentality goes

31:25

all the way back to pharmaceutical

31:28

research, biomedical research, basic science. That

31:30

is ingrained all the way through.

31:32

And I think one of the

31:34

challenges with getting funding for aging

31:37

research is that mentality on the

31:39

basic science world and how deeply

31:41

ingrained it is. It's very interesting

31:43

because you don't know which is the tail

31:45

and which is the dog. I've always assumed

31:47

that the one leading the charge

31:50

is the clinical side of things.

31:52

In other words, the engine, the

31:54

machine of Medicine 2.0, is built

31:56

around the delivery of care, the

31:58

delivery of care. as you said,

32:01

Steve, is built around, I'm going

32:03

to wait, I'm going to sit

32:05

here and hang, we're going to

32:07

wait, when you get the disease,

32:09

we're ready. You have the heart

32:12

attack, fantastic. You've got chest pain,

32:14

ST elevations, we've got a

32:16

stint for you, now you have

32:18

cancer, we're all in. And then

32:21

the research flows from that mindset.

32:23

Of course, I don't know, not

32:25

that it really matters, but it

32:27

might be that it's flipped. matters,

32:29

but if you could be health czar and

32:31

fix one of them, you might actually

32:34

start with the research side of things.

32:36

I would. And I mean, the reality

32:38

is the research flows from where the

32:40

dollars are going. This has been seen

32:42

over and over and over at NIH.

32:44

You shift resource allocation to a certain

32:46

area and the scientists will follow and

32:49

they will submit grants to get grants

32:51

in the place where the funding line

32:53

is the highest. So if somebody came

32:55

along and said we're going to go

32:57

to biology of aging. You'd have no

32:59

shortage of people. I mean, it would be

33:01

kind of messy at first, but you'd have

33:03

no shortage of people applying for grants and

33:05

becoming experts in the biology of aging. And

33:08

the system would work. You'd get the best

33:10

and the brightest that would go into that

33:12

and do that. So this then begs another question

33:14

that is a tired question, but I can't

33:16

help but ask it at this point. Is

33:18

aging a disease? Is that even a

33:21

relevant question? Call me, calling me, calling

33:23

me, calling me. It's important to use

33:25

words, to use words optimally, optimally, optimally.

33:27

and to distinguish causes

33:29

from effects. One of the

33:31

bad things about aging is it's

33:33

a risk factor for many diseases.

33:36

Some things are other risk factors

33:38

for diseases. Aging is a risk

33:40

factor for disease. And so saying

33:42

that aging is a disease confuses

33:45

that discussion. It makes

33:47

it impossible to see that

33:49

relationship. So calling aging a

33:51

disease is a fundamental

33:53

error. The question itself is

33:55

uncorrect. I agree. But I

33:57

think we have that idea.

34:00

for marketing purposes, not for

34:02

scientific purposes. And the

34:04

idea is, well, the money goes

34:06

to diseases, let's call aging a

34:08

disease, because I think what we're

34:10

trying to do is we're trying to

34:12

treat aging as we're trying to do,

34:15

is we're trying to treat aging as if

34:17

it were a disease, even though I would

34:19

agree with both of you. I don't think

34:21

it's a disease. I think that destroys

34:24

the word disease if we include aging

34:26

in it. But I think there was

34:28

a reason that you can... convince people

34:30

of the importance of aging research only

34:32

by crossing your fingers and saying, oh,

34:34

well, it's kind of a disease, isn't

34:37

it? You think you can fool them?

34:39

Yes, that's what marketing is, and it's

34:41

probably good for that. I just don't

34:43

like lying to people. It also creates

34:45

a negative feeling about the field in

34:48

some people as well. So I think

34:50

that should be considering. The other point

34:52

that people often raise, though, is we have

34:54

to call aging a disease in order for

34:56

FDA to approve a fundamental... misunderstanding

34:58

of how FDA operates, but

35:01

that is the other argument

35:03

you will often hear among

35:05

proponents of the idea that aging is

35:08

a disease. Very interesting. Well, so

35:10

now let's go one step deeper

35:12

on that. How do you

35:14

think about biologic versus chronologic

35:17

age? In concept and in practice.

35:19

On the right over here, Rich and

35:21

I were talking about that, I

35:23

don't believe there is one thing

35:25

as biological age. I think there is potentially

35:27

an age of your heart, an age

35:29

of your liver, an age of your

35:31

lungs, an age of your brain, but

35:34

I don't see why we wouldn't simply

35:36

call it health. In other words, I

35:38

got one of these epigenetic age clocks

35:40

done on me a while ago,

35:42

but I didn't know what to make

35:44

out of it. I thought, is this just

35:46

flattery? Or did it really tell me something?

35:48

You must have got a good result. I

35:51

got a good result. He's 13. 13 years

35:53

old. That may be the point of the

35:55

whole thing, right? So I'm dubious about some

35:58

number that is different than I know. good

36:00

health, for my age I'm in very

36:02

good health. So I knew that already.

36:04

Now I have a number for it.

36:06

I don't put much credence in that.

36:09

Let me agree with Steve, but just

36:11

put it in slightly different terminology. It's

36:13

a matter of taking a very rich,

36:15

complex data set and trying to collapse

36:17

it to a number. So if someone

36:20

wants to know how healthy I am,

36:22

he or she would need information. How

36:24

good is my eyesight, how good is

36:26

my hearing, how good is various kinds

36:28

of cognitive activities. my aerobic endurance, my

36:31

joints, all of that is pertinent to

36:33

how my health is, and also about

36:35

projected future health, then there's no need

36:37

once you've got that information, which is

36:40

very rich, to say, ah, there's a

36:42

number, a single number, a real number,

36:44

a point on the number line that

36:46

condenses that in any useful way. A

36:48

notion 40, 50 years ago that biological

36:51

age was not the same as chronological

36:53

age, for a little while was useful,

36:55

it emphasized that there might well be.

36:57

60-year-old people who were unusually like youthful

36:59

people, and 60-year-old people who were unusually

37:02

like 70-year-old people, would my drug or

37:04

my genetic mutant or whatever, help to

37:06

discriminate those people or change them in

37:08

some way, I can slow your biological

37:10

aging process. That's a discussion that was

37:13

maybe of interest 40 years ago, and

37:15

it's now time to drop the notion,

37:17

let alone the silly notion. that you

37:19

can count that biological age, that number

37:21

which some people, too many people, still

37:24

think is of value. You can figure

37:26

out what it is by measuring something,

37:28

transcriptions or epigenetic markers or something. I

37:30

can do it and give you personally,

37:33

your personal biological age. That's a waste

37:35

of everyone's time, and it also distracts

37:37

attention from things that actually are important

37:39

and need to be thought about. I

37:41

gotta talk, because... I think I disagree

37:44

fundamentally, and I'm surprised, but this will

37:46

be an interesting conversation. So I agree

37:48

that the idea of a kit, the

37:50

you can buy to measure biological age.

37:52

First of all, the stuff that's out

37:55

there doesn't work, and we can and

37:57

should talk about that. But also, I

37:59

sort of agree with the idea that

38:01

reducing it to one number, while conceptually,

38:03

I think it's possible, I think in

38:06

reality, is gonna be really, really difficult

38:08

to do. But do I believe that

38:10

there is a biological aging process that

38:12

is different from? chronological aging. Absolutely. Oh

38:15

yes, absolutely. Okay. Well, it sounded like

38:17

you guys were both saying, no, you

38:19

didn't think it was a real thing.

38:21

I agree with that completely. You can

38:23

agree with that and not like the

38:26

idea of a number of the constitutes

38:28

your biological age. Okay. There's two things

38:30

that kind of make me feel pretty

38:32

confident in this idea. One is, and

38:34

this is the example I use a

38:37

lot among the general public, is just

38:39

look at dogs compared to people. Everybody's

38:41

compared to people. means that dogs age

38:43

about seven times faster than people do.

38:45

But of course chronological time is the

38:48

same between dogs and people. It's the

38:50

biological aging process. And so you can

38:52

look across the animal kingdom and see

38:54

this. And dogs get almost all of

38:56

the same diseases and functional declines that

38:59

we do at the tissue and organ

39:01

level, but also the whole body level.

39:03

We also know now there are single

39:05

genes that significantly modulate. what I would

39:08

call the rate of aging. Now maybe

39:10

we have a different meaning to what

39:12

we mean. No, I agree. Okay. So

39:14

the fact that that's possible. Deftu, we

39:16

talked about Deftu a couple times, Tor,

39:19

we can turn these things up, turn

39:21

them down, and animals across the evolutionary

39:23

spectrum seem to age at different rates

39:25

by modulating single genes. So I don't

39:27

know of any other explanation other than

39:30

that there is this process, which we

39:32

call biological aging, that... can be changed

39:34

and the rate can be sped up

39:36

or slowed down. Can it be reversed?

39:38

That's an interesting question. Maybe we'll get

39:41

to that. But I think the process

39:43

is real. I think it's just really

39:45

really complicated and we probably only understand

39:47

5% of it at this point. Yeah,

39:49

I think for me that challenges, I

39:52

kind of land where Rich was, which

39:54

is if a patient says to me,

39:56

hey, why aren't you doing this biologic

39:58

age clock on me, my response is,

40:01

well, I know your VO2 Max, I

40:03

know your zone 2, I know your

40:05

muscle mass, I know your visceral fat,

40:07

we did a very complicated movement assessment

40:09

on you, I understand your balance, I

40:12

understand your lipids, your insulin, like I

40:14

know these 57 things about you and

40:16

I can tell you individually on each

40:18

of them how you're doing, that number...

40:20

doesn't tell me a single new piece

40:23

of information. But what if you were

40:25

to come up, and you probably do

40:27

this in your head, you come up

40:29

with some sort of composite, you probably

40:31

don't sit down and wait each of

40:34

those things and come to one number,

40:36

but you come up with some sort

40:38

of composite picture of health based on

40:40

all of those things, that's a different

40:42

biological aging clock. I think sometimes we

40:45

conflate and in part this because of

40:47

the way that irresponsible people in the

40:49

field and marketers have done this, we

40:51

conflate the epigenetic tests. with biological aging

40:54

clocks. There are all sorts of flavors

40:56

of biological aging clocks, including things like

40:58

frailty indices or metrics of a whole

41:00

bunch of functional markers. So I think

41:02

those probably are pretty good readouts of

41:05

biological age. Again, can you combine them

41:07

all to get to one number that's

41:09

meaningful for every person? That's much harder

41:11

to do. Yeah, tell us about your

41:13

experience, because this was, you did what

41:16

I wanted to do, but I've been

41:18

too lazy to do. coming up with

41:20

different names. So what I did was

41:22

I tested four different direct-to-consumer biological age

41:24

kits. They were all epigenetic biological age

41:27

tests before different companies, and I did

41:29

duplicates of each kit, and it was

41:31

from the same samples collected on the

41:33

same date. I really tried to put

41:35

my scientist hat on. I only had

41:38

two replicates, I didn't have three replicates,

41:40

but it's about the best I could

41:42

afford at that point. And it was

41:44

kind of expensive. And it was kind

41:47

of expensive. to me, very informative, fundamentally

41:49

sort of changed my views on these

41:51

epigenetic age tests. So they ranged. from

41:53

42 to 63, I was 53.75 years

41:55

at the time I did the test,

41:58

and the standard deviation, I can't remember,

42:00

it was either 7 or 9. So

42:02

mean if my chronological age, standard deviation

42:04

of 7 or 9, which I look

42:06

at that data, I'm not a statistician,

42:09

but I know enough statistics to say

42:11

that's completely useless. They converged on my

42:13

chronological age, but with a huge variation.

42:15

Even intro... So that varied between the...

42:17

tests. So I think three of the

42:20

four were reasonably close to each other.

42:22

Three of the four companies, the duplicates

42:24

were reasonably close to each other, but

42:26

the individual tests were far apart, and

42:28

one of the companies, the individual replicates,

42:31

was 20 years apart. So to me,

42:33

and some people will say, but maybe

42:35

the true diagnostic test is great, and

42:37

the Elysium test is terrible, or the

42:40

tally health test is terrible, and the

42:42

other one is great. Maybe, but how

42:44

do we know? My take home is

42:46

that the direct to consumer biological age...

42:48

testing industry is a complete mess. And

42:51

I have no idea who to believe

42:53

or if any of them are actually

42:55

giving accurate data. I know some of

42:57

the people at some of the companies

42:59

and I have my personal feelings about

43:02

who's trying to do it right and

43:04

who's sort of a charlatan, but across

43:06

the industry it's really hard to know.

43:08

The last thing I'll say on this

43:10

is the where I've sort of landed

43:13

is I think these are really good

43:15

research tools. I think the direct to

43:17

consumer component has gotten way ahead of

43:19

itself and I think I aligning with

43:22

what you were saying about the way

43:24

you think about these tests. I don't

43:26

think there's a lot of value in

43:28

clinical practice right now, because we don't

43:30

know precision or accuracy, and I don't

43:33

think you can make actionable recommendations based

43:35

on these tests. Furthermore, they fail in

43:37

the one thing that I think they're

43:39

attempting to do, and I usually use

43:41

this illustration with patients. So if I

43:44

have a 40-year-old patient who says I

43:46

really want to do one of these

43:48

tests, I say, if the answer comes

43:50

back and says you're 20, Is your

43:52

expectation that you will live another seven...

43:55

years? Conversely, if the answer comes back

43:57

and says 60, is it your expectation

43:59

that you will live another 30 years?

44:01

In other words, is this number predictive

44:03

of future years of life? Because right

44:06

now we have this thing called chronological

44:08

age that is the single best predictor

44:10

of future years of life. So do

44:12

we think biologic age, as determined by

44:15

these tests, is better as a predictor

44:17

of future years of life? Which, by

44:19

the way, would be very testable. How

44:21

many people have contacted you? to get

44:23

ITP sample data to say, can we

44:26

predict how much longer these mice we're

44:28

going to live? The answer to the

44:30

question is obvious and very well known.

44:32

You can tell if you have your

44:34

40-year-old patient and he or she is

44:37

fat, doesn't exercise, it's mostly cheeseburgers, you

44:39

know that their life expectancy is probably

44:41

not as good as a 40-year-old patient

44:43

in your next waiting room that has

44:45

extremely healthful habits and whose parents live

44:48

to be 100. Tons of public stuff.

44:50

I don't need a biologic age to

44:52

tell them. Right, that's what I'm saying.

44:54

There are tons of things you can

44:56

measure on individuals, four or five of

44:59

them, or all you really need to

45:01

ask of a 70-year-old. Yeah, MetLife does

45:03

this really, really, really, really, because their

45:05

buddies all, they'll lie there, they're writing

45:08

life insurance policies. So it's not at

45:10

all hard to figure out a very

45:12

small set of tests to tell you

45:14

how long a seven-year-year-year-year-old is likely to

45:16

live, is likely to live, there's likely

45:19

to live, there's nothing to do with.

45:21

methylation clocks or things like that. That's

45:23

the gold standard. When life insurance companies

45:25

start using biologic clocks as the cornerstone

45:27

of their actuarial algorithms, I'll start to

45:30

be far away from that. I'm going

45:32

to sound like a broken record here,

45:34

but you guys keep saying biological age

45:36

when what you mean is epigenetic age

45:38

or epigenetic tests. Not necessarily. And we

45:41

should explain to people that there is

45:43

a difference. So some of these clocks

45:45

use solely epigenetic measurements. Not all, most

45:47

of the direct-to-consumer ones are epigenetic. But

45:49

some of these tests use a litany

45:52

of biomarkers inclusive of epigenetics. So they'll

45:54

say, we've sampled your meth. pattern, but

45:56

we also looked at your vitamin D

45:58

level, your glucose level, your cholesterol level,

46:01

and a whole bunch of other things,

46:03

and we compressed all of that into

46:05

a number as well. So I guess,

46:07

let me frame it as a question

46:09

to you. So let's take the epigenetic

46:12

piece out. Again, I do think we

46:14

will get to a point where the

46:16

technology is developed far enough and the

46:18

quality control is good enough on the

46:20

consumer side that these tests will be

46:23

better than just chronological age. That's a

46:25

big statement. I don't know that I'm

46:27

disagreeing with you. I just want to

46:29

make sure you understand the state. I

46:31

mean, I think it's clear from the

46:34

research, unless you think that all of

46:36

the research that's been done on these

46:38

epigenetic aging clocks is somehow flawed. It's

46:40

clear that you can create algorithms that

46:42

can predict specific methylation patterns that are

46:45

more highly correlated with life expectancy than

46:47

chronological age. But I think the big

46:49

but here is that even if that's

46:51

the case, they would not be as

46:54

good as what Peter would predict after

46:56

all the Biological age. That's what I

46:58

want to get to. Yes, and I

47:00

think what you are actually doing is

47:02

looking at other biomarkers that have a

47:05

long-term clinical history that you're using to

47:07

come up with a surrogate, but really

47:09

is reflecting largely biological age. Maybe not

47:11

completely. And this is the other point

47:13

I wanted to make is I don't

47:16

think biological age and health are equal.

47:18

I think they're strongly overlapping. And certainly

47:20

you can identify many ways to reduce

47:22

health without accelerating biological aging. I think

47:24

that's easy. We can all think of

47:27

ways to do that. So let's take

47:29

a minute and try. Yeah, so let's

47:31

think about this for a second. I

47:33

have seen very impressive data where we

47:35

can look at tissue samples of organs

47:38

and we can tell, okay, I'm going

47:40

to show you a sample of nephrons.

47:42

And just based on nothing but the

47:44

methylation pattern, we know that if I

47:47

just said to you, one of these

47:49

is a 20 year old, one of

47:51

these is a 70 year old. It's

47:53

very easy to predict based on the

47:55

methylation pattern which Neffron came... from which

47:58

person. Completely agree with that. There are

48:00

a lot of things that change with

48:02

age. The literature has 25,000 things that

48:04

change with age. Average amount of methylation

48:06

at these 10 spots is number 11,4007

48:09

of those. So great, you've got another

48:11

thing that changes with age. So that's

48:13

the question. But that's not enough. Right.

48:15

So do you believe that all of

48:17

the research we're seeing on the epigenetic

48:20

clocks is going to be the 78th

48:22

variable that we would include in our

48:24

gestalt... I don't know. Yeah, it's a

48:26

good question. So I am hopeful that

48:29

epigenetic algorithms can get to the point

48:31

where they can replace many, certainly not

48:33

all, but many of the other biomarkers

48:35

that are being measured. I think the

48:37

thing that gives me hope is we

48:40

know that epigenetic changes are part of

48:42

biological aging. This again is a different

48:44

question, but if we look at the

48:46

hallmarks of aging, epigenetic dysregulation is one

48:48

of the 12. Some people will argue

48:51

it's the most important conversation, but it's

48:53

at least part. So that gives me

48:55

some hope that we are in fact

48:57

measuring something that plays a causal role

48:59

in the aging process. And I think

49:02

what's missing, I think what would give

49:04

all of us a lot more confidence

49:06

is if we had a mechanistic connection

49:08

to the specific methylation changes and some

49:10

cause of aging or age-related disease. In

49:13

other words, this change in methylation changes

49:15

this particular gene's expression level, which changes

49:17

the rate of biological aging. I think

49:19

if we had that, we'd feel a

49:22

lot more confident. Yeah, you and I

49:24

spoke about this very briefly at the

49:26

end of our last podcast, and I

49:28

want to come back to it with

49:30

all of us on this table, because

49:33

there's so much in what you just

49:35

said, Matt, that I'm going to lay

49:37

out a broad question, and then we

49:39

can start attacking it in different ways.

49:41

So one of the things I want

49:44

to address is do we believe that

49:46

it's possible that of the hallmarks of

49:48

aging, epigenetic change is the most important.

49:50

Another topic I want to address, do

49:52

we believe that the epigenetic changes that

49:55

we observe over time which are undeniable

49:57

are causal in the arrival of other

49:59

states? everything from the arrival of senescent

50:01

cells, the increase in inflammation, the reduced

50:03

function of the organs, which really is

50:06

the hallmark of aging. And if so,

50:08

does that mean that reversing the epigenetic

50:10

phenotype will undo the phenotype of interest?

50:12

And Rich, where I'm going, that you

50:15

and I left off was, what about

50:17

the proteome? What about the metabolism? So

50:19

you made three statements there. Broad general

50:21

statements. And I think each of the

50:23

three deserves careful... Amendment. Let's do it.

50:26

To be polite about it. The first

50:28

has to do with hommarks of aging,

50:30

which I think set the field back

50:32

dramatically. I think when you are officially

50:34

branded a homework of aging, by two

50:37

people sitting alone at their computers and

50:39

writing a review article, a homework of

50:41

aging... I thought they were walking around

50:43

a pond when they came up with

50:45

this. All right, okay, okay. Means that

50:48

somebody once said, I'm interested in aging,

50:50

that's kind of important, isn't it? Let's

50:52

put it on our list. You can't

50:54

tell if something is a hallmark of

50:56

aging. Does that mean it goes up

50:59

with age? It goes down with age.

51:01

You can change it in a way

51:03

that will extend lifespan. You can kill

51:05

a mouse or a worm by removing

51:08

it. Basically, it's something that somebody wants

51:10

to might be of interest to aging.

51:12

And the downside of that is once

51:14

you're officially branded as a hallmark of

51:16

aging, anyone who wants to write up

51:19

grant on that doesn't have to prove...

51:21

that they're a fundamental cause and effect

51:23

model has any merit. Because it's a

51:25

hallmark of aging. I don't have to

51:27

prove it anymore. Someone, I don't know

51:30

who or on what grounds, has decided

51:32

it's important. My reviewers know it's important

51:34

because they've read the hallmark of aging

51:36

paper, so I don't have to think

51:38

about whether it's important. The negative side

51:41

of that coin is that there are

51:43

lots of things that didn't make it

51:45

into the hallmark list. I really think

51:47

it's premature to close thought off on

51:49

some of those. It's easy to come

51:52

up with a dozen things that ought

51:54

to be investigated, but if you want

51:56

to investigate it, it's not on the

51:58

home marks list. What are you wasting?

52:01

So deciding which of the hallmarks is

52:03

the big daddy hallmark or whatever strikes

52:05

me is not the correct thing to

52:07

talk about in the hallmarks arena. So

52:09

maybe we should talk about that before

52:12

we go through all of these. I think

52:14

there's a lot to unpack there. You

52:16

guys could afford to give me a

52:18

little piece of paper and a pen.

52:20

I think the hallmarks is a list,

52:22

a kind of arbitrary list, not completely

52:24

arbitrary, because I had some reasons for

52:26

being there. I don't think any of

52:28

us would say that. those 12 things

52:30

are not involved in aging. But

52:32

that's a very little interest. Do any

52:34

of us want to rattle them up being

52:36

that I'm the only one that's got the

52:39

list sitting in front of me? We could

52:41

do a game where we each name one

52:43

and see who can't see if we get

52:45

to all 12. Yeah. But certainly in that

52:48

list, I would not consider epigenetics as the

52:50

key hallmark. Assuming there are such

52:52

things, I consider it to be

52:54

an interesting list. It became... biblically

52:57

sacrosan almost immediately and I've never understood

52:59

why but for some reason it did

53:01

so I'd agree with rich so conceptually

53:03

beautiful I mean so I agree completely

53:05

with rich and he knows I do

53:07

because we've talked about this before I

53:09

think the flip side is I think

53:11

the hallmarks have been immensely useful to

53:13

the field they are a very easy

53:15

way to communicate this idea of biological

53:17

aging and it helps convince some of

53:19

the scientific community that thought it was

53:21

all just hocus pocus pocus and snake

53:24

oil that there is some mechanistic research

53:26

happening. We can point to specific things

53:28

that are aging. So I think that

53:30

part of the hallmarks has been actually

53:32

really valuable and has contributed to the

53:34

popularization of longevity and at least to

53:36

the extent the science of longevity has

53:38

been popularized has contributed to

53:41

that. And it has been extremely detrimental to

53:43

the field and the way I think about

53:45

it is it just caused the field to

53:47

narrow prematurely. And this goes back to

53:49

what I alluded to before. I

53:51

don't know if we understand 80%

53:54

of biological aging or 0.205% of

53:56

biological aging. My guess is it's

53:58

closer to 0.205% and... And by and

54:00

large, the funding to look outside of

54:02

the hallmarks dried up once the hallmarks

54:05

became the dominant paradigm. And people stopped

54:07

looking. And I think we need to

54:09

go back to more discovery science and

54:11

thinking outside the box. So I think

54:14

it's been a double-edged sword. Would that

54:16

happen automatically if we could wave that

54:18

magic wand and increase funding? It would

54:20

help. I don't know that it would

54:22

help enough, but it would help. I

54:24

mean, you also kind of have to change

54:26

the mindset. about what people call fishing expeditions.

54:28

That's like a bad word in grant

54:30

review panels, fishing expedition, meaning you don't

54:33

really know what you're going to find,

54:35

but you got to go look before

54:37

you can figure out what's important. So

54:39

we have to kind of change that

54:41

mindset as well. One can usefully concreteize

54:43

this discussion. I imagine that one of

54:45

this, I don't read these papers because

54:47

they upset me, but I imagine inflammation

54:49

is on one or more of these.

54:51

Sure is. Chronic inflammation. chronic inflammation. What

54:53

that does is, if you say I'm

54:55

interested in chronic inflammation, so I'm doing

54:58

good stuff, huh? But what could

55:00

be happening is this particular set

55:02

of cytokines might be overexpressed by

55:04

some glial cells and that leads

55:06

to loss of cognitive function, whereas

55:08

this other overlapping set of cytokines

55:11

produced by the macrophages in your

55:13

fat may be due more prone

55:15

to diabetes or metabolic syndrome, whereas

55:17

this particular set of lymphocytes...

55:20

are necessary to repel COVID,

55:22

and that's why you are

55:24

more susceptible to COVID. So

55:26

learning what changes within the

55:29

extremely broad generic idea of

55:31

inflammation, what changes in what

55:33

cell types, in what people,

55:36

under what pharmacological or genetic

55:38

changes, how they are interacting

55:40

with other aspects of pathology,

55:43

that's marvelous to do, but

55:45

to say, oh, inflammation, that

55:47

gets bad when you're old. is

55:49

a way of avoiding the labor of

55:51

thinking, and that's why I'm against it.

55:54

And I think Matt brought up

55:56

a really important point, and we

55:58

scientists are to blame. is the

56:00

way that research gets reviewed. For

56:03

lazy reviewers having these 12

56:05

hallmarks is really helpful. This has

56:07

got one of the hallmarks in

56:10

it. This must be good stuff.

56:12

I do think reviewers need to be

56:14

more open to new ideas and

56:16

new approaches. I mean everybody

56:19

knows that NIH grants are

56:21

approved if they're incremental. If

56:23

they're really breakthrough, they don't

56:26

get approved. A very famous biologist,

56:28

E.O. Wilson, told me years ago,

56:30

he said, don't ever include your

56:32

best ideas in a grant. They

56:35

won't get funded. Do the standard

56:37

stuff, save your best ideas for

56:39

projects that you do on the

56:41

side. That's one of the reasons

56:43

I left academia. Draw me

56:45

nuts. Almost impossible to get the

56:47

important stuff funded. The second

56:50

of your multi-partite question was, does epigenetic

56:52

change? Is it causal? And the

56:54

third, which we may get to,

56:56

is can you reverse it, and

56:58

would that be a good thing?

57:00

So let's talk about the second

57:02

element here, is it causal. The problem

57:04

is what it means. There are some changes

57:07

that occur in this particular set

57:09

of 40 cells in the pineal,

57:11

and there are other changes that occur

57:13

in these cells in the bone marrow,

57:15

and there are other cells that change

57:17

in the gut and villus lining cells,

57:19

and the crypt cells, so they are

57:21

all epigenetic in some. are caused by

57:24

some things, and we don't really know

57:26

which, if any of these, count for

57:28

aging. If someone says, I'm going

57:30

to prove that an epigenetic

57:32

change is responsible for

57:35

aging, they haven't begun to come

57:37

to grips with the nitty-gritty.

57:39

People always ask, just as

57:41

you hinted, does your drug

57:44

change epigenetic things? And

57:46

unfortunately, that's where they stop

57:48

thinking. We're always willing to give people

57:50

tissues from our drug-treated mice if they

57:53

are keen on epigenetic changes that affect

57:55

neuron regeneration. Excellent. Their experts will send

57:57

them the brains and they can do

57:59

that. stuff, it's important. I'm not making

58:02

fun of it, but the general notion

58:04

that it's aging vaguely thought of is

58:06

due to epigenetic change, more vaguely thought

58:08

of, doesn't really get you anywhere. That's

58:11

my skeptical view. Is part of the

58:13

issue that you're saying, well, what's causing

58:15

the cause? No, it's just that the

58:18

concept of epigenetic change encompasses thousands of

58:20

changes in hundreds of cell types under

58:22

hundreds of influences. Of course, some of

58:25

that causes other stuff agreeing to that,

58:27

assenting to that notion that epigenetic change

58:29

is causal for all sorts of age-related

58:31

pathologies. Everyone can agree to that, but

58:34

it's meaningless, because what counts is to

58:36

say, this specific change is really important

58:38

in this disease. Here's an epigenetic alteration,

58:41

or this specific broad spectrum change in

58:43

multiple tissues. causes something good or bad.

58:45

You have to define what it is

58:48

before you can test it. So let's

58:50

use a specific example. When you look

58:52

at a patient with type 1 diabetes

58:54

and you look at their beta cells

58:57

in their pancreas, they look different epigenetically

58:59

than the beta cells of an age-matched

59:01

person without type 1 diabetes. And we

59:04

also know that their beta cells don't

59:06

function. So they've lost function. So let's

59:08

ask that question as a specific example.

59:11

Do you believe or what confidence would

59:13

you assign to the notion that the

59:15

epigenetic change on the beta cells of

59:17

the type 1 diabetic are indeed causal

59:20

to the loss of function of the

59:22

beta cell? My last exposure to the

59:24

causes of type 1 diabetes was on

59:27

eyes in medical school, which is more

59:29

than five years ago, but if I

59:31

vaguely remember it was an autoimmune disease,

59:33

right? So if your poor little helpless

59:36

beta cells are being attacked by antibodies

59:38

and macrophages and things... those stress reactions

59:40

are going to cause epigenetic change. And

59:43

when those epigenetic changes contribute to some

59:45

extent to the ill... fate of the

59:47

beta cells, it's possible. And if I

59:50

were an expert on diabetes pathogenesis, I'd

59:52

really want to know that. It doesn't

59:54

have anything to do with aging, but

59:56

it's an interesting question. But it's a

59:59

way to address causality. Yeah, but you

1:00:01

might equally say, no, it's the mitochondrial

1:00:03

that have changed. There are a hallmark

1:00:06

of diabetes. Yeah, or it's the glycated

1:00:08

proteins. There's a ton of things, and

1:00:10

there's no reason in the world at

1:00:13

this stage, I think, to actually give

1:00:15

epigenetics genetics primacy. over anything else. It's

1:00:17

a nice hypothesis. It's a hypothesis. You

1:00:19

can formulate these questions because a lot

1:00:22

is known about type 1 diabetes. And

1:00:24

I understand 0.05% of the biology of

1:00:26

aging. Zero zero five. Yeah. I was

1:00:29

giving... You're off by an order of

1:00:31

magnitude. He was raising you by a

1:00:33

log. Yeah. I thought you would log

1:00:36

off. Formulating the questions in exactly the

1:00:38

way Steve did. makes it clear how

1:00:40

difficult it is to evaluate the concept

1:00:42

that epigenetic change contributes to pathogenesis and

1:00:45

type 1 diabetes. And we know more

1:00:47

or less what is going on in

1:00:49

type... We don't know what's going on

1:00:52

in aging. We don't even know what

1:00:54

part of the body is going on

1:00:56

or parts more likely of the body.

1:00:59

I at least internally reframe it a

1:01:01

little bit and say, what would the

1:01:03

experiment be? What would you need to

1:01:05

do to convince yourself that either broadly

1:01:08

speaking epigenetic dysregulation causes aging, whatever that

1:01:10

means, or this specific epigenetic change that

1:01:12

is associated with chronological age causes aging?

1:01:15

And so that's an easier way for

1:01:17

me to think about it because I

1:01:19

feel like it's all a... fascinating conversation,

1:01:22

but we're never going to get to

1:01:24

the answer until somebody actually does the

1:01:26

experiment. Or decides that it can't be

1:01:28

formulated because it's too complicated. It gives

1:01:31

up. Yeah, that's right. But people are

1:01:33

trying to do both of those things.

1:01:35

I mean, people are using partial or

1:01:38

transient epigenetic reprogramming and asking can that

1:01:40

have effects on biological aging. I'm actually

1:01:42

cautiously optimistic it can. I don't think

1:01:44

it's going to be a game changer,

1:01:47

but I think you can modulate aspects

1:01:49

of biological aging. epigenetic modifications, so if

1:01:51

we think this particular epigenetic mark at

1:01:54

this particular location in the genome controls

1:01:56

aging. And I don't think it's going

1:01:58

to be that simple, but let's say

1:02:01

it is. You could go in, you

1:02:03

could modify that, and then see, do

1:02:05

you reduce disease? Do you increase lifespan?

1:02:07

Do you improve health span? So those

1:02:10

are the kinds of experiments that I

1:02:12

think would get us to where we

1:02:14

can have a lot of confidence. If

1:02:17

it's the case, if somebody, let's say

1:02:19

at Altos, at Altos, publishes a paper,

1:02:21

Three years from now, that they have

1:02:24

made a mouse live six years by

1:02:26

multiple rounds of transient epigenetic reprogramming, I'll

1:02:28

be like their biggest fan. They moved

1:02:30

the needle. That convinces me that that

1:02:33

strategy modulates biological aging. Nobody's done that

1:02:35

yet. What about something far less impressive,

1:02:37

but still with WALC? So consider, if

1:02:40

we could get to the point where

1:02:42

we could locally deliver vectors that would

1:02:44

epigenetically change chondracites... so that you could

1:02:47

take osteoarthritis in the knee and just

1:02:49

regenerate cartilage, regenerate cartilage by changing the

1:02:51

epigeno. But is that biological aging? I

1:02:53

wouldn't be convinced that's modulating the biological

1:02:56

aging process. I would be convinced that's

1:02:58

a clinically useful strategy for people who

1:03:00

benefit from that therapy. I guess it

1:03:03

kind of depends on why we think

1:03:05

an individual would be experiencing osteoarthritis. How

1:03:07

much of that is senescence? How much

1:03:10

of that is inflammation? Before we go

1:03:12

down that and ask, is it the

1:03:14

S? Yeah, yeah, let's talk about Sonecis.

1:03:16

If you think osteoarthritis of the knee

1:03:19

requires a knee joint replacement and that's

1:03:21

going to help your patient, you are

1:03:23

not rejuvenating. It's perfectly possible to do

1:03:26

great things with technology, including chondrocyte regeneration,

1:03:28

without having to decide that that's related

1:03:30

to aging. People don't age because they

1:03:33

fail to have titanium knee joints or

1:03:35

something. And one way I think about

1:03:37

this, and again this may be completely

1:03:39

wrong, but it's a useful way for

1:03:42

me to think about it, is I

1:03:44

think about age related disease as the

1:03:46

down. extreme effect of biological aging. For

1:03:49

most diseases, there becomes a point where

1:03:51

the pathology of that disease mechanistically is

1:03:53

no longer the same as biological aging.

1:03:56

In which case- He's very good. You

1:03:58

should listen to him. And one of

1:04:00

the implications of that is the interventions

1:04:02

that slow biological aging may not work.

1:04:05

Once you get past that point, but

1:04:07

things that do work for that disease

1:04:09

may have nothing to do with biological

1:04:12

aging. Does that make sense? Yeah, go

1:04:14

deeper on that idea, though. Let's use

1:04:16

the example. My favorite disease. Let's talk

1:04:18

about cancer. Cancer is an easy one.

1:04:21

We know with cancer in many cancers,

1:04:23

the process is you have one or

1:04:25

more mutations, which then often lead to

1:04:28

additional mutations. You get genome instability. Eventually

1:04:30

you get an oncogen that gets activated

1:04:32

and that leads to uncontrolled cell division.

1:04:35

There are more tumor suppression gene that

1:04:37

gets deactivated. Yeah, right. And if we

1:04:39

accept that immune surveillance is one important

1:04:41

anti-cancer mechanism. We know that immune surveillance

1:04:44

declines with age, so early on we're

1:04:46

clearing a lot of our cancers. As

1:04:48

our immune system declines, these cancers are

1:04:51

going to escape immune surveillance, they're going

1:04:53

to accumulate all these mutations, they're eventually

1:04:55

going to go into uncontrolled cell division.

1:04:58

That uncontrolled cell division at that point?

1:05:00

You can treat the cancer, but uncontrolled

1:05:02

cell division is not biological aging. it's

1:05:04

not a part of the normative aging

1:05:07

process. So the treatment there, so the

1:05:09

mechanism now is fundamentally different from normative

1:05:11

aging, and the treatment, let's just say

1:05:14

the treatment in this case is chemotherapy,

1:05:16

might benefit the cancer. It's not going

1:05:18

to do normal aging, right? And I

1:05:21

think rapamycin is a good example here

1:05:23

where I think we all believe that

1:05:25

rapamycin and inhibiting amtour slows biological aging,

1:05:27

at least in up to mice, hopefully

1:05:30

in dogs, hopefully in people. Yeah. So

1:05:32

it's a fundamental... node in the network,

1:05:34

that's the way I think about the

1:05:37

hallmarks of aging. It's a node in

1:05:39

the network that underlies the hallmarks of

1:05:41

aging. So we can manipulate amtor with

1:05:44

rapamycin, slow aging. Rapomycin is a pretty

1:05:46

good anti-cancer drug until the cancer is...

1:05:48

have evolved to ignore the M-tor break,

1:05:50

then then rapid mison doesn't work anymore.

1:05:53

And we know rapid mison doesn't work

1:05:55

for most cancers. That's an example. That's

1:05:57

been tested. We know that. Yeah, absolutely.

1:06:00

And it's because the cancers evolve to

1:06:02

bypass the M-tor break or to bypass

1:06:04

the ability of rapid mison to inhibit

1:06:07

M-tors. That's a really good point that

1:06:09

we all take for granted that I

1:06:11

think is worth noting. Rapomycin can be

1:06:13

unsuccessful as a chemotherapy agent and can

1:06:16

yet be very successful as a cancer

1:06:18

preventive agent. Absolutely. And it's exactly for

1:06:20

that reason. And I think this also

1:06:23

illustrates why traditional disease-based medicine is not

1:06:25

about the biology of aging. It's about

1:06:27

something of the biology of aging is

1:06:30

distinct and it needs to be investigated

1:06:32

in a different way. And we know

1:06:34

that in the aging field, but the

1:06:36

people in the cancer field, in the

1:06:39

cardiology field, in the neurology field, I

1:06:41

don't think they understand that. This gets

1:06:43

to, if I were health czar, this

1:06:46

is what I would do, because it

1:06:48

comes back to what Rich said at

1:06:50

the outset, which is, why is this

1:06:52

a zero-sum game? I mean, you didn't

1:06:55

ask it that way, but that's effectively

1:06:57

the problem you're dealing with, which is...

1:06:59

Why can't we study cardiology, oncology, and

1:07:02

neurology and aging without everybody feeling like

1:07:04

a thing? My way of saying that

1:07:06

in Peter terms is we need to

1:07:09

have medicine 2.0 and medicine 3.0 in

1:07:11

parallel because the tools of the medicine

1:07:13

2.0 scientist and physician, which we see

1:07:15

on display today, are putting the stent

1:07:18

in, giving the chemotherapy, lowering the cholesterol,

1:07:20

all of these things, the medicine 3.0

1:07:22

toolkit looks different. Different science. You're going

1:07:25

to use wrap a mice in here,

1:07:27

you're not going to use it over

1:07:29

here because it's too late. Instead of

1:07:32

saying one or the other, why isn't

1:07:34

it both? Why wouldn't we want both

1:07:36

of these running in parallel? Well, we

1:07:38

would, but of course, the zero-sum game

1:07:41

is a pretty good analogy for what's

1:07:43

actually going on. The amount of research

1:07:45

dollars at least available to NIH is

1:07:48

not infinitely expensible. It's set by a

1:07:50

complex political process that... allocates it amongst

1:07:52

institutions, so saying that it would be

1:07:55

a good idea to have more funds.

1:07:57

I agree with you, and I'll bet

1:07:59

these two guys do as well, but

1:08:01

it's not easy to do. Yeah, I

1:08:04

think I misspoke. It will be a

1:08:06

portfolio of reallocation. But it will be

1:08:08

worthwhile because the burden of this disease

1:08:11

will be lower. So in other words,

1:08:13

it's sort of like saying, right now

1:08:15

I spend $100,000 a year on the

1:08:18

barrier to my house to prevent anybody

1:08:20

from breaking in. and I spend $100

1:08:22

a year patrolling the neighborhood to make

1:08:24

sure there aren't too many bad guys

1:08:27

in the neighborhood. There's a scenario where

1:08:29

if your total budget is $100,000 and

1:08:31

$100. Maybe you could spend $80,000 in

1:08:34

total by spending more money patrolling the

1:08:36

neighborhood. I think we generally agree with

1:08:38

you that having a greater proportion of

1:08:41

available research dollars, both private and public,

1:08:43

going into the biology of aging and

1:08:45

its impact on late life health, would

1:08:47

be a good thing. I don't think

1:08:50

you're going to get an argument from

1:08:52

anybody in... the cardiology field, the neurology

1:08:54

field, or Alzheimer's, the Alzheimer's field. It's

1:08:57

their money. But wouldn't some of those

1:08:59

people, as the funding dollars move towards

1:09:01

the aging side, also want to move

1:09:03

and say, look, I'm going to study

1:09:06

this through the aging lens? I was

1:09:08

on the council for the National Aging

1:09:10

Institute for three years, and if at

1:09:13

any point I can swear to this

1:09:15

from personal testimony, somebody would say something

1:09:17

like, I wonder if maybe a few

1:09:20

percent of the Alzheimer's budget. might instead

1:09:22

go to studying how slow aging models

1:09:24

would have an impact on late life

1:09:26

neurodegenerative disease, the next day the director

1:09:29

of the aging institute would get a

1:09:31

call from two or three Congress people

1:09:33

who were on the Appropriations Committee stating

1:09:36

that this will not be happening because

1:09:38

there was an Alzheimer's association person who

1:09:40

got the call from the NIA staff

1:09:43

member in charge of Alzheimer's saying, tell

1:09:45

the congressman to call the director. and

1:09:47

let's put a stop to that reckless

1:09:49

idea. They're tied in to the political

1:09:52

process in ways. Well, we just need

1:09:54

to go maybe one step further because

1:09:56

those Congress people have a boss. They

1:09:59

report to somebody too. Would that be

1:10:01

at the stage? Yeah, no. I mean,

1:10:03

come on. Maybe it's because the public

1:10:06

doesn't understand this. Those people answer to

1:10:08

the public. That's a good example. These

1:10:10

are our dollars. That's right. That are

1:10:12

going on with you. But Alzheimer's association.

1:10:15

I mean, that's a patient advocacy group.

1:10:17

That is the public. Yes, although let's

1:10:19

ask the question, what have they done

1:10:22

for those patients lately? That's a different

1:10:24

question, but I mean, I'm just reinforcing

1:10:26

what you said. I think part of

1:10:29

this is educating people. If you know

1:10:31

somebody who's suffering from Alzheimer's disease, you

1:10:33

know very well that the only thing

1:10:35

we've got going for us right now

1:10:38

is prevention. We don't have too many

1:10:40

silver bullets in the treatment gun. Despite

1:10:42

massive spending. I was once in Congress

1:10:45

trying to lobby. with about six people

1:10:47

from the Alzheimer's Association in the same

1:10:49

room. I was totally ignored by staffers

1:10:52

that were in there. I mean, I

1:10:54

agree with all of this. I agree

1:10:56

with all of this. I think, again,

1:10:58

though, we should be careful not to

1:11:01

demonize people for wanting to cure Alzheimer's.

1:11:03

It's a good thing. It's a good

1:11:05

goal. I think the communication piece is

1:11:08

about the fact that it's going to

1:11:10

be much more efficient and effective to

1:11:12

keep people from getting it in the

1:11:15

first place. Once you've outpaced the biology

1:11:17

of aging with the pathology of the

1:11:19

disease, it gets a lot harder, a

1:11:21

lot harder to do anything about it.

1:11:24

So I think that communication part, honestly,

1:11:26

I don't know why we've been so

1:11:28

unsuccessful, because I think a lot of

1:11:31

us have been out there trying to

1:11:33

communicate this message for a long time,

1:11:35

but it's starting to permeate. We're at

1:11:37

that moment, I think, where people are

1:11:40

starting to get it, that biological aging

1:11:42

is a thing. It's malleable. It's going

1:11:44

to take a little while, but there's

1:11:47

reason to be optimistic. And there's also

1:11:49

the private sector is another reason, I

1:11:51

think, to be optimistic. So let's go.

1:11:54

record right now I think when we

1:11:56

if we defeat Alzheimer's disease it's going

1:11:58

to be because of the biology of

1:12:00

aging it's not going to be because

1:12:03

of the drugs that get rid of

1:12:05

absolutely yeah probably cancer probably heart disease

1:12:07

although I think Peter's more optimistic we

1:12:10

can prevent heart disease if you took

1:12:12

the tools of medicine 2.0 and just

1:12:14

applied them 30 years earlier we wouldn't

1:12:17

have ASCPD that's the one place where

1:12:19

it's word but again that's because the

1:12:21

mechanism of action is so well understood

1:12:23

with ASCD compared to Alzheimer's and cancer

1:12:26

a lot of infectious disease, a lot

1:12:28

of liver disease, a lot of kidney

1:12:30

disease, all of those things can be

1:12:33

improved dramatically by targeting the biology of

1:12:35

aging. You know, if I were to

1:12:37

write my book again, I would add

1:12:40

a fifth horseman, because I talked about

1:12:42

these four horsemen of ASCVD, cancer, neurodegenerative

1:12:44

and demanding diseases, and metabolic disease, but

1:12:46

I would actually add a fifth hallmark.

1:12:49

It's not really a hallmark of disease,

1:12:51

but it's the fifth thing that brings

1:12:53

life to a bad close, which is

1:12:56

immune dysfunction, which is immune dysfunction. And

1:12:58

I don't think I gave that enough

1:13:00

attention in the book, because of course,

1:13:03

as you said, it factors in very

1:13:05

heavily to oncogenesis, but also, as COVID

1:13:07

showed us, what a risk factor it

1:13:09

was to be old. And you know,

1:13:12

I'm reminded of this when I see

1:13:14

people my age get brutal pneumonias, and

1:13:16

like two months later, they're okay. And

1:13:19

you realize, two of my patients actually

1:13:21

in the past six months have had

1:13:23

really bad pneumonias. where you're looking at

1:13:26

the CT of their chest and you

1:13:28

cannot believe they're alive. But of course

1:13:30

they're fine. Three months later, four courses

1:13:32

of antibiotics later, they're fine. And you

1:13:35

realize, you do that to a 75-year-old,

1:13:37

it's over. And it simply comes down

1:13:39

to how their B cells and T

1:13:42

cells work. That to me is an

1:13:44

area where I'd love to see more

1:13:46

attention, which is, what would it take

1:13:49

to rejuvenate the immune system as a

1:13:51

proactive statement? I think that that's a

1:13:53

perfect example. Influenza pneumonia has never fallen

1:13:55

out of the top 10 causes of

1:13:58

death in the US. You know, it

1:14:00

used to be number two, but still

1:14:02

now it's number eight or nine, but

1:14:05

it's always there because you can't really

1:14:07

do anything about the late life immune

1:14:09

dysfunction. Just to follow this up, if

1:14:11

magically you become in charge and you're

1:14:14

able to double the amount of research

1:14:16

being done on the biology of aging

1:14:18

fundamentally, then we can afford to do,

1:14:21

let's give some mice to start with

1:14:23

a batch of anti-aging drugs. and see

1:14:25

if it makes them more resistant to

1:14:28

infectious illnesses, including pneumonias, but viral infections

1:14:30

as well and many others. I'd love

1:14:32

to know the answer to that, and

1:14:34

no one has actually really looked in

1:14:37

a serious way, because the ITP has

1:14:39

enough money to just measure lifespan. None

1:14:41

of the health span. Just hoping that

1:14:44

everybody else is now going to look

1:14:46

at the brain and the lungs and

1:14:48

the infection, the sensory systems. That really

1:14:51

ought to be done, and it's not

1:14:53

being done because of a lack of

1:14:55

a lack of money. You said something

1:14:57

a while ago, Rich, that I think

1:15:00

is timely now, which is with each

1:15:02

generation of these drugs, they get more

1:15:04

efficacious and less toxic. Not yet, but

1:15:07

that's the hope. Well, no, no, but

1:15:09

I'm going to use another example. The

1:15:11

GLP1s are the best example of this,

1:15:14

right? So you go back to the

1:15:16

very, very first generation of GLP1 agonists

1:15:18

barely lost any weight, horrible side effects,

1:15:20

horrible side effects, horrible side effects. Fast

1:15:23

forward to semi-glutide, quite a bit better

1:15:25

efficacy, still really bad side effects. Next

1:15:27

generation trusepetite, better efficacy, side effects are

1:15:30

almost gone. Now, why haven't we been

1:15:32

able to do that with these geo-protective

1:15:34

drugs? So we have this one study

1:15:37

using Evrolamus that gives us a hint

1:15:39

that says, hey, this might actually enhance

1:15:41

immune function in people in their mid-60s.

1:15:43

But we need the follow-up study, the

1:15:46

follow-up drug, the follow-up drug, imagine what

1:15:48

the fourth generation of that drug can

1:15:50

do. where it's tuned to get better

1:15:53

and better and a fewer side of

1:15:55

it. There are strong commercial motivations. You

1:15:57

know you're going to sell a lot

1:16:00

of the obesity drugs. They're very strong.

1:16:02

martial motivations to do those studies over

1:16:04

and over and over again until you

1:16:06

find one that works better. And there

1:16:09

are good preclinical models that you can

1:16:11

use that you're not wasting too much

1:16:13

of your time on clinical trials. That

1:16:16

could be done for anti-aging drugs as

1:16:18

well, although testing anti-aging drugs and people

1:16:20

is a whole separate set of tangle

1:16:23

of difficulties. I don't want to talk

1:16:25

about that right now. But I'm saying

1:16:27

it won't be quite as easy as

1:16:29

it was for anti-obacy medications, but no

1:16:32

one's doing even the first level of

1:16:34

research to find the optimal compounds for

1:16:36

efficacy without side effects or even to

1:16:39

begin to see if they have desirable

1:16:41

effects on aging rate indicators in people.

1:16:43

That's kind of a cheap and easy

1:16:45

study and no one has really tackled

1:16:48

that yet. Well, I just heard that

1:16:50

there are over 80 synolithic studies in

1:16:52

early clinical trials. It's a joke. It's

1:16:55

a joke. It's a joke. It's a

1:16:57

joke. We have to come back to

1:16:59

this. Are any of them powered for

1:17:02

anything other than safety? This is, I

1:17:04

think, a problem. Oh, it's all phase

1:17:06

one, exactly. So they're underpowered. They're almost

1:17:08

useless, in my opinion. Well, until they

1:17:11

get to phase two, phase three. Well,

1:17:13

until they get to phase two, phase

1:17:15

three, phase three. Well, until they get

1:17:18

to phase two, two, two. There's lots

1:17:20

of complicated issues here. I think endpoints

1:17:22

for clinical trials are really challenging, but

1:17:25

solvable. So there are two places I

1:17:27

wanted to go next, and I'm going

1:17:29

to let Rich decide, because he's going

1:17:31

to have the strongest point of view.

1:17:34

Can we talk about senescence? Or can

1:17:36

we talk about what biomarkers would be

1:17:38

necessary to help us study aging in

1:17:41

humans as we translate from your work

1:17:43

and Matt's work? I know what I

1:17:45

want to talk about, and it's the

1:17:48

second of those, too. I don't want

1:17:50

to spend the next three or four

1:17:52

hours explaining why senescence is silly and

1:17:54

anti-senalytics are untested at best. So there's

1:17:57

no way we're not talking about that.

1:17:59

Let's go on to item number two.

1:18:01

And I think the most important thing...

1:18:04

is to make a clear distinction between

1:18:06

biomarkers and aging rate indicators. Please explain

1:18:08

the difference to people, please. Okay, I'll

1:18:11

do my best. So a biomarker, allegedly,

1:18:13

and in real life, is something that

1:18:15

changes with age. So if you have

1:18:17

some drug that slows aging, the biomarkers,

1:18:20

many of them in the different cell

1:18:22

types and in the blood will change

1:18:24

more slowly. They are a good way

1:18:27

of looking at whether you're slowing it.

1:18:29

rate of change of biomarkers,

1:18:31

very established part of the

1:18:33

literature, and valuable. But you

1:18:35

have to wait till somebody's old, whether

1:18:38

it's a dog or a mouse or

1:18:40

a person, because only when they're old,

1:18:42

has the bio marker of aging, the

1:18:45

surrogate marker for biological aging, changed very

1:18:47

much. So in a clinical trial, certainly

1:18:49

in a human situation, no

1:18:51

one wants to wait 20 years to

1:18:53

see whether the biomarkers have changed. And

1:18:56

a one-year is such a tiny fraction

1:18:58

of a human lifespan that you

1:19:00

don't really anticipate detectable change

1:19:02

with a appropriately powered study.

1:19:04

It's like aging rate indicators, which

1:19:07

are much less well studied and much

1:19:09

less well established in principle, are things

1:19:11

you can measure that tell you whether

1:19:13

in a slow aging state or a

1:19:15

normal state. Can I just make a point

1:19:17

for the listener so they understand

1:19:19

the challenge of what we're talking

1:19:21

about? When we study blood pressure

1:19:23

drugs or cholesterol drugs, The

1:19:25

biomarkers change so rapidly

1:19:27

and we know the relationship

1:19:30

between the biomarker and the

1:19:32

disease state. So if your

1:19:34

blood pressure is 145 over 90,

1:19:36

on average, before I give you

1:19:38

this ace inhibitor, and three months

1:19:41

later, six months later, nine

1:19:43

months later, your blood pressure

1:19:45

is averaging 119 over 74, I

1:19:48

know I've done something well. Now I will

1:19:50

still probably in the phase three, in fact

1:19:52

I will in the phase three, have to

1:19:54

make sure that I also reduce some event

1:19:56

in you. But generally by the phase

1:19:58

two, I know that this drug is... not toxic

1:20:00

and that it's predictably lowering your blood pressure,

1:20:02

that's really, really valuable. A biomarker generically

1:20:04

is something that's easy to measure that is

1:20:06

informative about something that's hard to measure.

1:20:09

A classical example, a famous example is you

1:20:11

want to know how many cigarettes somebody

1:20:13

smokes a day, they'll lie to you. But

1:20:15

if you measure cotoning in their blood, that's

1:20:17

a byproduct of nicotine, you don't have to

1:20:20

ask them. You can find out how many

1:20:22

cigarettes they had in the last couple of

1:20:24

days by measuring blood. That's a biomarker of

1:20:26

cigarette consumption. Is it a marker of nicotine

1:20:28

or carbon monoxide? I don't know the answer

1:20:30

to that. I wasn't sure. In

1:20:32

principle, a biomarker of aging is, are many

1:20:34

of them, and they are measuring biological aging

1:20:36

processes, and they're useful in that regard. But

1:20:38

they don't tell you how fast you're aging. The

1:20:41

analogy I love to use is an

1:20:43

odometer is like a biomarker of aging

1:20:45

of your car. It tells you how

1:20:47

many miles your car has gone, but

1:20:49

it doesn't tell you how fast the

1:20:51

car is going. The speedometer tells you how

1:20:53

fast your car is going. What we

1:20:55

need, and what I think we're

1:20:57

just beginning now to document, is

1:21:00

things like the speedometer, aging rate

1:21:02

indicators that reliably discriminate

1:21:04

slow aging mice or people

1:21:06

from regular old mice or

1:21:08

people. We have now dozen

1:21:10

or so things that change in the fat,

1:21:13

in the blood, in the liver, in the brain,

1:21:15

and in the muscle that are always changed

1:21:17

in any slow aging mouse, whether

1:21:19

it's drug A, drug B, drug C,

1:21:21

calisthenic diet, or single gene

1:21:23

mutations. We've looked now at

1:21:25

five different single gene mutations, and

1:21:27

this whole set of 12,

1:21:29

or roughly 12, aging rate

1:21:31

indicators always changes in every slow aging

1:21:34

mouse, and it does so in youth,

1:21:36

which is the key point. So,

1:21:38

if it does so quickly after

1:21:40

an anti -aging drug is administered,

1:21:43

that's the transition. That's the bridge

1:21:45

you need for clinical studies in

1:21:47

people. If you want to know whether

1:21:49

metformin or canagliflozin or something slows

1:21:51

aging in people, and you don't want

1:21:53

to wait 20 years, but you've

1:21:55

got things that tell you whether they're

1:21:57

in a slow aging state, how

1:21:59

fast... they are aging versus normal, and

1:22:02

that's a big if. We don't yet have

1:22:04

evidence we can do that. We just have

1:22:06

hope we can do that. Then that allows

1:22:08

you quickly, quickly being within six

1:22:10

months to a year, to know whether

1:22:13

your anti-aging manipulation, alleged

1:22:15

anti-aging manipulation, has moved them

1:22:17

to a physiological status which is

1:22:20

associated with slower aging. A lot of

1:22:22

that can be done in mice with drugs,

1:22:24

with mutants. And are these all

1:22:26

proteins rich? No. No, some of them

1:22:28

are changes in the fat,

1:22:30

different classes of macrophages. The pro-inflammatory

1:22:33

macrophages, the anti-inflammatory macrophages, the good

1:22:35

ones, go up. UCP1, I recall

1:22:37

from our discussion. UCP1, goes up

1:22:40

in every one of our 10

1:22:42

different kinds, 11 now of slow

1:22:44

aging mice. Does it go up

1:22:47

in any of the mice that

1:22:49

did not receive a successful drug?

1:22:51

Well, we compare them to controls,

1:22:54

and the... Question you're asking is really

1:22:56

important. That's what we're doing in the

1:22:58

next five years. We just got a

1:23:00

grant to do that. We're going to

1:23:02

take mice and give them either a

1:23:04

good drug or a different drug that

1:23:06

doesn't work and then make those

1:23:08

comparisons. A really important thing to prove.

1:23:10

So far, our only control has been

1:23:13

untreated mice. At some point in this,

1:23:15

I had to bring this up. But

1:23:17

let's imagine that Rich is incredibly successful

1:23:19

at finding these things. That is a

1:23:22

very, very long way. from assuming that it's

1:23:24

going to be the same in people.

1:23:26

Most things that clinically

1:23:28

work in mice do not work in people.

1:23:30

It might be, and that would be

1:23:33

wonderful, but I think ultimately we're

1:23:35

going to have to find this

1:23:37

for people. And my thought is

1:23:39

the kind of evaluation that you

1:23:41

do routinely of your patients.

1:23:43

If we took a group of 65-year-olds

1:23:45

and we gave them a drug

1:23:48

that we thought was an anti-aging

1:23:50

drug, and follow them the next

1:23:52

five or six years doing these

1:23:54

evaluations, I think you could probably

1:23:57

safely say, this is slowing aging.

1:23:59

or not slowing aging. So I don't

1:24:02

think that it's going to be that

1:24:04

easy to jump from ice to people

1:24:06

in this. I've always wondered if in

1:24:08

people the easiest way to do it

1:24:11

would be to take the most obvious

1:24:13

thing that we know is going to

1:24:15

reduce the rate of aging. So it'd

1:24:18

be an interesting experiment, but you find

1:24:20

someone who is overweight diabetic and smokes

1:24:22

and has hypertension. You get hundreds of

1:24:24

these folks. You put half of them

1:24:27

on to be ethical, a plan where

1:24:29

you try to get them to stop

1:24:31

and presumably many don't. In the other

1:24:33

group you pull out all the stops

1:24:36

and you don't care because you're interested

1:24:38

not in testing the hypothesis, does this

1:24:40

thing help you? You're interested in getting

1:24:42

them to lose weight, not have diabetes,

1:24:45

stop smoking, exercise like crazy. The greatest

1:24:47

division between two groups of individuals where

1:24:49

we would I think be able to

1:24:52

agree that this group is now aging

1:24:54

slower, the group that we've... reconcile their

1:24:56

diabetes quit the smoking etc etc etc

1:24:58

and then I'd love to see riches

1:25:01

12 line up in that population that

1:25:03

would be great let me just say

1:25:05

that I think that people that study

1:25:07

animals myself included always underestimate how well

1:25:10

we can evaluate health in people with

1:25:12

a very very thorough evaluation because we

1:25:14

don't do that in our experiment. Why

1:25:17

do you think that is Steve why

1:25:19

is it? Because I was going to

1:25:21

ask about parabiosis later on in the

1:25:23

discussion. We might as well talk about

1:25:26

it now, right? Parabiosis seems to actually

1:25:28

kind of work in certain mouse models.

1:25:30

Do we have any reason to believe

1:25:32

it's going to work in humans? And

1:25:35

if not, why not? Why are mice

1:25:37

so different from people? But wait a

1:25:39

minute. I wouldn't say that just because

1:25:41

we don't have evidence that it works

1:25:44

in humans means mice or when it

1:25:46

comes to parabiosis, right? Yeah, yeah, yeah.

1:25:48

That's a different discussion. translation to people,

1:25:51

most have failed. I actually think that's

1:25:53

because those are artificial mouse models where

1:25:55

they tried to give young mice an

1:25:57

age-related disease. I'm more optimistic. I don't

1:26:00

know this. Rich doesn't have those mice,

1:26:02

yeah. I know. I'm more optimistic that

1:26:04

biological aging or normative aging is going

1:26:06

to be much more likely to translate

1:26:09

to people, both interventions and biomarkers, than

1:26:11

the specific disease interventions. I might be

1:26:13

wrong. I don't know the answer. We

1:26:15

would hope that's a case. That's fair.

1:26:18

I don't think we should rule out

1:26:20

the mice as a useful model. In

1:26:22

fact, I think there's reason to be

1:26:25

optimistic that it will. it's not a

1:26:27

pragmatic approach for population gerotherapeutics. But I'm

1:26:29

just wondering like why it wouldn't be

1:26:31

as efficacious. This is something that, I

1:26:34

mean aren't there six or eight clinical

1:26:36

trials going on right now? Different variants

1:26:38

of that, yeah. Yeah. Yeah. I haven't

1:26:40

seen them. I've seen the one that's

1:26:43

looking at it's not really a parabiosis

1:26:45

study, but it's looking at plasma ferises

1:26:47

for Alzheimer's for Alzheimer's. I consider that

1:26:50

a little bit different, but fair enough,

1:26:52

but fair enough, okay, okay, because they're

1:26:54

just using albuma. studies going on of

1:26:56

young blood. Yeah, okay, okay. But if

1:26:59

you think of peribiosis as both taking

1:27:01

away the bad stuff that accumulates with

1:27:03

age and adding in the good stuff

1:27:05

that's in young, some sort of plasma

1:27:08

exchange hits at least half that equation.

1:27:10

Okay, I'm gonna come back to this,

1:27:12

but my question was, why the difference?

1:27:14

You're saying, Matt, the difference is probably

1:27:17

amplified in disease specific cases like heart

1:27:19

disease, cancer and Alzheimer's disease. probably less

1:27:21

relevant when you're talking about aging, because

1:27:24

even a flawed mouse model still ages.

1:27:26

In fact, it's designed to age in

1:27:28

a certain way. Yeah, and I mean,

1:27:30

I think normative aging looks very similar.

1:27:33

Again, if we look for mice to

1:27:35

dogs to people, just broadly speaking, the

1:27:37

process looks pretty similar. So, I'm cautiously

1:27:39

optimistic that these things are going to

1:27:42

translate. Not to pay too much attention

1:27:44

to Steve's pessimism on this point, although

1:27:46

he's completely right. And it's for a

1:27:49

variety of reasons. Sometimes humans are different

1:27:51

from mice. Sometimes the drug has side

1:27:53

effects that are tolerable in mice, not

1:27:55

tolerable in people, etc. But I always

1:27:58

like to look at the other side

1:28:00

of the coin. That is... If your

1:28:02

goal is to develop a drug that

1:28:04

blunts pain in people and you screen

1:28:07

40 or 50 drugs and you find

1:28:09

a couple that inhibit pain in mice,

1:28:11

that's a really good start. It doesn't

1:28:13

guarantee they're going to work in people,

1:28:16

but it gives you this category of

1:28:18

snail-based neurotoxins. Let's make 40 of those

1:28:20

from 40 different snails. We'll find one

1:28:23

that actually in people works can be

1:28:25

made by a scalable process and doesn't

1:28:27

produce... serious side effects. So the mice,

1:28:29

it's not a one-to-one mapping. It works

1:28:32

in mice, it doesn't, it works in

1:28:34

people, but it's an important critical first

1:28:36

step which usually succeeds in finding a

1:28:38

set of drugs of related families or

1:28:41

with related targets at least that are

1:28:43

efficacious in people. Most drugs that are

1:28:45

used in people had useful rodent-based research

1:28:47

somewhere in their pedigree. Absolutely agree with

1:28:50

that, Rich, and nobody's saying that 100%

1:28:52

of things at work in mice do

1:28:54

not work. But I think there's a

1:28:57

critical difference for aging research, which it

1:28:59

takes four years to do one of

1:29:01

these in mice. And so if we

1:29:03

have to do 40 to find one

1:29:06

or two that works... That's why I

1:29:08

like aging rate indicators. Speed things up.

1:29:10

I'm stepping on your toes, Peter, but

1:29:12

the question I always come back to.

1:29:15

I agree. We need these aging rate

1:29:17

indicators. How do we get to the

1:29:19

point where we get to the point

1:29:22

where we get to the point where

1:29:24

we're confident that they're confident that they

1:29:26

actually work in people, How do we

1:29:28

get to the point that FDA is

1:29:31

confident that they work? That's the only

1:29:33

way you're going to be able to

1:29:35

use them in a clinical trial. I

1:29:37

don't see a path in the short

1:29:40

term. Well, I don't know that we

1:29:42

need that to tell the truth. So

1:29:44

I went to the FDA to try

1:29:46

to get them to approve a trial

1:29:49

of Metformin. And we didn't couch it

1:29:51

in aging because you're right, as soon

1:29:53

as you mention aging, their eyes glaze

1:29:56

over and they're not interested anymore. that

1:29:58

we're fine with that. But that's a

1:30:00

different end point. That's not a biomarker.

1:30:02

My question is that you've merged two

1:30:05

different difficult problems. Problem A, can we

1:30:07

find drugs that slow aging in people?

1:30:09

Problem B, can we... surmount the legal

1:30:11

and political barriers to getting them. That's

1:30:14

not what I was asking. I was

1:30:16

asking, how do we get to the

1:30:18

point? Okay. What I'm saying is that

1:30:21

you are focused on something I don't

1:30:23

have any answers to basically, which is

1:30:25

how do we get the FDA to

1:30:27

develop and approve clinical trials? I was

1:30:30

more interested in a step before that.

1:30:32

It'd be nice to have some drugs

1:30:34

that actually do work to slow aging

1:30:36

in people. But you have to trust

1:30:39

the biomarker of aging rate before you

1:30:41

can be confident that the drug that

1:30:43

moves the biomarker of aging rate works

1:30:45

in people. That's fundamentally what I'm asking.

1:30:48

How do we get to the point

1:30:50

where, let's just take FDA out of

1:30:52

the equation, the four of us would

1:30:55

sit and look at the data and

1:30:57

I'll be like, yep. Well, that's sort

1:30:59

of my thought experiment. I would have

1:31:01

to take an example in humans that

1:31:04

is so egregious, so let's say we

1:31:06

do that. Well, it would make me

1:31:08

worry. It would only show you the

1:31:10

positive signal. It would show you the

1:31:13

specificity and not the sensitivity of the

1:31:15

test. That's the problem. You might miss

1:31:17

the signal. If you found a proteomic

1:31:19

genomic, like if you found a multimodal

1:31:22

signal that detected a difference in rate

1:31:24

of aging between those two very extreme

1:31:26

sets, you might miss it with a

1:31:29

geoprotective drug, which wouldn't be as dramatic

1:31:31

as dramatic as that chain. So what

1:31:33

if I told you that there are

1:31:35

people who claim... There are epigenetic signatures

1:31:38

that do that, that correlate quite well.

1:31:40

They claim with health outcomes, 10-year mortality,

1:31:42

5-year mortality, 3-year mortality in people, and

1:31:44

are measuring the rate of biological aging.

1:31:47

Because it's out there. I mean, it's

1:31:49

in the literature. I mean, this is

1:31:51

not perfect, but it would be one

1:31:54

thing I would immediately think of, which

1:31:56

is I would take a really good

1:31:58

biobank that would have enough samples that

1:32:00

I could sample a bunch of human

1:32:03

stuff. and use an unbiased sample and

1:32:05

a biased sample. So I would determine

1:32:07

an algorithm based on one and see

1:32:09

how well it predicted on another based

1:32:12

on enough samples. That would have to

1:32:14

be true at a minimum. Yeah, I

1:32:16

think it is. I mean... Again, at

1:32:18

least it depends on how much faith

1:32:21

you put in these research studies, but

1:32:23

I mean people have published epigenetic algorithms,

1:32:25

Dunedin Pace is the one that most

1:32:28

people are going to talk about, that

1:32:30

correlate seemingly pretty well, at least with

1:32:32

mortality and with metrics of health span,

1:32:34

for lack of a better way of

1:32:37

framing it. So that exists. And Dunedin

1:32:39

Pace is using something besides epigenetic or

1:32:41

is it only epigenetic? I think it

1:32:43

uses something else. It was trained off

1:32:46

of... other biomarkers and then they found

1:32:48

epigenetic marks that correlate with those other

1:32:50

biomarkers. So it's a correlation to a

1:32:53

correlation, but there's still a correlation. What

1:32:55

do you think, Rich? Well, I wanted

1:32:57

to go back to the example you

1:32:59

gave where you took a lot of

1:33:02

people and gave them intense exercises and

1:33:04

dietary changes to improve their likely health

1:33:06

outcomes, and that's a good place to

1:33:08

start a discussion, because you said every

1:33:11

sensible person would see the treated group

1:33:13

as aging more slowly. And I would

1:33:15

want to ask before I agree to

1:33:17

that, do they also have improved cognition?

1:33:20

How are they doing in cataracts? How

1:33:22

are they doing in hearing? What happens

1:33:24

when you give them a flu shot?

1:33:27

Do they have a great flu shot?

1:33:29

So the things you've pointed to are

1:33:31

really important for both overall health and

1:33:33

for cardiovascular risk and the things linked

1:33:36

to that. So it's nice to know.

1:33:38

But to convince me that you now

1:33:40

have a slow aging group of people.

1:33:42

You need to go beyond the risk

1:33:45

factors for specific common human diseases. If

1:33:47

you could show that, then for the

1:33:49

first time I would be convinced you

1:33:51

had an effective anti-aging manipulation in people.

1:33:54

Currently I don't know that there is

1:33:56

any effective anti-aging manipulation in people. If

1:33:58

your approach got there, that would be

1:34:01

a terrific research model. Well, but now

1:34:03

we're getting into... the definition of aging

1:34:05

a little bit, which is would you

1:34:07

agree that the approach I'm describing would

1:34:10

produce a longer life? It's easy to

1:34:12

produce a longer life if you happen

1:34:14

to have a clinical condition. where you're

1:34:16

tied to a railroad track, and there's

1:34:19

a train coming, you can extend that

1:34:21

woman's life enormously by simply giving her

1:34:23

a knife and cutting the bonds and

1:34:26

letting her walk away from the track.

1:34:28

longevity promoting interventions. Well, if 80% of

1:34:30

people died as a result of trains

1:34:32

on train tracks, that might be a

1:34:35

worthwhile example. But given that 80% of

1:34:37

people die from these four chronic diseases.

1:34:39

I'm all in favor of protecting people

1:34:41

against chronic diseases. Yes, yes, yes. That's

1:34:44

good thing. And I'm glad that people

1:34:46

are doing that. No question about it.

1:34:48

Now, talking about the biology of aging,

1:34:50

there are all sorts of things that

1:34:53

also happen when you get older that

1:34:55

are not part of those chronic diseases.

1:34:57

And to make a case that you've

1:35:00

got an anti-aging manipulation, you need to

1:35:02

show that those are changed too. But

1:35:04

do all of them have to change

1:35:06

or just most of them? Don't enough

1:35:09

have to change that you increase the

1:35:11

length and quality of your life, and

1:35:13

if you still get a cataract at

1:35:15

the same rate, I'm not sure that

1:35:18

should be disqualifying. Right, but the important

1:35:20

thing I think about what Rich said

1:35:22

is all the stuff that he pointed

1:35:25

out could be easily done in humans.

1:35:27

Wouldn't be easily done in humans. Wouldn't

1:35:29

be hard to measure hearing. Well-known, famous,

1:35:31

long and slow aging dog breeds, and

1:35:34

it's true for horses too, certainly true

1:35:36

for mice, is that more or less

1:35:38

everything slows down together, the tiny dogs

1:35:40

that are very long-lived, it's not just

1:35:43

that they have a delay of cancer,

1:35:45

they have a delay in neurodegenerative disease,

1:35:47

a delay in digestive diseases, and joint

1:35:49

diseases, aging has been slowed in those

1:35:52

dogs. And if... The dogs did your

1:35:54

thing. But we might not have an

1:35:56

intervention that does that to your point,

1:35:59

Rich. I'm saying we might not have

1:36:01

a non pharmacologic method that does that.

1:36:03

It's not clear that even though exercise

1:36:05

clearly extends lifespan, it's not clear that

1:36:08

it's doing so by slowing aging. Those

1:36:10

are two different things, to your point.

1:36:12

It's not clear, but it's an interesting

1:36:14

question. Like, do you believe exercise slows

1:36:17

aging? Exercise, healthy diet, sleep? I have

1:36:19

no idea. I think so. My intuition

1:36:21

is I think so, but I can't

1:36:23

point to the evidence that tells me

1:36:26

so. Well, there's evidence to support it.

1:36:28

The question is, does it rise to

1:36:30

the level of evidence that would convince

1:36:33

Rich? I believe it probably does too,

1:36:35

but I'm not going to, say, with

1:36:37

100% certainty. I think here's where we

1:36:39

get back into health span versus lifespan.

1:36:42

The effect of exercise on longevity is

1:36:44

enormous. Somewhat depends on where you start.

1:36:46

I've always found these to be a

1:36:48

little bit problematic because I don't think

1:36:51

that defining it by the input is

1:36:53

as valuable as defining it by the

1:36:55

output. In other words, to say you

1:36:58

exercise this many minutes a week versus

1:37:00

that many minutes a week is a

1:37:02

little dirty because intensity matters, what you

1:37:04

do matters, sometimes the output is what

1:37:07

matters more, how strong you are, how

1:37:09

high your VO2 Max is, those tend

1:37:11

to be more predictive, because that's the

1:37:13

integral of the work that's been done.

1:37:16

But your point is, it's well taken.

1:37:18

The impact on health span is what

1:37:20

I tell my patients. If this amount

1:37:22

of exercise didn't make you live one

1:37:25

day longer, the quality in which your

1:37:27

life would improve would justify it. Now

1:37:29

fortunately we can move past this semantic

1:37:32

discussions because there's now molecular ways of

1:37:34

checking this. Exercises, I'll bet all of

1:37:36

you know, increases an enzyme called GPLD1

1:37:38

in the blood of exercise people and

1:37:41

in mice, and Sovieta's lap has shown

1:37:43

that if you elevate GPLD1... It does

1:37:45

great things to your brain, more neurogenesis

1:37:47

and more brain-derived protective factors, brain-driven neurotrophic

1:37:50

factors. Iresen also goes up in humans

1:37:52

and in mice. After exercise, it does

1:37:54

great things for your fat. As does

1:37:57

cloth, though? Let's leave that out for

1:37:59

a moment. Oh boy. Oh boy, I'm

1:38:01

striking all the nerves here today. All

1:38:03

right. You may be quite right. I

1:38:06

wanted to stick with the GPLD1 and

1:38:08

Iresen to make the point that they

1:38:10

also go up in all of the

1:38:12

slow-aging mice. That is, all the anti-aging

1:38:15

drugs, the calistricted diet, the isalusian restricted

1:38:17

diet, and five different single gene mutation.

1:38:19

that extend life span in mice, they

1:38:21

all elevate GPLD1. 17 Alpha Estridal? Yes.

1:38:24

Can Igaflosen? Both sexes? Well this is

1:38:26

the key question. I recent is sex-specific

1:38:28

GPL1 is in both sexes. This is

1:38:31

how one begins to answer that question.

1:38:33

This is the exact kind of question

1:38:35

one has to ask. So if you

1:38:37

are interested in the idea that exercise

1:38:40

regimes have a benefit beyond the obvious

1:38:42

exercise link physiological declines of age, Do

1:38:44

they improve cognition? And if so, how

1:38:46

these molecular changes are the things you

1:38:49

need to begin to investigate. The anti-aging

1:38:51

studies in mice show that the anti-aging

1:38:53

drugs, at least the ones we've looked

1:38:55

at so far, increase the same things

1:38:58

that exercise does. Rich, have you done

1:39:00

this experiment with an ITP cohort where

1:39:02

you run in addition to a drug

1:39:05

parallel? You know what I'm going to

1:39:07

ask? Well, you're going to ask if

1:39:09

we exercise our mice. Yes. Yeah, we've

1:39:11

never done that. So you haven't done

1:39:14

a sedentary versus exercise. You have not

1:39:16

done that. You haven't done a obesogenic

1:39:18

versus fasted. We never use obesogenic diets.

1:39:20

It's worth doing. The IDP doesn't do

1:39:23

it. We don't have the resources. We

1:39:25

have enough resources to test about five

1:39:27

drugs a year, but if we wanted

1:39:30

to test them in exercise versus non-exercised.

1:39:32

We got to get you a budget

1:39:34

increase because that will now get to

1:39:36

this question, because now we could look

1:39:39

at the soluble. Maybe it would, maybe

1:39:41

it wouldn't. In mice. I'm very agnostic

1:39:43

about what we can learn from exercising

1:39:45

mice, because mice are basically kept in

1:39:48

a jail cell their entire life. If

1:39:50

you took a bunch of people and

1:39:52

put an exercise wheel and a gel

1:39:54

cell, that we'd use it, would that

1:39:57

be the same? Would that substitute for

1:39:59

people that walk around to go inside,

1:40:01

they go outside, they go to the

1:40:04

gym, that do this? It wouldn't substitute

1:40:06

for all of it, no question. So

1:40:08

to me, it's a very low level

1:40:10

of exercise. If you didn't see anything

1:40:13

from it, then you wouldn't rule it

1:40:15

up. Right. So the molecular, testable, molecular

1:40:17

hypotheses that link the biology of aging

1:40:19

to anti-aging drugs and to exercise and

1:40:22

teasing out how those are interrelated and

1:40:24

which of your exercise regimes increase I

1:40:26

recent, increased GPLD1 and increased neurogenesis. That's

1:40:29

a research agenda that could be very

1:40:31

valuable. And then if you want to

1:40:33

screen drugs in people to see which

1:40:35

ones... deserve expensive long-term testing. The ones

1:40:38

that raised GPLD1, IRISN, and some aspect

1:40:40

of neurological function in addition to the

1:40:42

good stuff they're doing for the muscles.

1:40:44

That's an approach. I agree completely and

1:40:47

this gets back to what we were

1:40:49

talking about before with the epigenetic changes

1:40:51

is if you had a mechanistic connection,

1:40:53

which is what Rich is drawing there,

1:40:56

not only this is correlated with this

1:40:58

outcome, but here's why. We all feel

1:41:00

a lot more confident that this is

1:41:03

real that this is real. that it's

1:41:05

important, and especially if that mechanistic connection

1:41:07

is preserved in people. Do any of

1:41:09

you believe that G.L.P.1 agonists are geo-protective?

1:41:12

I'm super interested in that question. Yeah,

1:41:14

I don't know. We need to find

1:41:16

that out. They look good. I think

1:41:18

there's two parts, though. Are they geropotective?

1:41:21

I'm taking the first as a given.

1:41:23

Okay. Yeah. That's a different question is,

1:41:25

is chronic caloric restriction beneficial in normal

1:41:27

weight people? But most people taking GOP1

1:41:30

agonists aren't normal. Yes, yes. And I

1:41:32

think it's impossible at this point because

1:41:34

the studies are all done in obese

1:41:37

and patients with type 2 diabetes that

1:41:39

we can't disentangle them. So we will

1:41:41

just say that for that patient population,

1:41:43

the caloric restriction appears to be giroprotective.

1:41:46

But yes, you're right. I'm technically asking

1:41:48

the second question, which is in an

1:41:50

individual who is metabolically healthy but overweight.

1:41:52

where there's actually no evidence that weight

1:41:55

loss per se is necessary outside of

1:41:57

maybe some edge cases in orthopedic stuff.

1:41:59

Is there a giroprotective nature to this?

1:42:02

And where it's most talked about is

1:42:04

in dementia prevention right now. That's

1:42:06

where it's at least most complicated to

1:42:08

tease that out. So what do

1:42:10

you guys think? And it clearly

1:42:13

has neurological effects as effects on

1:42:15

addiction. The dementia connection is not

1:42:17

inconceivable. Right. Right. Right. I mean, Rich, this

1:42:19

is one for you to test. Yeah, why hasn't

1:42:21

the ITP tested this yet, Rich? Is it because

1:42:24

the oral ones are just not strong enough and

1:42:26

we want to... Yes. Can you break your

1:42:28

protocol? Because it's enormously laborious to

1:42:30

do weekly injections. That sounds like

1:42:32

an I need more money problem.

1:42:34

And also you need a separate

1:42:36

control group. That sounds like a

1:42:38

I need more money problem. You

1:42:40

get sham injections and are, yes,

1:42:42

if you increase our budget dramatically,

1:42:44

I think it's a worthwhile experiment.

1:42:46

But what we're waiting for is oral

1:42:49

drugs that work that you don't have

1:42:51

to do injections of drugs. I mean,

1:42:53

there is an oral semi-glutide formulation that's

1:42:55

taken daily. It was submitted to

1:42:58

us this. technically very laborious. Each

1:43:00

mouse has to be food deprived for

1:43:02

six hours. Then the material is administered

1:43:04

and then they have to have a

1:43:06

change in their water balance for the

1:43:09

next two hours. It is technically not

1:43:11

an injection, but it is not any less

1:43:13

laborious and in addition you have

1:43:15

to have your own separate control

1:43:17

group that gets all of those

1:43:20

different manipulations with a sham injection.

1:43:22

Could you do three instead of

1:43:24

five next year and make that

1:43:26

one of them? reallocate some funding?

1:43:28

Well, I'm not in charge. It's a heavy

1:43:30

lift. I'd vote against it. I would vote

1:43:32

for waiting about a year until somebody comes

1:43:34

up with a pill that you can just

1:43:36

mix into mouse food or water and give

1:43:38

it to the mice and it'll work.

1:43:40

And these are going to be mice

1:43:43

that are an incredible amount of stress

1:43:45

from all the handling, the injections. Yeah,

1:43:47

that's why the control group is necessary,

1:43:49

but the companies are putting so much

1:43:51

money into this. They understand why people

1:43:53

don't like to inject like to inject

1:43:55

to inject themselves. But I'm reasonably sure that

1:43:57

in a year or two there'll be some...

1:44:00

agent that works when you put it

1:44:02

in the food of a mouse or

1:44:04

pop it as a pill as a

1:44:06

person, those would be enormously important

1:44:09

to test. Do we know

1:44:11

if terzepotide, for instance, if

1:44:13

we're given to people of normal

1:44:15

body weight, do they also lose

1:44:17

15% of their body weight? I

1:44:19

have not seen the data on that.

1:44:21

I can tell you anecdotally, having

1:44:24

seen patients, it's going to

1:44:26

be dose dependent. So as you

1:44:28

know that drug is dosed from as low

1:44:31

as two and a half milligrams weekly to

1:44:33

as much as 15 milligrams

1:44:35

weekly? Usually people who don't

1:44:37

need to lose much weight, someone

1:44:39

who says, look, I just want to

1:44:41

lose this last 10 pounds and I've

1:44:43

done all the exercising and dieting I

1:44:46

can do, they typically just lose

1:44:48

that 10 pounds and they take a

1:44:50

very low dose. Now to your point, if

1:44:52

they took the 15 milligrams, would

1:44:54

they become sarcopenic? I don't know. how

1:44:56

constraining lack of resources are. I mean,

1:44:58

there are probably like 15 or 20.

1:45:01

And I mean, every time I hear

1:45:03

Rich talk about this stuff, it just

1:45:05

pisses me off. Because there's a bunch

1:45:07

of stuff that should be tested, should

1:45:09

have been tested by now, that hasn't

1:45:11

been tested, not because it's not a

1:45:14

good idea, but because there just isn't

1:45:16

any resources to do it. Well,

1:45:18

I think what's really frustrating as

1:45:20

well is that. These are the

1:45:22

types of experiments that would allow

1:45:24

us to actually start to economically

1:45:26

model the impact of these drugs

1:45:28

outside of just kind of a

1:45:30

disease state. For example, if

1:45:32

drugs like these are indeed neuroprotective

1:45:35

and people can work three years

1:45:37

longer or five years longer because

1:45:40

they're healthier, think of the impact

1:45:42

on that over at OMB. What does that

1:45:44

mean to tax take? What does that

1:45:46

mean to delaying Medicare? What does that

1:45:48

mean to reduce health care spending at

1:45:51

the time when it is most expensive?

1:45:53

So last estimate I saw was 38

1:45:55

trillion a year for every year of

1:45:57

health span. Wow. That was a McKenzie.

1:46:00

That's 38? I'll send you the link.

1:46:02

Not 3.8? No, 38. That's analysis by

1:46:04

Andrew Scott, his British economist. That's bigger

1:46:06

than I would have guessed. Wow. Can

1:46:08

we just, because I'm in the mood

1:46:10

to see you get spicy, can we just talk

1:46:12

about senescence for a minute? Senescent cells

1:46:15

he means, Rich. You know, the things

1:46:17

that drive aging. What do you mean?

1:46:19

Do you want me to talk about senescent

1:46:21

cells? Okay, yes, I'll be glad to do

1:46:23

that. It's a terrible historical

1:46:26

historical historical accident. Leonard Hayflick

1:46:28

way back found that human

1:46:30

cells would only divide 50 times

1:46:32

and stop. One of his colleagues, a

1:46:34

guy named Victoria Defending, made a joke

1:46:36

at lunch and said to him, hey

1:46:38

Len, maybe they're getting a hold. Ha ha

1:46:41

ha ha. And Len did not understand

1:46:43

it was a joke. He thought it

1:46:45

was a serious scientific hypothesis. It's clearly

1:46:47

nuts because we don't get old in

1:46:49

a way that it is modeled by

1:46:51

having embryonic lung fiber blasts stop

1:46:53

growing. But at the time, the hottest

1:46:56

technique in modern medicine was you could

1:46:58

grow cells in culture. I was really

1:47:00

so cool. You could do stuff with

1:47:02

them. So all the cell biologists who

1:47:05

really wanted to use the cool as

1:47:07

new toys wanted to have a way

1:47:09

of studying aging without all these messy

1:47:11

mice and rats and having to wait

1:47:13

and stuff. They could do it in

1:47:15

vitro, because this was in vitro aging.

1:47:17

This is in vitro senescence. And

1:47:19

the field to skip 30 or 40 years.

1:47:21

The field went ahead with this metaphor.

1:47:23

without ever questioning it. It's now such

1:47:26

an industry that the people who review

1:47:28

these grants and papers and advise billionaires

1:47:30

and advise startup companies, they all were

1:47:32

trained in labs that just do senescence

1:47:34

for a living so they never stopped

1:47:37

to question. One of the most famous

1:47:39

and best scientists in this area

1:47:41

is a woman named Judy Campisi. She

1:47:43

recently passed away. She and I were

1:47:46

assistant professors together at Boston University. She

1:47:48

and I were going to send in

1:47:50

a program project with a third-person Barbara

1:47:52

Gilcrest. I was going to study immunity in

1:47:54

aging, Barbara's going to study skin cells,

1:47:57

we talked, Judy, you want to study

1:47:59

cells in essence. So she read the literature,

1:48:01

she came back to us and she said,

1:48:03

it has nothing to do with aging.

1:48:05

I mean, it's good cell biology, it's

1:48:08

good about cancer biology, but of course

1:48:10

there's nothing to do with aging.

1:48:12

And we told Judy, of course it

1:48:14

has nothing to do with aging. And

1:48:16

we told Judy, of course it has

1:48:18

nothing to do with aging. We understand

1:48:21

that, but the reviewers think it is

1:48:23

aging. So if you can just keep a

1:48:25

straight face for the three hours of

1:48:27

the site visit, when she moved to

1:48:29

Berkeley. She took her grant with her, and

1:48:31

after a year or two,

1:48:33

she had apparently convinced herself

1:48:35

that it was aging. It was close

1:48:38

enough to aging. So the

1:48:40

notion that aging is due

1:48:42

to senescent cell accumulation is bad

1:48:44

for two reasons. It's a

1:48:47

grotesque oversimplification. The evidence for

1:48:49

this is awful. But even worse,

1:48:51

it again cuts off productive

1:48:53

thinking. There almost certainly are changes

1:48:55

that occur in some glial cells in

1:48:58

the brains that as you get older,

1:49:00

they start making bad cytokines is bad

1:49:02

for your brain. There probably are changes

1:49:05

in some bone marrow cells or some

1:49:07

cells in the lineage that leads to

1:49:09

the beta cells in the pancreas that

1:49:11

lose the ability to divide. And that's

1:49:13

bad for you. And finding out how

1:49:15

it happens is really important. But

1:49:18

once you convince yourself, that's all

1:49:20

the same thing. This cytokine, this

1:49:22

sort of proliferation. This change in

1:49:25

ability to make specific fibrous

1:49:27

connective tissue, let's call that

1:49:29

senescence. It's the same thing. You've

1:49:31

lost what you need to

1:49:33

think of good, careful,

1:49:36

well-defined experiments with well-defined

1:49:38

endpoints. If you say that senescent,

1:49:40

there is a thing called a

1:49:42

senescent cell, the thing that's happening

1:49:44

in this gli, and in this

1:49:46

marrow cell, and this pancreas, it's

1:49:48

due to the senescent cell accumulating.

1:49:51

You've blocked off productive

1:49:53

generation of research hypotheses. The

1:49:55

last point I'll mention in this

1:49:58

rant has to do... with senelitic

1:50:00

drugs. So the ITP was asked

1:50:03

to test an allegedly senelitic

1:50:05

drug called Faisitan. It was

1:50:07

given to us by someone who

1:50:09

is using this now for

1:50:11

clinical trials and who has

1:50:13

a company that's interested in

1:50:15

senelitic drugs. So we gave it to

1:50:17

mice. It had no beneficial effect whatsoever.

1:50:20

What's the mechanism of this

1:50:22

drug's action? Has no action or

1:50:24

had no effect? It had no effect.

1:50:26

Okay. What is it supposed to do?

1:50:29

It was supposed to kill senescent cells

1:50:31

or something. So we told this

1:50:33

guy sorry it had no effect.

1:50:35

He said, well, let's prove that

1:50:38

whether it had any change in senescent

1:50:40

cells. So we gave him

1:50:42

blind tissues from each of

1:50:44

the treated and untreated mice and

1:50:46

he tried a test and there

1:50:49

were no changes in senescent cells.

1:50:51

So then he said, well, send the brain

1:50:53

and the liver and the muscle, maybe

1:50:55

the senescent cells have been changed in

1:50:57

the brain, so we send blind samples

1:51:00

to a colleague of his. There were

1:51:02

no changes in senescent cells by any

1:51:04

of the markers that these folks looked

1:51:06

at. So this drug, which is now

1:51:08

being marketed in clinical trials, and you

1:51:11

can buy it, I'm sure. It's a

1:51:13

natural product. There's no evidence as far

1:51:15

as I know that it... either has

1:51:17

an anti-aging effect or removes senescent

1:51:20

cells. But once you've got a

1:51:22

commercial company pushing this stuff, and your

1:51:24

whole brand, your whole lab, your whole

1:51:26

program project, and all the people

1:51:28

who are reviewing you are convinced

1:51:30

senescent cells exist, they're bad and

1:51:32

drugs can kill them, it's a

1:51:34

snowball rolling downhill and a rant of

1:51:37

the sort of just delivered has no impact

1:51:39

on the field. So can I

1:51:41

give a counter example? Because there's

1:51:43

good experimental data. that these things can

1:51:46

be at least partially eliminated and

1:51:48

when you do that there's an

1:51:50

improvement in health and this has

1:51:52

been done both in a genetic

1:51:54

treatment which genetically would say prime

1:51:56

these cells to be genetically killed

1:51:59

and it's also been done with drugs,

1:52:01

not with FISA, I hasten to say.

1:52:03

So I think there's strong evidence

1:52:05

that getting rid of these P16 positive

1:52:07

cells, which is really what it's all

1:52:10

based on, can have an improvement

1:52:12

in health and in longevity. Is the

1:52:14

Van Derson paper you're talking about in

1:52:16

which they were allegedly to please? Yeah,

1:52:18

yeah, yeah, yeah. Let me tell you

1:52:20

about that, because I was on the

1:52:22

program project. Two papers. Okay, one was

1:52:24

with the short live mice and once.

1:52:26

Okay, so talk about the one that

1:52:29

is not the short live mice. There's

1:52:31

a paper, a famous paper, by Van

1:52:33

Derson Kirkland and several other colleagues Darren

1:52:35

Bay. Are these the guys at Mayo?

1:52:37

Yes, they would. I remember this. They've

1:52:39

left. Two of them have left, but

1:52:41

yes. They allege that they could remove

1:52:43

senescent cells by taking genetically modified mice,

1:52:46

giving them a drug. All the senescent

1:52:48

cells would go away. and the mice

1:52:50

lived longer, according to the paper. It was

1:52:52

on the cover of nature. It was on

1:52:54

the cover of nature. I remember this one.

1:52:56

I was a part of the program

1:52:59

project, so was Judy Kempisie. And

1:53:01

my job was to do the

1:53:03

lifespan experiment. We got the mice

1:53:05

from Kirkland and Vendersen. We got

1:53:08

Kempisie's mice. We got the drugs from

1:53:10

them, and we gave the drugs to the

1:53:12

mice at 18 months. And you know, they

1:53:14

had no effect on senescent cells.

1:53:16

Not one. We tried seven times. to

1:53:19

show depletion of senescent cells

1:53:21

in their mice using their

1:53:23

drug, and went zero for seven.

1:53:25

We then took the tissues,

1:53:27

blinded, and sent them to Judy's

1:53:29

lab, Judy Campesi's lab, so she

1:53:31

could measure P16 cells, but she

1:53:33

didn't know which ones were from

1:53:36

treated and which ones were untreated,

1:53:38

when we undid the code. There

1:53:40

was no effect on senescent cells

1:53:43

whatsoever. So I remained

1:53:45

somewhat skeptical. I asked Van

1:53:47

Derson. Had he measured the number of

1:53:49

senescent cells in his treated mice, no, we're

1:53:51

planning to do that. But what was the

1:53:54

phenotypic change in the mice when you did

1:53:56

this experiment? Oh, when I... I didn't want

1:53:58

to do an experiment. of lifespan

1:54:01

experiment with an alleged

1:54:03

anti-senolithic drug until I knew

1:54:05

that it was depleting senescence. So how

1:54:07

long did you treat for? I used their

1:54:10

protocol and I asked them, I asked

1:54:12

Darren Baker, what is the dose, how

1:54:14

long do you treat the mice, and

1:54:16

how long after you add the drug

1:54:18

should you wait before you detect the

1:54:20

removal of senescent cells? And his answer

1:54:23

astonishingly was, we don't know. We've

1:54:25

never looked at that. But the nature

1:54:27

mice were treated for how long. They

1:54:29

were a long time, a long time.

1:54:31

Yeah, treated, I think they started treatment

1:54:33

at middle age, right? And I mean,

1:54:36

in the published papers, they do show

1:54:38

a reduction in P16 positive cells, and

1:54:40

you're saying you couldn't replicate that in

1:54:42

your lab. But we're conflating a

1:54:44

bunch of different issues here. We're

1:54:46

conflating the genetic model with the

1:54:49

drugs and do-so-nescent cells even exist,

1:54:51

and I feel like, I mean,

1:54:53

I think Rich's skepticism skepticismicism is

1:54:55

valid in many ways. Whether we

1:54:57

agree on the definition of senescence,

1:55:00

what people are calling senescent cells

1:55:02

do accumulate in multiple tissues with age

1:55:04

in mice and people, and if you get

1:55:06

rid of them, you can see some health

1:55:08

benefits. Am I convinced they have big effects

1:55:10

on lifespan? No, I'm not, because the data

1:55:13

is mixed and even that genetic model other

1:55:15

people haven't been able to reproduce, so it's

1:55:17

messy. But I think partly maybe start

1:55:19

with what is the definition of a

1:55:21

senescent cell, because that's where a lot

1:55:23

of this confusion comes from. That's what

1:55:25

I was saying, that there is no

1:55:27

satisfactory definition. Satisfactory to you? I mean,

1:55:29

is your issue rich that we talk

1:55:31

about it like it's one cell? Yeah,

1:55:33

that's a big part of it. You

1:55:35

can't think about it clearly if you

1:55:37

imagine that these many, many different kinds

1:55:40

of cell intrinsic changes with

1:55:42

potential pathological impacts. are all aspects

1:55:44

of the same phenomenon. But we

1:55:46

do that with other things. We

1:55:48

have mitochondrial dysfunction. There's lots of

1:55:50

different ways to get to mitochondrial

1:55:52

dysfunction. So the NIH has just

1:55:54

put about $600 million into a

1:55:56

network of researchers to study cells

1:55:58

in essence. And I'm on the advice. group

1:56:00

for that and to the extent that

1:56:02

Rich is saying these are many

1:56:04

many different things all pretending to

1:56:06

be the same thing that's clearly

1:56:08

true but they're coming up

1:56:10

with bigger and bigger and broader

1:56:13

definitions of what a senescent

1:56:15

cell is but on the other

1:56:17

hand they're also coming up with

1:56:19

more more interesting things that those senescent

1:56:21

cells do either in tissue culture

1:56:23

which I don't put much or in mice

1:56:26

I don't think the NIH would put

1:56:28

that kind of money into something if

1:56:30

they didn't feel there was a valid

1:56:32

basis. I think part of this is

1:56:34

we're calling it senescence and I think

1:56:36

none of us, to me that's stolen

1:56:38

a really good word out of the

1:56:40

vocabulary. Senescent just means aging and it

1:56:42

used to be, you could talk about

1:56:44

calendar aging, you could talk about senescence

1:56:46

which is what we now think of

1:56:48

as aging and now you can't use

1:56:50

this anymore because any time you do

1:56:52

they think you're talking about these cells.

1:56:54

Is this what they call the zombie

1:56:56

cell? I keep trying to tell you.

1:56:58

I keep trying to purge that from

1:57:01

my memory. I mean, the most common

1:57:03

definition, I think, is just an irreversibly

1:57:05

arrested cell that doesn't die and

1:57:07

typically gives off a pattern of

1:57:09

inflammatory cytokines and other factors, which

1:57:11

is a catch-all for a lot

1:57:13

of different ways to get there

1:57:15

and a lot of different states that these

1:57:17

irreversibly arrested cells can exist in. Yeah,

1:57:19

but even neurons, they're not considering senescent

1:57:21

neurons, and neurons are post-mytotic. Right, but

1:57:23

they don't always give off this pattern

1:57:25

of signals, right? That's, I mean, again,

1:57:28

this is part of the problem, as

1:57:30

you mentioned, P16. I think the, even

1:57:32

at the molecular level, the catalog of

1:57:34

markers that people are using to define

1:57:36

a senescent cell is changing, and it

1:57:38

seems to change. Yeah. Yeah. Yeah. Yeah.

1:57:40

I agree with much of what you're

1:57:42

saying, I just don't think we should.

1:57:44

throw the baby out with the bathwater

1:57:46

here and say there's nothing to this. I

1:57:48

think there is something to it and I think

1:57:50

there's lots of evidence that are there enough

1:57:52

similarities between all the different classes of

1:57:54

senescent cells that people are studying now

1:57:56

that they should be categorized as one

1:57:58

thing? I think that's a... valid conversation

1:58:01

to have. It's a good discussion point.

1:58:03

I don't think we know the answer.

1:58:05

And they discussed this a lot in

1:58:07

the synect, because even the sasp, even

1:58:09

these things that are oozing out of

1:58:12

the cells, varies quite a bit, depending

1:58:14

on the nature of the cell. That's

1:58:16

the problem, of course. You referred

1:58:18

to it, as almost anyone would, as

1:58:21

the sasp, the setup, the set-ups, and

1:58:23

essence associated proteins, secretory proteins.

1:58:25

And once you think of it as

1:58:27

the sasp, you've lost. Because the key

1:58:30

point is not to do that. The

1:58:32

key point is, here's a set of

1:58:34

set of kinds that this cell has

1:58:36

begun to make. That's really interesting. Here's

1:58:38

another set, overlapping probably. They make it

1:58:41

when you've made them stop dividing for

1:58:43

a separate reason. That's interesting. We should

1:58:45

study that. But to think you've proven

1:58:47

something about this cell type when you've

1:58:49

actually been looking at this cell type

1:58:52

because the sasp has been changed. But

1:58:54

do you think it's possible that a drug

1:58:56

such as rapomycin has part of

1:58:58

its effect? on aging through a

1:59:00

broad inhibition of a subset of

1:59:02

the sasps? I think it's very

1:59:05

likely that rapomice and changes cytokine

1:59:07

production by many different cell types

1:59:09

and that some of those changes probably

1:59:11

have health benefits. I would like

1:59:13

to know what it does to

1:59:15

the cytokine production from the macrophages

1:59:17

in the fat and the glial

1:59:19

cells in the brain and cells

1:59:21

that are in charge of protecting

1:59:23

you from viral infections. But the mistake

1:59:25

is to say, yes, it's affecting the

1:59:28

SAS. It's easy to see an analogy. If

1:59:30

I said, here's a drug and

1:59:32

it helps you because it affects

1:59:34

neurons, you'd laugh at me. Because

1:59:36

what you really want to know

1:59:38

is, is it motor neurons, sympathetic

1:59:40

neurons, parasympathetic neurons, neurons, and your

1:59:42

hypothalamus, what part of the hypothalamus,

1:59:44

the ones that control appetite? And

1:59:46

I said, no, no, no, it

1:59:48

affects neurons. I've got a drug

1:59:50

that affects neurons. But I mean,

1:59:53

people are aware of these complications

1:59:55

and are studying these complications now.

1:59:57

It seems to me that it's

1:59:59

the terminology. you object to and I

2:00:01

can appreciate that. It's thinking that I

2:00:03

object to you. The terminology is problematic

2:00:05

because it makes people stop thinking about

2:00:08

the important details and start imagining

2:00:10

that they've had a thought when

2:00:12

they say I have a drug that removes

2:00:14

senescent cells. The problem is that the

2:00:16

words trap you into patterns of thought

2:00:18

that are in this case non-productive and

2:00:20

misleading. Maybe inefficient, but the field is making,

2:00:22

I would say, quite a bit of progress.

2:00:24

And I think the way you learn about

2:00:26

the complexities, you start with a simple model,

2:00:28

you study it, and then your model gets

2:00:30

more complicated. So I totally get the frustration

2:00:33

rich, because I get as frustrated as you

2:00:35

are about senescent cells about other things. But

2:00:37

I think this is also part of the natural process

2:00:39

here. And I think what Steve said is really

2:00:41

important. The fraction of the NIH budget

2:00:43

that goes to study the biology of aging

2:00:45

of aginging through NIA has remained tiny. But

2:00:47

senescent cells are actually a really good

2:00:50

example of how a bunch of people

2:00:52

in other institutes are studying aging,

2:00:54

and they don't even know it. They're

2:00:56

studying senescence in cancer, or senescence in

2:00:58

kidney disease. So it actually has had

2:01:01

an impact in broadening the appeal and

2:01:03

scope of the field outside of NIA

2:01:05

in ways that I certainly didn't anticipate.

2:01:07

Do you think that going back to the

2:01:09

meta problem at the beginning of our discussion,

2:01:12

do you think that's maybe a better way

2:01:14

to think about... Allocating funds.

2:01:16

So for example, the NCI

2:01:18

obviously receives the most funding

2:01:20

within NIH. Maybe some of the

2:01:23

NCI funding goes to the NCI

2:01:25

to study cancer prevention through geo

2:01:28

protection. Right? If the turf war

2:01:30

is what matters. That's a good idea.

2:01:33

No, no, no. We've actually, a group

2:01:35

of us who are lobbying Congress have

2:01:37

actually asked the NIH to

2:01:39

tell us exactly this. How much

2:01:42

work in geroscience is going on?

2:01:44

in all these other institutes. Of

2:01:46

course, they're going to have

2:01:48

some motivation to minimize that or

2:01:50

maximize it or something, but at least

2:01:52

it will give us an idea. Right

2:01:54

now we have no idea how much

2:01:56

of the NCI budget is going to

2:01:58

this or NIDDK or... anything else. They

2:02:01

already have produced a report that told

2:02:03

us how much they were spending in the

2:02:05

NIA, but we already knew that. We

2:02:07

wanted to know how much they're spending

2:02:10

in the other institutes. I mean, I

2:02:12

think that could alleviate some of the

2:02:14

turf war issues, but I think what

2:02:16

you really need is the change in

2:02:18

leadership and leaders who actually recognize why

2:02:20

this is important. And that's where it

2:02:23

starts. We can have a conversation about

2:02:25

how much power does the NIH positions

2:02:27

who get it. It's going to have

2:02:29

an impact. Let's talk a little bit about Metformin. Rich,

2:02:31

do you think Metformin is geo-protective? In humans, I

2:02:34

know it doesn't appear to be in your mice. I think the

2:02:36

evidence is uncertain. There's a famous paper from Bannister that alleged that

2:02:38

diabetics on Metformin had lower mortality risks than age-like. You don't listen

2:02:40

to my podcast, do you don't listen to my podcast? I do,

2:02:42

actually, no, no, you know, you know, you know, it was a

2:02:44

different, it was a different, it was a different, it was

2:02:46

a different, it was a different, it was a different, it

2:02:48

was a different, it was a different, it was a different,

2:02:50

it was a different, it, it was a different, it, it

2:02:53

was a different, it, it, it, it was, it was, it

2:02:55

was, it was, it was, it was, it was, it was,

2:02:57

it was, it was, it was, it was, I did a

2:02:59

very lengthy treatise in a journal club comparing the Bannister paper

2:03:01

to the Keys paper and came to the conclusion that the

2:03:04

Bannister paper had too many methodology flaws to be valid. That's

2:03:06

exactly what I was going to say. As a matter of

2:03:08

fact, Keys, Christensen, who's the senior member of the group, and

2:03:10

I have just written a review article, which says exactly that.

2:03:12

You've just, that's the title of one paper. Is it

2:03:14

out yet? That's under review. Okay. Yes. So you

2:03:16

know exactly what I know exactly what I was going

2:03:18

to what I was going to what I was going to

2:03:21

what I was going to say. The question is

2:03:23

to whether Metformin would be

2:03:25

giroprotective, that is... In a

2:03:27

non-diabetic. In humans, I think,

2:03:30

is interesting, unanswered. It's not

2:03:32

the drug. I would have looked at

2:03:34

myself, if I had a big set

2:03:36

of dogs, for instance, and I wanted

2:03:38

to give them a drug that modified

2:03:40

their glucose homeostasis, I

2:03:42

would probably start with

2:03:45

something like canaglithosin that actually

2:03:47

does work in mice, which is known

2:03:49

to be safe over the long term.

2:03:51

in people. Metform and is safe over

2:03:53

the long term in people, but I

2:03:55

don't think there's much evidence that it's

2:03:57

anti-aging, leaving aside how great it is

2:03:59

for... What do you think Steve? I

2:04:01

think it's very promising. I'm skeptical

2:04:03

because I'm always skeptical in the

2:04:05

absence of evidence, but the observational

2:04:07

evidence, ignoring the banister paper, just

2:04:09

the consistency of the observational data,

2:04:11

that it reduces dementia, cancer, cardiovascular

2:04:14

disease, suggests to me there's enough

2:04:16

smoke there to look to see

2:04:18

if there's fire or not. I'll

2:04:20

send you the keys review article

2:04:22

and then you can rethink that.

2:04:24

But sorry Steve, you're saying it

2:04:26

does all of those things in

2:04:28

diabetics. Well, most of the studies

2:04:30

have been done in diabetics, absolutely.

2:04:32

And how much of that is

2:04:34

just because you're carrying the diabetes

2:04:36

is an open question. And how

2:04:38

much of that is a selection

2:04:40

for people in diabetes that are

2:04:42

progressing much less slowly because they're

2:04:44

the ones that stay on a

2:04:46

single agent as opposed to the

2:04:48

ones that progress into... Right, which

2:04:50

is why you have to do

2:04:52

the study. Yeah. Where is Tame

2:04:54

in the world of... Tame is

2:04:56

in a very preliminary state. There's

2:04:58

now enough money to get it

2:05:00

started. It has not enrolled? Anything

2:05:02

else? It's enrolling right now. Okay.

2:05:04

Previously they didn't want to start

2:05:06

it until they had enough money

2:05:08

to do the whole thing. It's

2:05:10

been impossible to get that. But

2:05:12

yeah, it's been around for eight

2:05:14

years now. And I was in

2:05:16

on the original discussion about, do

2:05:18

we do rapid mice and do

2:05:20

we do met foremen? And it

2:05:22

was all about cost and safety.

2:05:24

That was the whole thing. I

2:05:26

went in strongly advocating for rapid

2:05:28

mice and I came out saying,

2:05:30

okay, there are these cost issues.

2:05:32

And I think it was important

2:05:34

because when we went to the

2:05:36

FDA, I didn't want them to

2:05:38

think that we were trying to

2:05:40

make a bunch of money with

2:05:42

this trial and nobody's going to

2:05:44

get rich. from Metform. Why is

2:05:46

generic sorolum is so expensive still?

2:05:48

I think it's supply and demand,

2:05:50

honestly. If there was demand, yeah,

2:05:52

I think so. But coming back

2:05:54

to the Metform in question, I

2:05:56

mean, I think, first of all,

2:05:58

we don't know the answer. I

2:06:00

mean, Rich is right. We don't

2:06:02

know. So... What are our opinions?

2:06:04

My opinion is diabetes probably accelerates

2:06:06

biological aging and Metformin is effective

2:06:08

at reducing diabetic symptoms and probably

2:06:10

reduces biological aging in that context.

2:06:12

Probably doesn't. And people who are

2:06:14

not diabetic. That's my intuition. Let

2:06:16

me push back on that for

2:06:18

a second, which is diabetes is

2:06:20

an artificial diagnosis in that we

2:06:22

just make a cutoff. We say

2:06:24

you're hemoglobin A1C is 6.5. But

2:06:26

there are data that we've looked

2:06:28

at that suggest a monotonic improvement

2:06:30

in all-cause mortality as average blood

2:06:32

glucose goes down, measured by hemoglobin

2:06:34

A1C, in the non-diabetic range. Meaning,

2:06:36

people with an A1C of 5

2:06:38

live longer than people with an

2:06:40

A1C of 6, all of whom

2:06:42

are non-diabetic. Point being, if Metformins

2:06:44

gero protection comes through the regulation

2:06:46

of glucose in the patient with

2:06:48

diabetes, Does it stand to reason

2:06:50

that even in patients without diabetes

2:06:52

further attenuation of hepatic glucose output

2:06:54

is going to improve all-cause mortality?

2:06:56

Maybe. I don't know the answer,

2:06:58

obviously. I think the question is,

2:07:00

is the biomarker in this case

2:07:02

A1C, what is that actually reflecting?

2:07:04

Is that presumably reflecting some aspect

2:07:06

of metabolic homeostasis? And so, first

2:07:08

of all, does metformin in non-diabetics

2:07:10

have the desired effect or the...

2:07:12

effect we would associate with reduced

2:07:14

mortality in non-diabetics consistently, would be

2:07:16

question number one. I don't know

2:07:18

the answer to that, you probably

2:07:20

do. And I don't want to

2:07:22

speak for near, because it's been

2:07:24

a while since we've spoken, but

2:07:27

the last time I had near

2:07:29

on the podcast, his rationale for

2:07:31

why Metformin was neuroprotective, had nothing

2:07:33

to do with glucose homeostasis and

2:07:35

a non-diabetic. It was, and I

2:07:37

know you're going to love this,

2:07:39

I mean Rich, you're really going

2:07:41

to love this, there, you're really

2:07:43

going to love this. But my

2:07:45

point being not to say that

2:07:47

that's incorrect correct or anything it's

2:07:49

that there was something much more

2:07:51

primal about Metformin's actions. Now, here's

2:07:53

my pushback on that. Metformin requires

2:07:55

an organic cation transporter to get

2:07:57

into cells, as I've learned somewhat

2:07:59

recently, that muscles don't have. So,

2:08:01

if you look at the tracer

2:08:03

studies, Metformin does not get into

2:08:05

muscles. It gets into enterocytes and

2:08:07

the liver. It's very concentrated in

2:08:09

the liver. Gets in the gut.

2:08:11

Unclear from these tracer studies, if

2:08:13

it's getting into immune cells. So

2:08:15

Navcendel tells me that he believes

2:08:17

they are getting into immune cells

2:08:19

as well. So the question is,

2:08:21

at least I think we need

2:08:23

to ask ourselves the question, if

2:08:25

it's working, which cells is it

2:08:27

working on and how? And so

2:08:29

the liver part's easy. Everybody gets

2:08:31

big concentration of metformin shows up

2:08:33

here. We sort of understand that

2:08:35

that reduces hepatic glucose output. After

2:08:37

that, I'm sort of scratching my

2:08:39

head going, I don't know how

2:08:41

it works. Well, we know it

2:08:43

has a target in the mitochondrial

2:08:45

complex one, it inhibits, we know

2:08:47

it affects. Well, that's my point,

2:08:49

like it's not in the muscle.

2:08:51

That's a question. And we also

2:08:53

know that it activates AMPK. But

2:08:55

those mechanisms are probably related. This

2:08:57

is why Neur points at two

2:08:59

of the hallmarks. I just have

2:09:01

to tell you this. But here's

2:09:03

interesting thing. Good friend of ours,

2:09:05

George Martin, George Martin, George Martin,

2:09:07

who died a couple of ours,

2:09:09

George Martin, who died a couple

2:09:11

of a couple of a couple

2:09:13

of a couple of a couple

2:09:15

of years ago. once went through

2:09:17

and catalogued all the human diseases

2:09:19

he could and tried to look

2:09:21

at the similarities of their phenotypic

2:09:23

changes relative to what happens with

2:09:25

normal aging. He came up with

2:09:27

diabetes as having the most similarities

2:09:29

to accelerated aging of any of

2:09:31

the groups that he looked at

2:09:33

which in this sentence the glycosylation,

2:09:35

the hyper growth factors like insulin,

2:09:37

IGF1, all these things. I mean,

2:09:39

there's logic to that. Let me

2:09:41

agree with the... emphasis you were

2:09:43

just putting on organ specific and

2:09:45

tissue specific changes and I think

2:09:47

it's about time to get away

2:09:49

from what does Metforman do to

2:09:51

the body or any of these

2:09:53

drugs for that matter and start

2:09:55

to think what does it do

2:09:57

to each of the interesting players

2:09:59

and how they talk to one

2:10:01

another. Someone in my lab has

2:10:03

been looking at the enzymes related

2:10:05

to de novo lipogenesis, and she's

2:10:07

been looking at a couple of

2:10:09

different kinds of slow aging mice,

2:10:11

and it has major effects in

2:10:13

the liver, and it has major

2:10:15

effects on white and brown adipose

2:10:17

tissue, and they go in different

2:10:19

directions, and which is primary, which

2:10:21

is reactive, whether any of these

2:10:23

are related to the effects of

2:10:25

the mutations on the muscle or

2:10:27

the brain, is now an open

2:10:29

question. having a diagram of hallmarks

2:10:31

which are changed by a drug

2:10:33

is much less useful than asking

2:10:35

what specific changes in what cell

2:10:37

types of which organs that talk

2:10:40

to each other are being changed

2:10:42

by this drug as a primary

2:10:44

or as a secondary or as

2:10:46

a compensatory effect. That's how you'll

2:10:48

start to get into first mechanisms,

2:10:50

but also start to be able

2:10:52

to think clearly about ways of

2:10:54

targeting therapy so that it has

2:10:56

a benefit with fewer and fewer

2:10:58

side effects. Let's use Kanaga Flosen

2:11:00

as an example. We've demonstrated, and

2:11:02

I use we very liberally here,

2:11:04

you've demonstrated that it reduces all-cause

2:11:06

mortality in your mice in males.

2:11:08

And we know exactly what Kanaga

2:11:10

Flosin does in the kidney, and

2:11:12

we know that those mice lived

2:11:14

longer. Do you believe that the

2:11:16

longevity benefit came through glycemic control?

2:11:18

Because there was no difference in

2:11:20

weight, if I recall. They actually

2:11:22

lost weight. Males and females lost

2:11:24

weight on Kanaga Flosin. Was the

2:11:26

difference in weight statistically significant between

2:11:28

the long-lived males and the normal

2:11:30

males? The mice treated with the

2:11:32

drug were lighter in weight than

2:11:34

controls and that's true of both

2:11:36

sexes. So the weight loss wasn't

2:11:38

necessarily what explained. The cat actually

2:11:40

lost more weight in females than

2:11:42

in males. So the question is

2:11:44

very valid and we do not

2:11:46

know the answer, SGL2, is on

2:11:48

many other cell types and it's

2:11:50

quite possible, very plausible. that canagglithosin

2:11:52

had an effect principally through controlling

2:11:54

peak daily blood glucose, not average,

2:11:56

but peak. And it's also possible

2:11:58

that it had effects on cells

2:12:00

of unknown origin in the brain

2:12:02

and all of these are... very

2:12:04

valid and I don't think anyone

2:12:06

knows the answer. It's well worth

2:12:08

evaluating. There are other inhibitors of

2:12:10

SGLT to an SGLT one that

2:12:12

have differential cell specificities and differential

2:12:14

effects on different cell types and

2:12:16

looking at those would help give

2:12:18

you glimpses into this question. We

2:12:20

guessed it had to do with

2:12:22

glucose but we might be... So

2:12:24

what is your intuition Steve? Going

2:12:26

back to Metformin? My intuition is

2:12:28

that it might work. I don't

2:12:30

have a strong opinion. There's enough

2:12:32

suggestive evidence that I think it's

2:12:34

worth a trial. I think that

2:12:36

if we wait until we figure

2:12:38

out exactly what each drug does

2:12:40

in each cell type, it will

2:12:42

take us forever to get any

2:12:44

therapies and in medicine, there have

2:12:46

been many, many advances that came

2:12:48

about before we understood the mechanistic

2:12:50

underpinning. And if there's enough suggestive

2:12:52

evidence and there's not a lot

2:12:54

of side effects, suggest me that

2:12:56

it's worth digging into now because

2:12:58

the benefits are so enormous. Like

2:13:00

we said, one year healthy aging,

2:13:02

38 trillion dollars. That should talk

2:13:04

to Congress if nothing else does.

2:13:06

Well, and I would also say,

2:13:08

tame could be successful independent of

2:13:10

whether Metformin is effective at slowing

2:13:12

biological aging. By getting others into

2:13:14

the field, do you mean? Well,

2:13:16

also, even just hitting the endpoints,

2:13:18

right? So the end point is

2:13:20

multimorbidity or comorbidity. So it's quite

2:13:22

possible that the trial will be

2:13:24

successful, even if Metformin is not

2:13:26

effective, gerotherapeutic. It's also true. It

2:13:28

may not succeed. for a variety

2:13:30

of reasons the clinical trials don't

2:13:32

succeed. I sort of agree with

2:13:34

Steve, like I'm supportive of doing

2:13:36

the trial. I also agree, I

2:13:38

think, with probably both Steve and

2:13:40

Rich, that it's not what I

2:13:42

would pick if I was going

2:13:44

to do one trial, if we

2:13:46

could only do one trial, but

2:13:48

we have to start somewhere. Matt,

2:13:50

why do you think that the

2:13:53

ITP studies for rapamice and always

2:13:55

worked? Because inhibiting Mtor, Increases lifespan

2:13:57

and slow as aging. I know

2:13:59

what you're at. Yeah, you know

2:14:01

where I'm going. Because most people

2:14:03

who are using rapid mice and

2:14:05

off label have moved to once

2:14:07

weekly or some sort of cycling

2:14:09

like that. So I think one

2:14:11

question is, would that increase lifespan

2:14:13

in mice as much or more

2:14:15

than daily? We don't effectively know

2:14:17

the answer to that question. I

2:14:19

don't think you guys do some

2:14:21

intermittent. It can increase lifespan, but

2:14:23

it's never been dose optimized, right?

2:14:25

I think this is the question.

2:14:27

Is the metabolic rate of the

2:14:29

mouse so fast that giving the

2:14:31

mouse daily RAPA is not the

2:14:33

same as giving the human daily

2:14:35

RAPA? Yes, and the RAPA in

2:14:37

the ITP study is in the

2:14:39

food. So it's not a single

2:14:41

dose, or it's not a single...

2:14:43

They're just chowing on it all

2:14:45

day. Well, at least during the

2:14:47

period of day that they're eating

2:14:49

and have access. I'll let Rich

2:14:51

talk about what they know about

2:14:53

the blood levels, but it is

2:14:55

a fundamentally way of delivering the

2:14:57

drug. I mean, I guess in

2:14:59

2008 or seven when you did

2:15:01

the first study, maybe it wasn't

2:15:03

clear this idea of Mtor 1

2:15:05

versus Mtor 2 and the constitutive

2:15:07

dosing. Maybe we should ask how

2:15:09

many people at this table actually

2:15:11

believe that model. Yeah, that's kind

2:15:13

of where I want to go.

2:15:15

Yeah, that's kind of where I

2:15:17

want to go. I want to

2:15:19

understand what I want to go.

2:15:21

I want to understand what I'm

2:15:23

talking about. That the bad side

2:15:25

effects come from Mtor 1. I

2:15:27

don't know enough to say. Many

2:15:29

of our slow-aging mice, actually, M-tore

2:15:31

Complex 1 function is down in

2:15:33

all of them, but M-tore Complex

2:15:35

2 is often up. And it's

2:15:37

up in an interesting way. Mice

2:15:39

eat mostly at night, and they

2:15:41

more or less fast during the

2:15:43

day. In our slow-aging mice, M-tore

2:15:45

Complex 2 is elevated, but it

2:15:47

no longer responds in the fasting

2:15:49

period, but it doesn't respond to

2:15:51

food in the same way. So

2:15:53

their complex changes. in both its

2:15:55

baseline state and its response to

2:15:57

food weatheries, what happened in people,

2:15:59

what happened in people taking it

2:16:01

every other day, every fifth day,

2:16:03

whether they are beneficial or harmful

2:16:05

or a mixture. really don't know.

2:16:07

The Emtor complex two-story is trickier.

2:16:09

The other thing is that is,

2:16:11

I think, important but not really

2:16:13

appreciated is that it's not only

2:16:15

Emtor complex one drugs like rapomice

2:16:17

and not only lower the overall

2:16:19

effect, but it also changes the

2:16:21

substrate specificity so that the kinase

2:16:23

that is susceptible to torque inhibition

2:16:25

that looks at a ribosomal protein,

2:16:27

S6, that goes down and doesn't

2:16:29

work nearly as well as inhibited.

2:16:31

For how long? I don't know.

2:16:33

But the other aspect of tour

2:16:35

downstream is on a protein called

2:16:37

40 BP1 that's involved in translation.

2:16:39

It does not change that kinase.

2:16:41

What it does is it changes

2:16:43

the total amount of the protein.

2:16:45

So the proportion of the protein

2:16:47

that's phosphorylate drops down, but the

2:16:49

actual kinase that adds the phosphate

2:16:51

to that substrate is unchanged. So

2:16:53

whether that's important, that it's having

2:16:55

at least two different pathways. that

2:16:57

are being influenced in one case

2:16:59

by changing the substrate and in

2:17:01

the other case by changing the

2:17:04

kinase. No one's really looked at

2:17:06

that. They say it's a drug

2:17:08

that blocks and torque kinase one

2:17:10

function. And downstream is where a

2:17:12

lot of the action is. I

2:17:14

know your lab at one point

2:17:16

was interested in cell type specific

2:17:18

inhibitors of the torque complex one.

2:17:20

I don't know whether you... Everything

2:17:22

you just said, rich occurs in

2:17:24

what cell? Mouse liver. What about

2:17:26

muscle? Well, the overall decline in

2:17:28

the ratio of phosphorylated versus substrate.

2:17:30

We also published that, I think,

2:17:32

in Muscle and Kidney. I would

2:17:34

have to go back to the

2:17:36

papers and see whether we also

2:17:38

found the elevation of the substrate

2:17:40

before ABP1 in both of those

2:17:42

tissues. I vaguely recall that it

2:17:44

was the substrate that changed, not

2:17:46

the kinase in those tissues as

2:17:48

well, but I'd rather look it

2:17:50

up before I sign my name

2:17:52

to it. But even what Rich

2:17:54

is saying is, wow, I mean,

2:17:56

really important and informative, also only

2:17:58

a tiny piece of all the

2:18:00

downstream things that Amtor affects, and

2:18:02

I think the point is we

2:18:04

just really don't have a... understanding

2:18:06

of how rapomycin or fasting or

2:18:08

other drugs that hit emptor are

2:18:10

affecting all of the things that

2:18:12

are downstream of. I agree completely.

2:18:14

Let me give you an example.

2:18:16

Linda Partridge just published in bio-arcove

2:18:18

at least a nice paper. Rapomycin

2:18:20

increased lifespan of her mice. If

2:18:22

she added an inhibitor of a

2:18:24

different kinase called irk, it did

2:18:26

better. The inhibition by irk worked

2:18:28

by itself, but it actually improved

2:18:30

on rapomycin. So two people in

2:18:32

my lab are looking at that.

2:18:34

And it turns out that the

2:18:36

Erk-Kainase inhibitor is working in an

2:18:38

entirely different pathway. It's affecting the

2:18:40

proteome by increasing the degradation through

2:18:42

a chavron immediate autophagee mechanism, which

2:18:44

is not affected by Raphamison. At

2:18:46

least at the dose they used,

2:18:48

right? At least at the dose

2:18:50

they used, right? At least at

2:18:52

the dose they used, right, that's

2:18:54

right. Sorry, what model was this?

2:18:56

This was mice. Nice. Yeah, it

2:18:58

was a black six. Oh, okay.

2:19:00

So it's agreeing with an amplifying

2:19:02

the question, there may well be

2:19:04

multiple cell intrinsic pathways, some of

2:19:06

which are tour-dependent, some of which

2:19:08

are map kinase, erk-dependent, which can

2:19:10

synergize, as in the Partridge case

2:19:12

for lifespan, but also potentially synergize

2:19:14

for health impact. Yeah, and here's,

2:19:16

I mean, I think an important,

2:19:18

again, limitation to what's been done.

2:19:20

There are drugs out there that's

2:19:22

been done. There are drugs out

2:19:24

there, ATP competitive inhibitors that have

2:19:26

different affinities for different types of

2:19:28

kinases. Haven't been tested for longevity.

2:19:30

These dual kinase inhibitors. In fact,

2:19:32

in the restor bio trial, the

2:19:34

last one, the phase three, which

2:19:36

did not get to completion, they

2:19:38

substituted. They took the wrapalog out

2:19:40

and used an ATP competitive drug.

2:19:42

Didn't know that. So what is

2:19:44

your belief, Matt, around? dosing wrapa

2:19:46

in humans than or even in

2:19:48

your dogs. You're doing it. We're

2:19:50

doing it once a week now.

2:19:52

We've moved to once a week.

2:19:54

So, I mean, maybe it's worth

2:19:56

at least talking about how that

2:19:58

evolved and this is my... understanding

2:20:00

of how we got to where

2:20:02

we are today, which is that

2:20:04

most people using wrapamison off label

2:20:06

for potential health span effects, most

2:20:08

doctors prescribing it, are recommending once

2:20:10

weekly dosing in the three to

2:20:12

six, sometimes eight 10 milligram range.

2:20:14

So the first place I'm aware

2:20:17

of in the literature where this

2:20:19

was showing to have a potential

2:20:21

benefit for anything related to aging

2:20:23

was Joan Manick's work. when she

2:20:25

was first at Novartis and then

2:20:27

at Restor Bio looking at flu

2:20:29

vaccine response in elderly people, and

2:20:31

they were using everlimas, so a

2:20:33

derivative of rapomycin, and they found

2:20:35

that for vaccine response, it was

2:20:37

most effective and had the least

2:20:39

side effects at once weekly dosing

2:20:41

at five milligrams, and they tested.

2:20:43

daily, one or two migs, five

2:20:45

migs once a week, 20 minutes

2:20:47

a week. Yeah, it was once,

2:20:49

it was a milligram a day,

2:20:51

five once a week, 21s a

2:20:53

week. Now I've had both Lloyd

2:20:55

Clixtine and Joan Manic on the

2:20:57

podcast. It's been so long that

2:20:59

I don't recall if I asked

2:21:01

them why. They designed the trial

2:21:03

with those forearms. So my understanding

2:21:05

is that Novartis had internal data

2:21:07

at that point on side effects

2:21:09

and had an internal hypothesis that

2:21:11

if you let the trough levels

2:21:13

bottom levels bottom out. that reduced

2:21:15

side effects. The side effects in

2:21:17

organ transplant patients were largely driven

2:21:19

high troughs. High troughs. Yep. And

2:21:21

then after that they developed, based

2:21:23

off of David Sabatini's work and

2:21:25

then Dudley Laming, after he left

2:21:27

David's lab, a hypothesis that chronic

2:21:29

treatment with rapomycin, which maybe would

2:21:31

be equivalent to daily dosing in

2:21:33

people, this was all done in

2:21:35

cells, led to off-target effects on

2:21:37

Mtor Complex 2, that were driving

2:21:39

the side effects. So that got...

2:21:41

sort of dogmatized as the truth,

2:21:43

actually don't think there's a ton

2:21:45

of evidence beyond those initial papers

2:21:47

to support the idea that the

2:21:49

side effects are all through Mtor

2:21:51

Complex too. The idea is, if

2:21:53

you dose once a week... You

2:21:55

let the trough levels bottom out.

2:21:57

You don't get the off-target effects

2:21:59

on Mtor Complex too. You avoid

2:22:01

the side effects. Again, we don't

2:22:03

have definitive data. The data I've

2:22:05

seen seem consistent with that idea.

2:22:07

People dosing daily seem to be

2:22:09

more likely to have side effects.

2:22:11

Mostly things like bacterial infections or

2:22:13

the really severe mouth stores, but

2:22:15

sort of anecdotal and I don't

2:22:17

know for sure how strong that

2:22:19

data is in people. It did

2:22:21

hold up in all of the

2:22:23

restor biocclinical clinical trials that I'm

2:22:25

aware of. really didn't show any

2:22:27

side effects different from placebo. In

2:22:29

the dog study, you're using a

2:22:31

slow release formulation. It's an enteric

2:22:33

coded. It's a different formulation than

2:22:35

what the ITP uses, but all

2:22:37

of the human cirrolumus formulations have

2:22:39

some way to get to the

2:22:41

small intestine. So it's not substantially

2:22:43

different, I don't think, than wrap

2:22:45

immune or the generic cirrolumus you

2:22:47

would get. Let's do the closest

2:22:49

thing that a group like this

2:22:51

could do in terms of a

2:22:53

speed round. I'm going to go

2:22:55

through a couple of other ideas.

2:22:57

I just want to get the,

2:22:59

what are you thinking about this?

2:23:01

Can we say anything positive about

2:23:03

risveratrol? No. Rich? No. Why does

2:23:05

this thing not die? Why is

2:23:07

there still a hundred different risveratrols

2:23:09

being sold on Amazon? Why do

2:23:11

I still get people asking me,

2:23:13

do you take resveratrol? Should I

2:23:15

be taking resveratrol? It has a

2:23:17

good PR team. I think it's

2:23:19

really hard to prove something doesn't

2:23:21

work. So once it gets in

2:23:23

the consciousness as improving health, I

2:23:25

mean, even in the longevity field,

2:23:27

Jesus Christ, I was saying the

2:23:30

resveratrol stuff was garbage for 10

2:23:32

years before people believed it. Now

2:23:34

everybody believes it. But it takes

2:23:36

a really long time. studying resveratrol

2:23:38

in the aging field anymore. Think

2:23:40

if you went to a conference

2:23:42

and asked scientists, what do you

2:23:44

think about resveratrol? You'd get the

2:23:46

same answer here with maybe one

2:23:48

exception. But I think. I think

2:23:50

it takes a really hard time.

2:23:52

Just one exception. It takes a

2:23:54

long time to change. Bad ideas

2:23:56

don't die hard. That's right. And

2:23:58

that's true in the scientific literature.

2:24:00

And it's especially true when there's

2:24:02

a profit motive to continue selling

2:24:04

this stuff. And I'm not 100%

2:24:06

convinced that there are no health

2:24:08

benefits from resveratrol. Pretty convinced there's

2:24:10

no reason to believe it affects

2:24:12

the biology of aging or is

2:24:14

a longevity drug. But I can't

2:24:16

say for sure that. Nobody would

2:24:18

ever benefit from any dose of

2:24:20

resveratrol. Yeah, but we couldn't say

2:24:22

that about anything. I agree Yeah,

2:24:24

yeah. Now we could say that

2:24:26

if you were force-fed the highest

2:24:28

fat diet in the world such

2:24:30

that your liver encroached on your

2:24:32

lungs through your diaphragm, isn't there

2:24:34

a chance rich that under that

2:24:36

situation resveratrol might help? I have

2:24:38

no idea. Wasn't that the one

2:24:40

and only one experiment that worked?

2:24:42

Yeah, the famous experiment which was

2:24:44

published as resveratrol, the first drug

2:24:46

ever found to extend mouse lifespan,

2:24:48

to extend mouse life spent. It

2:24:50

turns out that the mice die

2:24:52

because they were on a 60%

2:24:54

coconut oil diet. It's poisonous to

2:24:56

the extent that it causes the

2:24:58

liver to fill with fat and

2:25:00

compresses the thorax so that they

2:25:02

cannot inhale. Three or four papers

2:25:04

later, they published as an obscure

2:25:06

paragraph in a discussion section on

2:25:08

a paper. Pearson was the first

2:25:10

author of the second paper that,

2:25:12

oh, by the way, all these

2:25:14

mice on the coconut oil diet.

2:25:16

Finally, we've looked at them. They're

2:25:18

all dying because of lung compaction

2:25:20

due to expansion of the liver.

2:25:22

So the notion that their drug

2:25:24

had slowed aging because on the

2:25:26

60% coconut on our diet, it

2:25:28

temporarily extended lifespan was due to

2:25:30

the prevention of this extremely bizarre

2:25:32

phenomenon. I just cannot get enough

2:25:34

of that story. Let's all documented

2:25:36

in the literature. I believe I

2:25:38

know it won't. Two separate papers.

2:25:40

All right, let's have a word

2:25:42

on NAD, NR, NMMN. Steve, what

2:25:44

is your point of view on

2:25:46

this? Well, the current state of

2:25:48

evidence. I'm skeptical. It's one of

2:25:50

those things that makes a great

2:25:52

deal of conceptual sense, but the

2:25:54

evidence at this point is not

2:25:56

very compelling. And we have the

2:25:58

ITP evidence that is, I think,

2:26:00

the strongest. And there was strongest

2:26:02

negative evidence. Yes. Yes. Yeah. Just

2:26:04

make it fair. I assume that

2:26:06

people knew that I guess I

2:26:08

should. And is it your view,

2:26:10

Steve, that this stuff probably does

2:26:12

not extend lifespan? Maybe there is

2:26:14

some other health span benefit out

2:26:16

there that has just not been

2:26:18

studied. The right experiment hasn't been

2:26:20

done. It hasn't been powered. Pick

2:26:22

your favorite excuse. I think NED

2:26:24

is very, very interesting molecule. And

2:26:26

I don't think NED is very,

2:26:28

very interesting molecule. And I don't

2:26:30

think we could throw out manipulating

2:26:32

NED is something that could be

2:26:34

important for agents. I just don't

2:26:36

think the evidence is there at

2:26:38

this point. Do you think if

2:26:40

you're going to manipulate it? That,

2:26:43

I don't know. I will express

2:26:45

complete ignorance on that. Matt, what

2:26:47

is your point of view on

2:26:49

all of this? Yeah, well, I

2:26:51

think the way you framed that

2:26:53

question to Steve is indicative of

2:26:55

why it's so hard to disprove

2:26:57

something, especially when there are people

2:26:59

out there who have money to

2:27:01

make, who really want to make

2:27:03

the case that you should buy

2:27:05

this stuff. Because it's always possible

2:27:07

that there's some way that this

2:27:09

could be beneficial. Having said that,

2:27:11

NAD, like Steve said, central molecule

2:27:13

in thousands of chemical reactions of

2:27:15

chemical reactions of chemical reactions of

2:27:17

chemical reactions, really important. Good reason,

2:27:19

I don't know about good reason,

2:27:21

some reason to believe that NAD

2:27:23

homeostasis declines with age, like lots

2:27:25

of many other things, so it's

2:27:27

plausible that if you fix that,

2:27:29

you can get benefits from it.

2:27:31

The data is decidedly mixed, both

2:27:33

in the literature, preclinical literature, and

2:27:35

in people as to whether or

2:27:37

not boosting NAD increases lifespan, improves

2:27:39

health span. So I think there's

2:27:41

lots of issues. What's the most

2:27:43

positive data you would point to?

2:27:45

Well, for Lifespan, the original study

2:27:47

by Johann Auerx's lab, where they

2:27:49

started treating, I think at 20

2:27:51

months of age, was published in

2:27:53

science, I believe, showed an effect

2:27:55

that was reasonable. good size except

2:27:57

the controls were short-lived, which is

2:27:59

a different issue. Yeah, that's different

2:28:01

issue, right? There's a number of

2:28:03

cases where something was reported to

2:28:05

increase lifespan when the controls were

2:28:07

short-lived and then when the study

2:28:09

was repeated and longer-lived, you didn't

2:28:11

see an longer-lived, you didn't see

2:28:13

an effect. I don't know why

2:28:15

there was a difference between that

2:28:17

study and the ITP. But that's

2:28:19

probably the best case you can

2:28:21

point to. There's studies in C.

2:28:23

But then I think when you

2:28:25

talk about the precursors, it's even

2:28:27

more complicated than maybe boosting NAD

2:28:29

could slow aging because can you

2:28:31

get the right doses in people?

2:28:33

You talked about bioavailability. Is there

2:28:35

any difference between NMMN, NNR, Niacin,

2:28:37

nicotine, or when you take it

2:28:39

orally, the data suggests that it

2:28:41

all gets broken down to Niacin

2:28:43

in the gut? So why are

2:28:45

people taking 70-dollar NMM? Or NNR?

2:28:47

Why are people selling it? Some

2:28:49

of them are scientists. dodge that

2:28:51

question. It's complicated. I don't personally

2:28:53

believe there is enough evidence to

2:28:55

think that NED precursors as they

2:28:57

are being marketed today are likely

2:28:59

to benefit most people. Some people,

2:29:01

probably people who have conditions of

2:29:03

dysregulated NED, could get a benefit.

2:29:05

I don't think there's any difference

2:29:07

between the various molecules that are

2:29:09

being marketed right now. And there's

2:29:11

at least one study in mice

2:29:13

that giving NMMN to aged mice

2:29:15

causes kidney inflammation and potentially kidney

2:29:17

pathology. I'm not saying NMM is

2:29:19

dangerous, but when you try to

2:29:21

weigh the risk reward, if it

2:29:23

causes kidney pathology in aged mice,

2:29:25

at least at high doses, could

2:29:27

it do the same thing in

2:29:29

dogs or people? Yeah, it could.

2:29:31

And it bothers me, particularly in

2:29:33

the companion animal space, that people

2:29:35

are marketing NMM. for people's pets

2:29:37

when they know that it might

2:29:39

cause kidney disease in people's dogs

2:29:41

and cats. That's problematic to me.

2:29:43

We talked briefly about bit. See,

2:29:45

is there going to be a

2:29:47

day when the substance found in

2:29:49

the blood of someone much younger

2:29:51

than you when infused into you

2:29:53

whilst some of your old blood

2:29:56

is removed is going to, assuming

2:29:58

we figure out what frequency that

2:30:00

has to be done in fact

2:30:02

your life? Yeah, I think this

2:30:04

is an incredibly interesting question and

2:30:06

it really deserves to be investigated.

2:30:08

in detail because if it's true

2:30:10

it's a real game changer because

2:30:12

we do transfusions I mean this

2:30:14

is not exotic medicine I think

2:30:16

we very much need to know

2:30:18

whether this works the same way

2:30:20

in people and also it would

2:30:22

be nice to know how much

2:30:24

of it is due to the

2:30:26

taking young blood versus how much

2:30:28

of it is getting rid of

2:30:30

the old book but the evidence

2:30:32

from mice is very very compelling

2:30:34

it is Steve if we could

2:30:36

design the perfect experiments that would

2:30:38

try to ask these questions. Let's

2:30:40

just say we started by doing

2:30:42

just the one experiment, which was

2:30:44

the full peribiosis, so the putting

2:30:46

in the taking out, we didn't

2:30:48

try to disentangle the effect, and

2:30:50

there was no benefit in humans.

2:30:52

What would be your best hypothesis

2:30:54

as to why it would have

2:30:56

failed, assuming it was statistically powered

2:30:58

correctly, and there was no methodology

2:31:00

error. If this was a biologic

2:31:02

result, why would you think, given

2:31:04

how favorable this has been in

2:31:06

mice, it would not occur in

2:31:08

humans? products that ended up in

2:31:10

the circulation of humans was a

2:31:12

very different nature than in mice.

2:31:14

The number of things that differ

2:31:16

between humans and mice and blood

2:31:18

would be enormous. So pinning it

2:31:20

down would be. But I think

2:31:22

there probably is some reason to

2:31:24

suspect that it may work. I'm

2:31:26

very impressed. I mean if it

2:31:28

does work, this is an opportunity

2:31:30

that we had the technology to

2:31:32

do this 50 years ago, right?

2:31:34

Right. And it may not work

2:31:36

in young people. But it may

2:31:38

work in older people. I think

2:31:40

there's a lot of drugs that

2:31:42

could affect aging. That because young

2:31:44

people haven't aged as much might

2:31:46

not have minimal effect, but you

2:31:48

give it to somebody, you know,

2:31:50

50 years later, might have a

2:31:52

big effect. I find myself frustrated

2:31:54

by the question, rather than by

2:31:56

the answer, because... You've got a

2:31:58

horrible question, Asker here, Rich, is

2:32:00

the problem. I think you are

2:32:02

well above average, but this particular

2:32:04

one, I think, is illustrative, because

2:32:06

the reason like people like probiosis

2:32:08

is that they've seen it in

2:32:10

a sci-fi movie. It sounds exactly

2:32:12

like what you do in sci-fi

2:32:14

and they're flashing lights and it's

2:32:16

so sexy and it's just so

2:32:18

great and you can take the

2:32:20

blood of young virgins and give

2:32:22

it to old people and they

2:32:24

stand up and they can get

2:32:26

on. I didn't realize they had

2:32:28

to be virgins. But none of

2:32:30

that is pertinent. Pertinent is, is

2:32:32

there something that is in the

2:32:34

blood of old people that it

2:32:36

would be good to remove and

2:32:38

if so, what is it? And

2:32:40

is there something, a cell, a

2:32:42

molecule, a set of three molecules?

2:32:44

that's in the blood of young

2:32:46

people or mice that would be

2:32:48

good for you. The only virtue

2:32:50

of this parabiosis circus is to

2:32:52

suggest that, you know, the answer

2:32:54

might be yes. There might be

2:32:56

something you could remove from old

2:32:58

blood a cell or some plasma

2:33:00

molecule, and there might be something

2:33:02

good in the blood of young

2:33:04

individuals. So the challenge now is

2:33:06

to find out what those things

2:33:09

are, and then you can do

2:33:11

real-life science. Real life science is

2:33:13

not done by taking blood from

2:33:15

young people and putting it into

2:33:17

old people. That's medieval science where

2:33:19

there's a complex mixture of dozens

2:33:21

and hundreds of potential. In other

2:33:23

words, you might start with that

2:33:25

and no one thinks that if

2:33:27

you do that experiment where you

2:33:29

literally take blood out of an

2:33:31

old person and discard it and

2:33:33

take blood out of a young

2:33:35

person and put it in. and

2:33:37

you get a favorable result nobody

2:33:39

thinks that that's what's going to

2:33:41

the FDA that is the proof

2:33:43

of concept what experiments would be

2:33:45

worth you've a limited amount of

2:33:47

volunteers, doctors, and money, what experiments

2:33:49

are most informative? And in my

2:33:51

view, by far the most informative

2:33:53

experiments are, what is in the

2:33:55

blood of young mice that is

2:33:57

so good, and what is in

2:33:59

the blood of an- But I

2:34:01

don't know, would you want to

2:34:03

go on that fishing expedition until

2:34:05

you at least saw a signal?

2:34:07

Yes, people are doing it. I

2:34:09

mean, there are companies doing it,

2:34:11

and on the basic research side.

2:34:13

Of course they are. I'm asking

2:34:15

a different question, though, which is...

2:34:17

Yes, that's the only way you

2:34:19

can turn your idea into science.

2:34:21

Well, on the other hand, if

2:34:23

it has a positive effect, I

2:34:25

don't think it really matters. That's

2:34:27

something to be investigated later. My

2:34:29

thought is it's not simple. It's

2:34:31

not GDF 11, for sure. If

2:34:33

it were simple, there's enough people

2:34:35

looking at it, they would have

2:34:37

figured it out. My guess is

2:34:39

some combination. If there's something there,

2:34:41

there's some combination. I mean, why

2:34:43

can't you do both? I think

2:34:45

Peter and I are saying the

2:34:47

same thing. Would we love to

2:34:49

understand the mechanism? Yeah, absolutely. Do

2:34:51

we have to understand the mechanism?

2:34:53

Yeah, absolutely. Do we have to

2:34:55

understand the mechanism? Do we have

2:34:57

to understand the mechanism? Yeah, absolutely.

2:34:59

Do we have to understand the

2:35:01

mechanism? loves to test individual chemical

2:35:03

compounds, even sometimes ones where the

2:35:05

mechanism of action is not known.

2:35:07

And that's very sensible. We are

2:35:09

very dubious about, let's take a

2:35:11

little vis and a little of

2:35:13

that and a little of that,

2:35:15

and we're really dubious about taking,

2:35:17

let's grind up a little dubious

2:35:19

about, and a little of that,

2:35:21

a little of that, and we're

2:35:23

really dubious about taking, let's grind

2:35:25

up the asparagus, who's really dubious

2:35:27

about taking, let's grind. And we're

2:35:29

really dubious of each drug. What

2:35:31

I'm saying is that if you

2:35:33

have a very complex mixture of

2:35:35

hundreds of molecules and something happens,

2:35:37

you don't know what to do

2:35:39

next because it could be any

2:35:41

one or two or eight or

2:35:43

ten of those and you haven't

2:35:45

really decided you have troubles and

2:35:47

then with standardization with mechanistic tests

2:35:49

and with transferring to... a key

2:35:51

species like something. But my thought

2:35:53

is we still wouldn't be using

2:35:55

anesthesia if we had to wait

2:35:57

until we figured out how it

2:35:59

worked. Yeah, and it doesn't have

2:36:01

to be peribiosis. It doesn't have

2:36:03

to be taking blood from young

2:36:05

people and putting it into old

2:36:07

people, right? There are other variants

2:36:09

of this that can be done

2:36:11

clinically, and there's some evidence to

2:36:13

support things like therapeutic plasma exchange

2:36:15

or things like that. So should

2:36:17

we test it? I think so.

2:36:20

And my gut feeling is, yeah,

2:36:22

probably will have some benefits in

2:36:24

benefits in people. So if you

2:36:26

could only do one experiment, would

2:36:28

you do a plasma for recess

2:36:30

experiment? And if so, would you

2:36:32

test? The simplest one is you

2:36:34

literally just exchange old plasma for

2:36:36

albumin. That's what they're typically doing

2:36:38

in these studies. Yeah. First of

2:36:40

all, I don't know enough about

2:36:42

this area to be confident in

2:36:44

my answer, but yeah, that's probably

2:36:46

where I would look to start

2:36:48

simply because it's going to be

2:36:50

logistically easier to do from a

2:36:52

clinical trial perspective. So scientifically then,

2:36:54

the hypothesis is it's the presence

2:36:56

of something bad. Well, it's both.

2:36:58

That is worse than the absence

2:37:00

of something good. Because the albumin's

2:37:02

not going to give you the

2:37:04

young person. That's the problem with

2:37:06

that experiment to me. We don't

2:37:08

know now. We don't. If it's

2:37:10

young blood is good, old blood

2:37:12

is bad or some combination. We

2:37:14

would automatically, if we only did

2:37:16

the plasma ferasis, we would only

2:37:18

be testing part of that. I'd

2:37:20

push back on that. I think

2:37:22

we do have reason to believe

2:37:24

it's a combination of both. There's

2:37:26

data in both directions. That's why

2:37:28

I proposed starting with that experiment.

2:37:30

Yeah. I think that's again, as

2:37:32

much as anything, sure, in this

2:37:34

field. That's not as sure as

2:37:36

rapid mice and mice, but there's

2:37:38

at least evidence to support that

2:37:40

idea. Last thing I'll say you

2:37:42

asked why might it fail might

2:37:44

fail in fail in humans. the

2:37:46

peribiosis experiment itself shortens lifespan in

2:37:48

rodents. And so just the fact

2:37:50

that you're surgically connecting these animals

2:37:52

together. So it may be that

2:37:54

the benefit from peribiosis true peribiosis

2:37:56

in that context is somehow related

2:37:58

to the shortening of lifespan due

2:38:00

to the procedure. I don't think

2:38:02

that's the case because there's other

2:38:04

lines of evidence that argue against

2:38:06

that, but there may be something

2:38:08

about the procedure itself that is...

2:38:10

That increases muscle repair and improves

2:38:12

cardiac function? It just seems to

2:38:14

me that... I agree. I'm just

2:38:16

saying that may be an alternative

2:38:18

explanation for something that's limiting in

2:38:20

those mouse experiments. Just seems like

2:38:22

there's not enough time and not

2:38:24

enough money to do the work.

2:38:26

Hopefully some of that's changing. If

2:38:28

we were to do another longevity

2:38:30

roundtable next year, which is problematic

2:38:32

because this table, you guys are

2:38:34

going to have to get awfully

2:38:36

cozy, any nominations for folks you'd

2:38:38

want to invite to a longevity

2:38:40

roundtable next time? There's so many

2:38:42

people we could do this with,

2:38:44

right? And I'm guessing nobody wants

2:38:46

to give their seat up next

2:38:48

year. Let's make this table bigger.

2:38:50

I think it would be good

2:38:52

to invite Vadym Gladyship. I disagree

2:38:54

with some of what he says.

2:38:56

I think he always has something

2:38:58

interesting to say. Who's your nominee?

2:39:00

I've made some more time to

2:39:02

think about it. All right. Matt,

2:39:04

anybody? I mean, I think we

2:39:06

would all agree there are tons

2:39:08

of great people in the field.

2:39:10

I mean, I think Brian Kennedy,

2:39:12

and I think Brian is going

2:39:14

to be on your podcast in

2:39:16

an upcoming date, is somebody who

2:39:18

also thinks broadly and deeply about

2:39:20

the science and is fantastic. So

2:39:22

he would be great to have

2:39:24

great to have some differing. I

2:39:26

mean we differ. sometimes on opinions,

2:39:28

but I think more or less

2:39:30

are aligned. Be interesting to have

2:39:33

some different voices as well. All

2:39:35

right. So we think we'll do

2:39:37

another longevity roundtable around the oval

2:39:39

table? Sure. Let's do it. Let's

2:39:41

see where we are a year

2:39:43

from now. I think there's going

2:39:45

to be a lot of new

2:39:47

stuff. That's what's new in aging

2:39:49

research. Rate of progress. The derivative

2:39:51

is... Very much positive. You know

2:39:53

who else I want to throw

2:39:55

out there is Morgan Levine? I

2:39:57

think she'd be really interesting to

2:39:59

have because she while she is

2:40:01

an expert in epigenetics and biomarkers

2:40:03

I think takes a pretty clear-eyed

2:40:05

view of that space. Now is

2:40:07

Morgan at Yale still? She's at

2:40:09

Altus. She's at Altus. Yeah, okay,

2:40:11

I wasn't sure if she was

2:40:13

there full-time. Got it. Yeah, I

2:40:15

would second that. That's an excellent

2:40:17

idea. All right. Well, Rich, you

2:40:19

can get back to me on

2:40:21

your nominees as well. I will

2:40:23

definitely do that. All right, gentlemen.

2:40:25

Thank you for making the committee

2:40:27

on to this. Thank you. Thank

2:40:29

you for listening to this week's

2:40:31

episode of The Drive. Head over

2:40:33

to Peter atiamd.com/show notes if you

2:40:35

want to dig deeper into this

2:40:37

episode. You can also find me

2:40:39

on YouTube, Instagram, and Twitter, all

2:40:41

with the handle Peter Atia MD.

2:40:43

You can also leave us review

2:40:45

on Apple podcasts or whatever podcast

2:40:47

player you use. This podcast is

2:40:49

for general informational purposes only and

2:40:51

does not constitute the practice of

2:40:53

medicine, nursing, or other professional health

2:40:55

care services, including the giving of

2:40:57

medical advice. No doctor-patient relationship is

2:40:59

formed. The use of this information

2:41:01

and the materials linked to this

2:41:03

podcast is at the user's own

2:41:05

risk. The content on this podcast

2:41:07

is not intended to be a

2:41:09

substitute for professional medical advice, diagnosis,

2:41:11

or treatment. users should not disregard

2:41:13

or delay in obtaining medical advice

2:41:15

from any medical condition they have,

2:41:17

and they should seek the assistance

2:41:19

of their health care professionals for

2:41:21

any such conditions. Finally, I take

2:41:23

all conflicts of interest very seriously.

2:41:25

For all of my disclosures and

2:41:27

the companies I invest in or

2:41:29

advise, please visit Peter atiamd.com/about where

2:41:31

I keep an up-to-date and active

2:41:33

list of all disclosures.

Rate

Join Podchaser to...

  • Rate podcasts and episodes
  • Follow podcasts and creators
  • Create podcast and episode lists
  • & much more

Episode Tags

Do you host or manage this podcast?
Claim and edit this page to your liking.
,

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features