Episode Transcript
Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.
Use Ctrl + F to search
0:09
Hey everyone, welcome to the Drive
0:12
podcast. I'm your host Peter Atia. This
0:14
podcast, my website, and my weekly newsletter
0:16
all focus on the goal of translating
0:18
the science of longevity into something accessible
0:21
for everyone. Our goal is to provide
0:23
the best content in health and wellness
0:25
and we've established a great team of
0:28
analysts to make this happen. It is
0:30
extremely important to me to provide all
0:32
of this content without relying on paid
0:34
ads. To do this, our work is
0:37
made entirely possible by our members, and
0:39
in return, we offer exclusive, member-only
0:41
content and benefits above and beyond
0:43
what is available for free. If
0:45
you want to take your knowledge
0:47
of this space to the next
0:49
level, it's our goal to ensure
0:52
members get back much more than
0:54
the price of the subscription. If
0:56
you want to learn more about
0:58
the benefits of our premium membership,
1:00
head over to Peter atiamd.com forward
1:02
slash subscribe. Welcome to
1:05
a special episode of The
1:07
Drive. Today we're introducing a
1:09
new format to the podcast.
1:11
It's our inaugural roundtable conversation.
1:14
For this one, we have
1:16
gathered three brilliant minds, all
1:18
former guests of the podcast,
1:21
to sit down and have
1:23
a... nuanced, funny, sometimes a
1:25
little heated discussion about one
1:27
of the most fascinating and
1:29
rapidly evolving areas of medicine
1:31
today. Geoscience, also known, I
1:34
guess, as longevity science. So
1:36
joining me for this episode,
1:38
are doctors Steve Osted, an
1:40
expert in aging biology and
1:42
author of groundbreaking research on
1:44
extending health span. Richard Miller,
1:47
pioneer of the study of
1:49
anti-aging interventions, through the interventions
1:51
testing program, or ITP, which
1:53
you hear me reference a
1:55
lot, and Matt Caberlin, whose
1:57
expertise explores the intersection of
1:59
genetics, aging, and translational research, and
2:01
Matt, of course, is famous for
2:03
his work in the Dog Aging
2:05
Project. So in today's roundtable,
2:07
we discuss a number of things,
2:09
such as the relationship between health
2:12
span and lifespan. And what does
2:14
health span actually mean? Is it
2:16
something we should try to define?
2:18
Can you improve one without improving
2:20
the other? What has caused a surge
2:22
in the public interest in longevity science
2:25
and what major barriers are preventing longevity
2:27
research from reaching its full potential? This
2:29
actually was one of my favorite parts
2:32
of the discussion. How do we evaluate
2:34
the effectiveness of interventions like rapomycin,
2:36
synalytics, or calorie restriction in humans,
2:38
where it's very difficult to study
2:41
them for obvious reasons? Are there reliable biomarkers
2:43
or aging rate indicators that can measure
2:45
biologic aging, which of course is a
2:47
very hot topic? What role do
2:49
epigenetic changes play in aging?
2:51
Specifically, are they causal? Are
2:53
senescent cells a valid target
2:55
for longevity interventions? Or has
2:57
their role in aging been
2:59
overstated? Are GLP1 receptor agonists,
3:02
for example, drugs like
3:04
terzepotide and semaglutide, potentially
3:06
geroprotective beyond just their
3:08
weight loss effects? How
3:10
do we overcome the funding
3:13
and political challenges that prioritize
3:15
disease-specific research over foundational aging
3:18
science? What would it take to
3:20
make longevity research more mainstream and
3:22
gain broader support from the public
3:24
and policy makers? Anyway, this is
3:26
a new format, this idea of
3:29
doing a roundtable, so we really
3:31
want to hear from you. Is
3:33
it something you like? If so,
3:35
what are other topics you would
3:37
like to see for roundtables? So,
3:39
without further delay, please enjoy this
3:42
roundtable discussion with Steve Osted,
3:44
Rich Miller, and Matt Kaverlin.
3:46
This is a lot of fun. I am
3:48
excited to be sitting down with you guys
3:50
today. Where do we want to begin? Let
3:53
me start by saying the following. The
3:55
term longevity, someone sent me something the
3:57
other day that was like list of,
3:59
I don't know. whether it was how many
4:01
times the word longevity was searched on
4:03
Google or something like that, but it
4:05
literally looks like Bitcoin. So we are
4:07
clearly at peak longevity in terms of
4:09
public interest, which for all of you
4:12
who have kind of devoted decades plural to
4:14
this, I just want to kind of get
4:16
a reaction from you, each of you, on
4:18
what that means, why you think it's happening
4:20
and maybe even extending the metaphor a little
4:22
bit. Is there a bubble going on? We'll
4:25
start with you Steve. It's a surprise to me
4:27
that longevity has become so big because for
4:29
a long time we try to move away
4:31
from that in the aging field because we
4:33
were worried that people were thinking of
4:35
longevity as well. We're going to keep
4:38
frail, feeble, old people alive longer. That's
4:40
what longevity meant. When really what we're
4:42
trying to do is extend health. So I'm
4:44
kind of surprised, but I think it's
4:46
because there are certain people of a
4:49
certain age who've started to think about
4:51
their own longevity. And then I think
4:53
there's a whole new generation of tech
4:55
entrepreneurs that really feel like this is
4:58
a problem that will allow them to
5:00
live healthily for several decades at
5:02
least longer than they are now.
5:04
So I think it's a combination.
5:06
It's a multi-generational thing. That kind
5:08
of surprises me. And you haven't seen
5:10
this before to be clear. So 30
5:12
years ago you didn't see glimmers of
5:14
this? No, 30 years ago I would
5:17
have said, let's not even say the
5:19
word longevity. Let's say health span. That's
5:21
changed quite clearly as more and more
5:23
people have been from the outside. They're
5:25
sort of peaking in at the field.
5:27
I don't think the people in the
5:29
field itself have changed the way they
5:31
talk that much, but the people eavesdropping
5:33
on the field certainly have. Rich, is
5:35
that your experience? Well, I think there
5:38
are two aspects that I would want
5:40
to emphasize in response to your question, and
5:42
to what Steve said. People have always
5:44
been fascinated for millennia on things they
5:46
could do to stay alive and healthy
5:49
as long as possible. But there were
5:51
actually scientific discoveries in the 90s
5:53
that showed that it could be done.
5:55
And then in the last 20 years there's
5:57
evidence that it can be done at
5:59
least. in mice with pills. So that
6:01
naturally should lead to
6:04
speculation that there could be pills
6:06
you could give to people that
6:08
would postpone poor health for
6:10
a substantial amount of time.
6:12
20 to 30% is what we're seeing
6:15
in mice and 20 to 30% would
6:17
be very important for people. So
6:19
I think that is a part of it.
6:21
The other part is that... They're
6:23
now people who are making a lot of
6:26
money by selling stuff that is untested to
6:28
be polite about it or is useless to
6:30
be less polite about it to gullible
6:32
customers. And so people who want
6:34
to make a lot of money
6:36
have finally found that there's an
6:38
impetus that will allow them to
6:40
sell stuff even if there's no
6:42
evidence that it works, that they
6:44
control an enormous amount of advertising
6:46
dollars, both formal and informal. That's
6:48
a big part of the difference.
6:50
The one comment I wanted to make
6:53
with regard to something Steve said
6:55
has to do with the alleged
6:57
balance between health span and
6:59
life span. It's become fashionable
7:01
for the last 20 or 30 years
7:03
to imagine that you get one or
7:05
the other, that you have to make
7:07
a choice. It's a decision, and that
7:09
if you give up on life span,
7:11
that allows you to extend health
7:14
span. I think that's ridiculous
7:16
and controverted by all
7:19
the available evidence. That is, all
7:21
of the drugs at least that extend
7:23
lifespan in mice and could potentially
7:25
do so in people, do so
7:27
by postponing diseases. Both the diseases
7:29
that will kill you, that's why
7:31
they extend lifespan, and the diseases
7:33
that won't kill you, but which
7:35
will annoy you, and make you
7:37
very unhappy to be old. Which
7:39
is true, by the way, of
7:41
non-molecular tools as well. Yes, absolutely.
7:43
Yeah, absolutely. That's a good point.
7:45
Not being insulin resistant. I agree
7:47
with you. So the notion it's time to put
7:49
behind us and to make fun of the
7:51
notion that I'm not interested in
7:54
lifespan, don't put me on that boat.
7:56
I am interested in health spend because
7:58
they are linked together. and they
8:00
go up and down together, getting
8:02
people disabused of that false metaphor,
8:05
the seesaw metaphor is
8:07
probably an important goal
8:09
for the public interface
8:11
between longevity scientists, aging
8:13
scientists. Now I just want to
8:15
push on one thing though, you
8:17
talked about obviously the discoveries of
8:19
molecules, you've been personally
8:21
central to that work, but there
8:23
was still a lag, rich. I mean it was...
8:26
15 years ago, the first
8:28
ITP was published showing the
8:30
overwhelmingly surprising and positive results
8:33
of rapomycin. Those results were
8:35
repeated. Why a decade, let's be
8:37
generous and charitable and call it
8:39
a still decade-long lag from that.
8:41
And by the way, I'll throw
8:43
one more thing in there. If
8:45
you go back to Cynthia Kenyon's
8:47
work, which may have been... the
8:49
thin end of the wedge into
8:51
the idea that lifespan was malleable,
8:54
albeit through a genetic manipulation in
8:56
a less relevant model. Yeah. There's
8:58
still a lag. Do you buy
9:00
Steve's argument that it's a confluence
9:02
of technology, tech entrepreneurs? Let
9:04
me answer your question first.
9:06
Why the lag? I think there's
9:08
a whole batch of reasons and
9:10
they're important and they're easy to spell
9:12
out. One is the prevailing attitude is
9:14
that aging is there. There's nothing
9:16
you can do about it. I'm gonna... not
9:19
be able to outwit aiding, though I may
9:21
be able to be maybe healthier in
9:23
my older years. The notion that aging
9:25
is not malleable, though wrong and
9:28
provably wrong, is still
9:30
the overwhelming opinion even
9:32
of reasonably educated scientists
9:34
and certainly of the lay public.
9:36
Then commercially, there are companies
9:38
that make a ton of money selling
9:41
stuff that doesn't work by pretending with
9:43
a wink and a nod and a
9:45
lawyer that it might... slow the aging
9:47
process down. And since they can make
9:50
a lot of money, they don't actually
9:52
have to spend valuable marketing
9:54
dollars on doing research and
9:56
stuff to prove that it works.
9:58
Some of the drugs... that at least
10:00
in the hands of our mouse
10:03
group, the ITP interventions testing program,
10:05
some of the drugs are the patent
10:07
is owned by another company or they're
10:09
out of patent or it's a natural
10:11
product, none of that says take me
10:13
to whoever owns a big pharmaceutical firm.
10:16
And also even if you do it right
10:18
and you really want to do it and you've
10:20
got a very large budget, it's not
10:22
an overnight kind of thing. Any one
10:24
drug, a leading agent that like rapidism
10:26
which you mentioned in the... half a
10:28
dozen others that we've shown work, at
10:31
least in mice, finding something in
10:33
that same family that works really
10:35
well, that is safe for people, that's
10:37
the member of the 20 congeners
10:39
of that drug that's best and
10:41
most potent and safest, that's not
10:44
at all trivial. That takes a
10:46
long time and it takes a
10:48
commitment of money and time and
10:50
effort and intellectual resources, where the
10:52
place where we can start to make
10:55
an argument that that's a good idea.
10:57
But make a good argument that that's
10:59
a good idea to people who
11:01
actually have the resources to carry
11:03
it out as not so far
11:05
been enormously successful, unfortunately. Can I
11:08
push back a little on
11:10
what Rich said about health
11:12
span versus lifespan? Several papers
11:14
have come out recently showing that
11:16
the gap between health span and
11:18
lifespan in people is actually
11:20
increasing, and it's increasing faster
11:22
among women than men. So in humans,
11:25
this is a very real gap, and it's
11:27
a growing gap, and I think one of
11:29
the advantages of the kind of geroscience, the
11:31
stuff that we do, is that Rich is
11:33
right. We don't see this in our experimental
11:36
systems. So this to me emphasizes the
11:38
fact that we need to change the focus.
11:40
I think one of the reasons that the
11:42
gap exists is we're getting better and better
11:44
and better and better at treating heart
11:46
disease and cancer and all these things
11:48
and keeping people alive. when they wouldn't have
11:51
been alive 10 years ago. But this is
11:53
a really important factor, I think, about thinking
11:55
of public health globally. But I think you're
11:57
both right. I think you're looking at it
11:59
from different... So Steve, you're pointing out
12:01
that you can make people live longer when
12:03
they're sick. I think what Rich is
12:05
saying, which I agree with, hopefully I'm
12:07
going to paraphrase you correctly, which is if
12:09
we target the biology of aging, I
12:11
haven't seen anything to make me believe
12:14
that you can separate health span and
12:16
lifespan, meaning that I haven't seen things
12:18
that slow aging, increase lifespan, don't increase
12:20
health span. I don't actually think that's
12:22
plausible. And I think that's an important
12:24
point, that if we're doing something different.
12:27
then with the way that medicine is
12:29
operating now, which is targeting individual diseases,
12:31
after they occur. This is a very
12:33
important point. It came up in a
12:35
recent podcast that I did with Sam
12:38
Sutaria, talking about health care costs. And
12:40
in that discussion, one of the
12:42
things that emerged, which I think most
12:44
people are sadly familiar with this statistic
12:46
today, is that among the OECD nations,
12:48
the United States has the lowest
12:50
life expectancy. which is ironic given that
12:52
we are spending on average about 80%
12:55
more in some cases double what most
12:57
other developed nations spend on health care.
12:59
So how do you reconcile this?
13:01
Well some made a very interesting
13:03
point which is that's aggregate life
13:06
expectancy, but why is that the case?
13:08
That's because the United States has by
13:10
far the greatest rate of death in
13:12
middle age. So when you look at
13:14
maternal and infant mortality, we're horrible.
13:17
When you look at gun violence...
13:19
and suicide and homicide were horrible,
13:21
and most of all when you
13:23
look at overdoses were horrible. When
13:25
you kill a whole bunch of people
13:27
in their 40s and 50s, you cannot
13:29
have a very high life expectancy. Understood.
13:32
But what Sam pointed out was, once
13:34
an American reaches the age of, and I
13:36
forget the exact age, I think it was
13:38
about 65, all of a sudden they jumped to
13:40
the top of the list. That was
13:42
very interesting to me. In other words,
13:45
if you look at the blended... life expectancy,
13:47
we're not doing very well. But if you
13:49
look at life expectancy, just inmeasured as years
13:51
alive, once you escape those big causes of
13:53
death in middle age, we actually do quite
13:56
well. And it comes down to what you're
13:58
saying, which is we get very... good at
14:00
delaying death in chronic disease. That's what
14:02
I call the Medicine 2.0 machine at
14:05
its absolute finest. We are going to
14:07
keep you along an extra six months
14:09
once you have cancer. We are going
14:11
to get you through that third revascularization
14:14
procedure. And so now the question is,
14:16
because my intuition is where yours is
14:18
Steve, I don't think we're getting any
14:20
healthier. Even if we're incrementally figuring out
14:23
ways to extend life in the face
14:25
of chronic disease, I don't see it
14:27
being a quality of life. Part of
14:29
this might be, how do we define
14:31
health span? Yeah. I agree with you.
14:34
And I think it's even worse, though,
14:36
than the way you laid it out.
14:38
So if you look at the statistics,
14:40
if you accept that 60% of Americans
14:43
have at least one chronic disease, and
14:45
the median age in the United States,
14:47
and the 60% of Americans have at
14:49
least one chronic disease, and the median
14:52
age in the definition of health span,
14:54
I would not define health span as
14:56
ending once you have your first chronic
14:58
disease, but that's... The definition most people
15:01
would use, if you use that definition,
15:03
most people are spending three decades or
15:05
more in the absence of health span,
15:07
or in six span. So the situation
15:10
is, even in the United States, where
15:12
life expectancy is relatively short compared to
15:14
other nations, a big chunk of that
15:16
life expectancy is not spent in good
15:19
health. And it's exactly for this reason.
15:21
But the two different issues that are
15:23
being confused here in the discussion... One
15:25
is the issue of whether you can
15:28
help middle-aged people live longer. And everybody's
15:30
agreed that we're getting better at that,
15:32
we're pretty good at it. And that
15:34
certainly contributes to whatever you think health
15:37
span might mean. That's an issue, however,
15:39
that is quite different from a concoction
15:41
that slows aging, do so by extending
15:43
health span. Those both have the word
15:45
health span in them. But they're different
15:48
and shouldn't ever be confused with one
15:50
another. The other point in this question
15:52
you asked us what is... health span.
15:54
My own personal answer to that is
15:57
it's a useless term. That is because
15:59
no one can define it. It's not
16:01
because no one is smart, it's because
16:03
the term itself is vacuous and nebulous.
16:06
If you have somebody that gets a
16:08
certain chronic disease here and then another
16:10
one and then they fall down and
16:12
bump their head and by the way
16:15
they go to the hospital and with
16:17
COVID etc. etc. etc. defining when in
16:19
that 20 to 30 year period They
16:21
flicked the switch, now they have gotten
16:24
to the end of the health spend,
16:26
is impossible. And of no interest. The
16:28
general notion that people are interested in
16:30
is whether you can do stuff to
16:33
keep people healthy for a long time,
16:35
either without changing their life expectancy or
16:37
by changing their life. Those are interesting,
16:39
but you don't have to assign a
16:42
number, a health spend digit. I don't
16:44
like the medical definition of health span,
16:46
which I believe is, quote, the period
16:48
of time in which an individual is
16:51
free of disability and disease. I find
16:53
that to be a very unhelpful definition.
16:55
Yeah. But part of the reason it's
16:57
awful is it's binary. Yeah, exactly. You
16:59
got it. But if we made it
17:02
analog instead of digital, I'm not saying
17:04
that makes it easy. It's still very
17:06
challenging. But now it allows us to
17:08
start talking about things. Except it's a
17:11
concept. It's a qualitative. to where we
17:13
can actually come up with a way
17:15
to measure, whether we call it health
17:17
span or not, that doesn't really matter.
17:20
I kind of agree with Rich, like
17:22
I agree with what you're saying, except
17:24
I think it's a really useful term
17:26
as a concept. I think it's a
17:29
really useful way to communicate to a
17:31
broader audience what one of the goals
17:33
is, which is to increase the healthy
17:35
period of life. I kind of like
17:38
the term health for health. And you
17:40
don't have to pretend you can define
17:42
it as a number. But I think
17:44
we all could agree. There's a period
17:47
of life where you are in relatively
17:49
good health. And then there's a period
17:51
of life where you aren't. And so
17:53
I think the idea that we're trying
17:56
to increase that component of life is
17:58
really important. So I don't think we're
18:00
actually. disagreeing on much other than whether
18:02
we like the word. Well I also
18:05
think there's an individualization of this that
18:07
we're missing. To me health is a
18:09
state of your physical being that you
18:11
can do the things you like to
18:14
do. Therefore if you like to climb
18:16
mountains your health span is going to
18:18
be different than if you like to
18:20
play golf for instance and a lot
18:22
of this is personal. If you can't
18:25
run a marathon anymore some people will
18:27
say oh my health is... And we
18:29
never pay attention to the mental health
18:31
piece, at least the biologists. So I
18:34
have a question for you, Steve. What
18:36
is my health span? I would only
18:38
be able to ask you that. So
18:40
we do this exercise, guys, because I
18:43
completely agree with you, Steve. We call
18:45
it the marginal decade exercise. So we
18:47
say to every one of our patients,
18:49
and I write about this a lot
18:52
in the book, everyone will have a
18:54
marginal decade, which I define as the
18:56
last decade of your life. So obviously
18:58
by definition, everyone has a marginal decade.
19:01
Everyone has a marginal decade. But most
19:03
people have a pretty good sense when
19:05
they're in it. Okay, so the exercise
19:07
we do is we go through with
19:10
the patient and we say, what are
19:12
the things that are most important to
19:14
you to be able to do in
19:16
your marginal decade? And they generally fall
19:19
into three buckets with a sub bucket.
19:21
Physical, cognitive, emotional, social. The physical bucket
19:23
we kind of divide into activities of
19:25
daily living and recreational activities. So that's
19:28
where, again, most people obviously into it
19:30
into it that... Boy, I would really
19:32
not be happy if I couldn't take
19:34
care of myself. If I couldn't get
19:36
out of bed, get dressed, shave, cook,
19:39
that would be disappointing to me. But
19:41
then, of course, you have different levels
19:43
of ambition within the recreational side. I've
19:45
got patients who say, when the day
19:48
comes that I can't helly ski, I'm
19:50
going to be devastated. And other people
19:52
are like, I just want to be
19:54
able to garden. That's going to create
19:57
a very different standard. On the cognitive
19:59
side, you have people who say, I
20:01
want to be able to run my
20:03
hedge fund and still make money and
20:06
make really important investment decisions. And other
20:08
people are like, I want to be
20:10
able to do crossword puzzles and read
20:12
the newspaper. I agree with you. You
20:15
can't define it, but it doesn't mean...
20:17
we shouldn't try to personalize it. Okay,
20:19
but I want to come back to
20:21
you Matt with the original question. Why
20:24
are we at a point where? Why
20:26
is longevity gone mainstream? Yeah, for a
20:28
lot better way. Yeah, so I mean
20:30
I think both of the points that
20:33
Stephen Rich raised are part of the
20:35
equation. I mean I think it's a
20:37
convergence of all of these factors and
20:39
maybe a few others. I do think
20:42
the science has matured to the point
20:44
where more people are believing that we
20:46
can actually modulate. the biology of aging.
20:48
I think the concept of biological aging
20:50
has become popularized through a variety of
20:53
mechanisms, including some influencers, individuals who I
20:55
personally think often err on the side
20:57
of being a little bit less scientific
20:59
than they should be, but I think
21:02
they've helped popularize the concept. So I
21:04
think it's been a combination of these
21:06
factors. And why it has taken so
21:08
long? I mean, I just think that's
21:11
the pace that science moves and the
21:13
rate at which these concepts can sort
21:15
of permeate the public... So it's frustrating
21:17
in a sense that it's moved so
21:20
slowly. I also wonder, because you sort
21:22
of said, are we at a longevity
21:24
bubble? I don't know. I think maybe
21:26
we're still kind of in the early
21:29
days of this hockey stick moment, where
21:31
you're getting this exponential increase in attention.
21:33
My hope is, as we go forward,
21:35
it will become more scientific and less
21:38
snake oily, and it's a spectrum. There's
21:40
this huge gray area in the field
21:42
right now of what's real and what's
21:44
not real, and I think none of
21:47
us at this at this table, actually.
21:49
can really define exactly where in that
21:51
gray area that line is or is
21:53
there a line? To that point, Matt,
21:56
what is the collective wisdom of the
21:58
group on the funding appetite for that?
22:00
Because I agree with you completely, like
22:02
if we could channel this exuberance away
22:05
from kind of the highly commercial speculative
22:07
grifting towards the budget increasing legitimate investigative,
22:09
that would be awesome. What is the
22:11
appetite right now of NIA with respect?
22:13
back to this? I think it's hard
22:16
to say it. I mean, NIH is
22:18
a moving target, and as we all
22:20
know, there's going to be a lot
22:22
of change coming in the near future.
22:25
So, cautiously optimistic, I would say, if
22:27
you look historically, it's been really pretty
22:29
terrible. The percent of NIH budget that
22:31
goes to biology of aging, I think
22:34
is still probably around half of 1%.
22:36
Sorry, just to put numbers in perspective,
22:38
fraction that goes to biology of aging.
22:40
Yes, yes, yes, yes. Yeah. But I'm
22:43
saying there are 17 groups of NIH.
22:45
NIA being one of them gets what
22:47
fraction of NIAH budget roughly? I think
22:49
it's roughly 3%. 3% of NIA budget
22:52
is NIA. Within NIA, how much goes
22:54
to this type of research? It was
22:56
about 350 million a few years ago.
22:58
It might be a little higher than
23:01
that, but I don't think it's ticked
23:03
up any more proportional to the increase
23:05
in the increase in NIA budget since
23:07
then since then. What's your level of
23:10
optimism, Rich? You're obviously very close to
23:12
this. That NIH will wake up and
23:14
start to pay attention to aging research
23:16
the way they should? It's near zero.
23:19
It's been near zero for 30 years
23:21
now. Even with this outside attention? Well,
23:23
it's gone up. I mean, they funded
23:25
the ITP, the interventions testing program, 20
23:27
years ago, and they liked it, and
23:30
they doubled our budget about 15 years
23:32
ago. So that's something. And I'm very,
23:34
very grateful to them for that, but
23:36
there's still an enormous untapped potential for
23:39
making progress in the basic biology of
23:41
aging. And the reason is, again, a
23:43
matter of defending turf. If you are
23:45
a cardiologist researcher or an oncologist researcher
23:48
or an AIDS researcher or an Alzheimer's
23:50
researcher, any time somebody says, the smart
23:52
play is to reduce your budget by
23:54
10 percent or your institute's budget by
23:57
10 percent. We're going to go there
23:59
faster if we spend money on aging
24:01
and its relationship to the disease you
24:03
care about. You get the porcupine defense.
24:06
You don't take any of my money
24:08
because Alzheimer's is important. little kids with
24:10
leukemia are important, breast cancer is important,
24:12
you go away. And that is the
24:15
predominant feeling. Most of the people making
24:17
those decisions were not trained in aging
24:19
research, they view it as something interesting,
24:21
I read something about that in Time
24:24
Magazine the other day, but they don't
24:26
understand that to actually conquer or slow
24:28
down or effect or protect against the
24:30
disease they care about, the smart play
24:33
is to do aging research. And so
24:35
they view your suggestion, which I of
24:37
course agree with 100% as an imposition,
24:39
an invasion to be repelled at any
24:41
cost. No one in a position of
24:44
power has had whatever it takes to
24:46
reverse that. And if he or she
24:48
tried to do that, Congress would even
24:50
a good Congress would smack them down.
24:53
The Alzheimer's group has 100 lobbyists, the
24:55
cancer group has 100 lobbyists, the AIDS
24:57
group has 100 lobbyists. The aging group
24:59
has two lobbyists, one who's a lawyer
25:02
and one who takes the calls, and
25:04
that's not enough to do it. Can
25:06
I just add something real quick? I
25:08
agree completely. And I think, as well,
25:11
the reputation of the field has hindered
25:13
that transition as well. So historically, the
25:15
field was viewed as not very mechanistic,
25:17
kind of phenomenological, became much more mechanistic,
25:20
starting around the time of Cynthia Canyon's
25:22
work, and since then. but has continued
25:24
to have a reputation problem as not
25:26
being as rigorous as other areas of
25:29
research. So I think it is absolutely
25:31
a turf for, and there's this overcoming
25:33
the reputational problem which makes it harder
25:35
for serious people in funding and policy
25:38
circles to give it the attention it
25:40
deserves, in my opinion. So I've got
25:42
a different take on this. I actually
25:44
think that this is a very good
25:47
time for aging research funding, and that's
25:49
not because of what's going on at
25:51
the NIA. but it's what's going on
25:53
in the private sector. There's more and
25:55
more money. There's even interest now in
25:58
big pharma that was very spotty in
26:00
the past. So I think if We
26:02
focused entirely on the Nationalist and on
26:04
aging. We would get a false impression
26:07
of what the funding climate is in
26:09
the field now. And I think we
26:11
need to take advantage of that. Got
26:13
to make sure that it doesn't get
26:16
captured by the people who are doing
26:18
the fleshy, but bad science. You're saying,
26:20
look, Calico, Altos, other private companies, especially
26:22
within biotech and pharma that are looking
26:25
at geoprot protective molecules building on the
26:27
work of the ITP. Yeah, I think
26:29
it's safe to say the amount of
26:31
money that's being spent privately probably out
26:34
does public spending, I mean in a
26:36
given year two to one easily. It
26:38
could, although how much of that is
26:40
actually going to biology of age? I
26:43
think it's still an open question. You
26:45
mentioned Calico and Alton's right? Exactly. I
26:47
actually agree with Steve. I don't think
26:49
what Rich and I were communicating as
26:52
opposed to what Steve was communicating. There
26:54
are a lot of this hockey stick
26:56
moment. And I think Steve's right, there
26:58
are real opportunities for more resources to
27:01
be focused on the scientific side and
27:03
hopefully less focused on the non-scientific aspects
27:05
of what are going on. And you
27:07
asked the question of can we shift
27:10
resources from the more consumer-facing, maybe not
27:12
as rigorous stuff, and into the more
27:14
rigorous stuff. I'm not a fan of
27:16
that stuff at all. But maybe you
27:18
need that stuff to kind of move
27:21
the needle and get people's attention, and
27:23
at least people are talking about longevity
27:25
now. Now you've questioned I'm embarrassed I
27:27
don't know the answer because I spent
27:30
more than two years working there What's
27:32
the mission statement of the NIH? It's
27:34
to preserve and enhance human health I
27:36
mean it's basically the same thing that
27:39
we do that we're supposed to enhance
27:41
human health I mean it's basically the
27:43
same thing that we do that we're
27:45
supposed to be doing Yeah and I
27:48
didn't actually get to give you my
27:50
spiel here but what I started to
27:52
say about the NIA budget is if
27:54
you look at the United States Nine
27:57
of them have biological aging as their
27:59
greatest risk. factor and it's not even
28:01
close. Yet half of one percent of
28:03
the research budget that's supposed to
28:05
be focused on improving human health
28:07
goes to study that risk factor. I
28:10
mean I think it is extremely frustrating to
28:12
all of us sitting at this
28:14
table that that hasn't changed but
28:16
there's reason to be optimistic that
28:19
maybe it will change in the near future.
28:21
Let's state that again because it is
28:23
so profound I want to make sure
28:25
not a single person missed that statement.
28:27
The top 10 causes of death
28:30
in the United States are
28:32
well enumerated and incredibly predictable,
28:35
and they increase by category,
28:37
by decade, 3 to 8
28:39
percent, monotonically, with no exception.
28:42
Point being, 90 percent of, and
28:44
more than 90 percent on an
28:46
adjusted basis of what causes death,
28:49
goes up with age. And yet, a
28:51
few basis points of federal
28:53
R&D goes to addressing that.
28:55
Let me give you an example of what
28:57
the sort of point that Matthew and
28:59
you have been making. About once every
29:01
five years, I give a talk and
29:04
invite a talk at the University of
29:06
Michigan Cancer Center, and I point out
29:08
that we have drugs now, anti-aging
29:10
drugs, in mice, and they extend mouse
29:12
lifespan, and they do it mostly
29:14
by postponing cancer, because most of
29:16
our mice die of cancer. And if you
29:19
look at age-adjusted cancer incidence rates,
29:21
our drugs reduce these by a
29:23
factor of 10. Wouldn't they like
29:25
to know why? As cancer scientists,
29:27
we now have a batch of
29:29
drugs that postpone cancer. Wouldn't they
29:31
like to study them? Invariably, I get
29:34
one call back from somebody who says,
29:36
that's interesting, maybe we should talk
29:38
about that, and then it dies, and
29:40
then five years later, I'm asked to give
29:42
the same talk or related talk. So they
29:45
know how to do cancer research, they are
29:47
cancer scientists, that's how they know how
29:49
to do cancer research, and you certainly
29:51
don't do it by... diverting your
29:54
lab's attention to aging. That's
29:56
insane, but that insanity is
29:58
how medical research is organized
30:00
and breaking that addiction to
30:02
the kinds of models you grew up on
30:04
because there are a better idea, it's not
30:07
an easy thing. It may not even be
30:09
a possible thing to do. That's a
30:11
major hassle. I think this is because
30:13
we think about health all wrong. We
30:15
think, let's wait to get cancer and
30:17
see what we can do about it.
30:19
That's what cancer biologists do. You have
30:21
cancer, okay, how can we better treat
30:24
that? Or could we have diagnosed it
30:26
earlier? What Rich is saying and what
30:28
we can know how to do in
30:30
lots of model organ, it prevents you
30:32
from getting cancer, delay it for a
30:34
considerable amount of time, that's a little
30:36
bit harder to study if you're a
30:38
cancer biologist, because you want to see
30:41
the cancer before you can study it.
30:43
I think that's why we need aging
30:45
biologists, rather than people focused on certain
30:47
disease to come and try to use
30:49
what we do. If we prevented the
30:51
cancers, they'd be out of a job.
30:53
people or mice will get cancer. They'll just
30:56
have 10 extra years of life if they're
30:58
a person or 10 extra months of life.
31:00
They'll get cancer. They'll need specialists. It'll be
31:02
all right. Yeah, I think that's important. I
31:04
mean, I think the reactive disease care component
31:06
is still going to be there. Even if we're
31:08
insanely successful at slowing agent, people are still
31:10
going to get sick. But I think Steve's
31:13
point is really important. Like Peter, you've been
31:15
a leader in helping people recognize the need
31:17
to shift the need to shift the medical.
31:19
approach from reactive to proactive. I
31:21
think what a lot of people
31:23
don't realize is that mentality goes
31:25
all the way back to pharmaceutical
31:28
research, biomedical research, basic science. That
31:30
is ingrained all the way through.
31:32
And I think one of the
31:34
challenges with getting funding for aging
31:37
research is that mentality on the
31:39
basic science world and how deeply
31:41
ingrained it is. It's very interesting
31:43
because you don't know which is the tail
31:45
and which is the dog. I've always assumed
31:47
that the one leading the charge
31:50
is the clinical side of things.
31:52
In other words, the engine, the
31:54
machine of Medicine 2.0, is built
31:56
around the delivery of care, the
31:58
delivery of care. as you said,
32:01
Steve, is built around, I'm going
32:03
to wait, I'm going to sit
32:05
here and hang, we're going to
32:07
wait, when you get the disease,
32:09
we're ready. You have the heart
32:12
attack, fantastic. You've got chest pain,
32:14
ST elevations, we've got a
32:16
stint for you, now you have
32:18
cancer, we're all in. And then
32:21
the research flows from that mindset.
32:23
Of course, I don't know, not
32:25
that it really matters, but it
32:27
might be that it's flipped. matters,
32:29
but if you could be health czar and
32:31
fix one of them, you might actually
32:34
start with the research side of things.
32:36
I would. And I mean, the reality
32:38
is the research flows from where the
32:40
dollars are going. This has been seen
32:42
over and over and over at NIH.
32:44
You shift resource allocation to a certain
32:46
area and the scientists will follow and
32:49
they will submit grants to get grants
32:51
in the place where the funding line
32:53
is the highest. So if somebody came
32:55
along and said we're going to go
32:57
to biology of aging. You'd have no
32:59
shortage of people. I mean, it would be
33:01
kind of messy at first, but you'd have
33:03
no shortage of people applying for grants and
33:05
becoming experts in the biology of aging. And
33:08
the system would work. You'd get the best
33:10
and the brightest that would go into that
33:12
and do that. So this then begs another question
33:14
that is a tired question, but I can't
33:16
help but ask it at this point. Is
33:18
aging a disease? Is that even a
33:21
relevant question? Call me, calling me, calling
33:23
me, calling me. It's important to use
33:25
words, to use words optimally, optimally, optimally.
33:27
and to distinguish causes
33:29
from effects. One of the
33:31
bad things about aging is it's
33:33
a risk factor for many diseases.
33:36
Some things are other risk factors
33:38
for diseases. Aging is a risk
33:40
factor for disease. And so saying
33:42
that aging is a disease confuses
33:45
that discussion. It makes
33:47
it impossible to see that
33:49
relationship. So calling aging a
33:51
disease is a fundamental
33:53
error. The question itself is
33:55
uncorrect. I agree. But I
33:57
think we have that idea.
34:00
for marketing purposes, not for
34:02
scientific purposes. And the
34:04
idea is, well, the money goes
34:06
to diseases, let's call aging a
34:08
disease, because I think what we're
34:10
trying to do is we're trying to
34:12
treat aging as we're trying to do,
34:15
is we're trying to treat aging as if
34:17
it were a disease, even though I would
34:19
agree with both of you. I don't think
34:21
it's a disease. I think that destroys
34:24
the word disease if we include aging
34:26
in it. But I think there was
34:28
a reason that you can... convince people
34:30
of the importance of aging research only
34:32
by crossing your fingers and saying, oh,
34:34
well, it's kind of a disease, isn't
34:37
it? You think you can fool them?
34:39
Yes, that's what marketing is, and it's
34:41
probably good for that. I just don't
34:43
like lying to people. It also creates
34:45
a negative feeling about the field in
34:48
some people as well. So I think
34:50
that should be considering. The other point
34:52
that people often raise, though, is we have
34:54
to call aging a disease in order for
34:56
FDA to approve a fundamental... misunderstanding
34:58
of how FDA operates, but
35:01
that is the other argument
35:03
you will often hear among
35:05
proponents of the idea that aging is
35:08
a disease. Very interesting. Well, so
35:10
now let's go one step deeper
35:12
on that. How do you
35:14
think about biologic versus chronologic
35:17
age? In concept and in practice.
35:19
On the right over here, Rich and
35:21
I were talking about that, I
35:23
don't believe there is one thing
35:25
as biological age. I think there is potentially
35:27
an age of your heart, an age
35:29
of your liver, an age of your
35:31
lungs, an age of your brain, but
35:34
I don't see why we wouldn't simply
35:36
call it health. In other words, I
35:38
got one of these epigenetic age clocks
35:40
done on me a while ago,
35:42
but I didn't know what to make
35:44
out of it. I thought, is this just
35:46
flattery? Or did it really tell me something?
35:48
You must have got a good result. I
35:51
got a good result. He's 13. 13 years
35:53
old. That may be the point of the
35:55
whole thing, right? So I'm dubious about some
35:58
number that is different than I know. good
36:00
health, for my age I'm in very
36:02
good health. So I knew that already.
36:04
Now I have a number for it.
36:06
I don't put much credence in that.
36:09
Let me agree with Steve, but just
36:11
put it in slightly different terminology. It's
36:13
a matter of taking a very rich,
36:15
complex data set and trying to collapse
36:17
it to a number. So if someone
36:20
wants to know how healthy I am,
36:22
he or she would need information. How
36:24
good is my eyesight, how good is
36:26
my hearing, how good is various kinds
36:28
of cognitive activities. my aerobic endurance, my
36:31
joints, all of that is pertinent to
36:33
how my health is, and also about
36:35
projected future health, then there's no need
36:37
once you've got that information, which is
36:40
very rich, to say, ah, there's a
36:42
number, a single number, a real number,
36:44
a point on the number line that
36:46
condenses that in any useful way. A
36:48
notion 40, 50 years ago that biological
36:51
age was not the same as chronological
36:53
age, for a little while was useful,
36:55
it emphasized that there might well be.
36:57
60-year-old people who were unusually like youthful
36:59
people, and 60-year-old people who were unusually
37:02
like 70-year-old people, would my drug or
37:04
my genetic mutant or whatever, help to
37:06
discriminate those people or change them in
37:08
some way, I can slow your biological
37:10
aging process. That's a discussion that was
37:13
maybe of interest 40 years ago, and
37:15
it's now time to drop the notion,
37:17
let alone the silly notion. that you
37:19
can count that biological age, that number
37:21
which some people, too many people, still
37:24
think is of value. You can figure
37:26
out what it is by measuring something,
37:28
transcriptions or epigenetic markers or something. I
37:30
can do it and give you personally,
37:33
your personal biological age. That's a waste
37:35
of everyone's time, and it also distracts
37:37
attention from things that actually are important
37:39
and need to be thought about. I
37:41
gotta talk, because... I think I disagree
37:44
fundamentally, and I'm surprised, but this will
37:46
be an interesting conversation. So I agree
37:48
that the idea of a kit, the
37:50
you can buy to measure biological age.
37:52
First of all, the stuff that's out
37:55
there doesn't work, and we can and
37:57
should talk about that. But also, I
37:59
sort of agree with the idea that
38:01
reducing it to one number, while conceptually,
38:03
I think it's possible, I think in
38:06
reality, is gonna be really, really difficult
38:08
to do. But do I believe that
38:10
there is a biological aging process that
38:12
is different from? chronological aging. Absolutely. Oh
38:15
yes, absolutely. Okay. Well, it sounded like
38:17
you guys were both saying, no, you
38:19
didn't think it was a real thing.
38:21
I agree with that completely. You can
38:23
agree with that and not like the
38:26
idea of a number of the constitutes
38:28
your biological age. Okay. There's two things
38:30
that kind of make me feel pretty
38:32
confident in this idea. One is, and
38:34
this is the example I use a
38:37
lot among the general public, is just
38:39
look at dogs compared to people. Everybody's
38:41
compared to people. means that dogs age
38:43
about seven times faster than people do.
38:45
But of course chronological time is the
38:48
same between dogs and people. It's the
38:50
biological aging process. And so you can
38:52
look across the animal kingdom and see
38:54
this. And dogs get almost all of
38:56
the same diseases and functional declines that
38:59
we do at the tissue and organ
39:01
level, but also the whole body level.
39:03
We also know now there are single
39:05
genes that significantly modulate. what I would
39:08
call the rate of aging. Now maybe
39:10
we have a different meaning to what
39:12
we mean. No, I agree. Okay. So
39:14
the fact that that's possible. Deftu, we
39:16
talked about Deftu a couple times, Tor,
39:19
we can turn these things up, turn
39:21
them down, and animals across the evolutionary
39:23
spectrum seem to age at different rates
39:25
by modulating single genes. So I don't
39:27
know of any other explanation other than
39:30
that there is this process, which we
39:32
call biological aging, that... can be changed
39:34
and the rate can be sped up
39:36
or slowed down. Can it be reversed?
39:38
That's an interesting question. Maybe we'll get
39:41
to that. But I think the process
39:43
is real. I think it's just really
39:45
really complicated and we probably only understand
39:47
5% of it at this point. Yeah,
39:49
I think for me that challenges, I
39:52
kind of land where Rich was, which
39:54
is if a patient says to me,
39:56
hey, why aren't you doing this biologic
39:58
age clock on me, my response is,
40:01
well, I know your VO2 Max, I
40:03
know your zone 2, I know your
40:05
muscle mass, I know your visceral fat,
40:07
we did a very complicated movement assessment
40:09
on you, I understand your balance, I
40:12
understand your lipids, your insulin, like I
40:14
know these 57 things about you and
40:16
I can tell you individually on each
40:18
of them how you're doing, that number...
40:20
doesn't tell me a single new piece
40:23
of information. But what if you were
40:25
to come up, and you probably do
40:27
this in your head, you come up
40:29
with some sort of composite, you probably
40:31
don't sit down and wait each of
40:34
those things and come to one number,
40:36
but you come up with some sort
40:38
of composite picture of health based on
40:40
all of those things, that's a different
40:42
biological aging clock. I think sometimes we
40:45
conflate and in part this because of
40:47
the way that irresponsible people in the
40:49
field and marketers have done this, we
40:51
conflate the epigenetic tests. with biological aging
40:54
clocks. There are all sorts of flavors
40:56
of biological aging clocks, including things like
40:58
frailty indices or metrics of a whole
41:00
bunch of functional markers. So I think
41:02
those probably are pretty good readouts of
41:05
biological age. Again, can you combine them
41:07
all to get to one number that's
41:09
meaningful for every person? That's much harder
41:11
to do. Yeah, tell us about your
41:13
experience, because this was, you did what
41:16
I wanted to do, but I've been
41:18
too lazy to do. coming up with
41:20
different names. So what I did was
41:22
I tested four different direct-to-consumer biological age
41:24
kits. They were all epigenetic biological age
41:27
tests before different companies, and I did
41:29
duplicates of each kit, and it was
41:31
from the same samples collected on the
41:33
same date. I really tried to put
41:35
my scientist hat on. I only had
41:38
two replicates, I didn't have three replicates,
41:40
but it's about the best I could
41:42
afford at that point. And it was
41:44
kind of expensive. And it was kind
41:47
of expensive. to me, very informative, fundamentally
41:49
sort of changed my views on these
41:51
epigenetic age tests. So they ranged. from
41:53
42 to 63, I was 53.75 years
41:55
at the time I did the test,
41:58
and the standard deviation, I can't remember,
42:00
it was either 7 or 9. So
42:02
mean if my chronological age, standard deviation
42:04
of 7 or 9, which I look
42:06
at that data, I'm not a statistician,
42:09
but I know enough statistics to say
42:11
that's completely useless. They converged on my
42:13
chronological age, but with a huge variation.
42:15
Even intro... So that varied between the...
42:17
tests. So I think three of the
42:20
four were reasonably close to each other.
42:22
Three of the four companies, the duplicates
42:24
were reasonably close to each other, but
42:26
the individual tests were far apart, and
42:28
one of the companies, the individual replicates,
42:31
was 20 years apart. So to me,
42:33
and some people will say, but maybe
42:35
the true diagnostic test is great, and
42:37
the Elysium test is terrible, or the
42:40
tally health test is terrible, and the
42:42
other one is great. Maybe, but how
42:44
do we know? My take home is
42:46
that the direct to consumer biological age...
42:48
testing industry is a complete mess. And
42:51
I have no idea who to believe
42:53
or if any of them are actually
42:55
giving accurate data. I know some of
42:57
the people at some of the companies
42:59
and I have my personal feelings about
43:02
who's trying to do it right and
43:04
who's sort of a charlatan, but across
43:06
the industry it's really hard to know.
43:08
The last thing I'll say on this
43:10
is the where I've sort of landed
43:13
is I think these are really good
43:15
research tools. I think the direct to
43:17
consumer component has gotten way ahead of
43:19
itself and I think I aligning with
43:22
what you were saying about the way
43:24
you think about these tests. I don't
43:26
think there's a lot of value in
43:28
clinical practice right now, because we don't
43:30
know precision or accuracy, and I don't
43:33
think you can make actionable recommendations based
43:35
on these tests. Furthermore, they fail in
43:37
the one thing that I think they're
43:39
attempting to do, and I usually use
43:41
this illustration with patients. So if I
43:44
have a 40-year-old patient who says I
43:46
really want to do one of these
43:48
tests, I say, if the answer comes
43:50
back and says you're 20, Is your
43:52
expectation that you will live another seven...
43:55
years? Conversely, if the answer comes back
43:57
and says 60, is it your expectation
43:59
that you will live another 30 years?
44:01
In other words, is this number predictive
44:03
of future years of life? Because right
44:06
now we have this thing called chronological
44:08
age that is the single best predictor
44:10
of future years of life. So do
44:12
we think biologic age, as determined by
44:15
these tests, is better as a predictor
44:17
of future years of life? Which, by
44:19
the way, would be very testable. How
44:21
many people have contacted you? to get
44:23
ITP sample data to say, can we
44:26
predict how much longer these mice we're
44:28
going to live? The answer to the
44:30
question is obvious and very well known.
44:32
You can tell if you have your
44:34
40-year-old patient and he or she is
44:37
fat, doesn't exercise, it's mostly cheeseburgers, you
44:39
know that their life expectancy is probably
44:41
not as good as a 40-year-old patient
44:43
in your next waiting room that has
44:45
extremely healthful habits and whose parents live
44:48
to be 100. Tons of public stuff.
44:50
I don't need a biologic age to
44:52
tell them. Right, that's what I'm saying.
44:54
There are tons of things you can
44:56
measure on individuals, four or five of
44:59
them, or all you really need to
45:01
ask of a 70-year-old. Yeah, MetLife does
45:03
this really, really, really, really, because their
45:05
buddies all, they'll lie there, they're writing
45:08
life insurance policies. So it's not at
45:10
all hard to figure out a very
45:12
small set of tests to tell you
45:14
how long a seven-year-year-year-year-old is likely to
45:16
live, is likely to live, there's likely
45:19
to live, there's nothing to do with.
45:21
methylation clocks or things like that. That's
45:23
the gold standard. When life insurance companies
45:25
start using biologic clocks as the cornerstone
45:27
of their actuarial algorithms, I'll start to
45:30
be far away from that. I'm going
45:32
to sound like a broken record here,
45:34
but you guys keep saying biological age
45:36
when what you mean is epigenetic age
45:38
or epigenetic tests. Not necessarily. And we
45:41
should explain to people that there is
45:43
a difference. So some of these clocks
45:45
use solely epigenetic measurements. Not all, most
45:47
of the direct-to-consumer ones are epigenetic. But
45:49
some of these tests use a litany
45:52
of biomarkers inclusive of epigenetics. So they'll
45:54
say, we've sampled your meth. pattern, but
45:56
we also looked at your vitamin D
45:58
level, your glucose level, your cholesterol level,
46:01
and a whole bunch of other things,
46:03
and we compressed all of that into
46:05
a number as well. So I guess,
46:07
let me frame it as a question
46:09
to you. So let's take the epigenetic
46:12
piece out. Again, I do think we
46:14
will get to a point where the
46:16
technology is developed far enough and the
46:18
quality control is good enough on the
46:20
consumer side that these tests will be
46:23
better than just chronological age. That's a
46:25
big statement. I don't know that I'm
46:27
disagreeing with you. I just want to
46:29
make sure you understand the state. I
46:31
mean, I think it's clear from the
46:34
research, unless you think that all of
46:36
the research that's been done on these
46:38
epigenetic aging clocks is somehow flawed. It's
46:40
clear that you can create algorithms that
46:42
can predict specific methylation patterns that are
46:45
more highly correlated with life expectancy than
46:47
chronological age. But I think the big
46:49
but here is that even if that's
46:51
the case, they would not be as
46:54
good as what Peter would predict after
46:56
all the Biological age. That's what I
46:58
want to get to. Yes, and I
47:00
think what you are actually doing is
47:02
looking at other biomarkers that have a
47:05
long-term clinical history that you're using to
47:07
come up with a surrogate, but really
47:09
is reflecting largely biological age. Maybe not
47:11
completely. And this is the other point
47:13
I wanted to make is I don't
47:16
think biological age and health are equal.
47:18
I think they're strongly overlapping. And certainly
47:20
you can identify many ways to reduce
47:22
health without accelerating biological aging. I think
47:24
that's easy. We can all think of
47:27
ways to do that. So let's take
47:29
a minute and try. Yeah, so let's
47:31
think about this for a second. I
47:33
have seen very impressive data where we
47:35
can look at tissue samples of organs
47:38
and we can tell, okay, I'm going
47:40
to show you a sample of nephrons.
47:42
And just based on nothing but the
47:44
methylation pattern, we know that if I
47:47
just said to you, one of these
47:49
is a 20 year old, one of
47:51
these is a 70 year old. It's
47:53
very easy to predict based on the
47:55
methylation pattern which Neffron came... from which
47:58
person. Completely agree with that. There are
48:00
a lot of things that change with
48:02
age. The literature has 25,000 things that
48:04
change with age. Average amount of methylation
48:06
at these 10 spots is number 11,4007
48:09
of those. So great, you've got another
48:11
thing that changes with age. So that's
48:13
the question. But that's not enough. Right.
48:15
So do you believe that all of
48:17
the research we're seeing on the epigenetic
48:20
clocks is going to be the 78th
48:22
variable that we would include in our
48:24
gestalt... I don't know. Yeah, it's a
48:26
good question. So I am hopeful that
48:29
epigenetic algorithms can get to the point
48:31
where they can replace many, certainly not
48:33
all, but many of the other biomarkers
48:35
that are being measured. I think the
48:37
thing that gives me hope is we
48:40
know that epigenetic changes are part of
48:42
biological aging. This again is a different
48:44
question, but if we look at the
48:46
hallmarks of aging, epigenetic dysregulation is one
48:48
of the 12. Some people will argue
48:51
it's the most important conversation, but it's
48:53
at least part. So that gives me
48:55
some hope that we are in fact
48:57
measuring something that plays a causal role
48:59
in the aging process. And I think
49:02
what's missing, I think what would give
49:04
all of us a lot more confidence
49:06
is if we had a mechanistic connection
49:08
to the specific methylation changes and some
49:10
cause of aging or age-related disease. In
49:13
other words, this change in methylation changes
49:15
this particular gene's expression level, which changes
49:17
the rate of biological aging. I think
49:19
if we had that, we'd feel a
49:22
lot more confident. Yeah, you and I
49:24
spoke about this very briefly at the
49:26
end of our last podcast, and I
49:28
want to come back to it with
49:30
all of us on this table, because
49:33
there's so much in what you just
49:35
said, Matt, that I'm going to lay
49:37
out a broad question, and then we
49:39
can start attacking it in different ways.
49:41
So one of the things I want
49:44
to address is do we believe that
49:46
it's possible that of the hallmarks of
49:48
aging, epigenetic change is the most important.
49:50
Another topic I want to address, do
49:52
we believe that the epigenetic changes that
49:55
we observe over time which are undeniable
49:57
are causal in the arrival of other
49:59
states? everything from the arrival of senescent
50:01
cells, the increase in inflammation, the reduced
50:03
function of the organs, which really is
50:06
the hallmark of aging. And if so,
50:08
does that mean that reversing the epigenetic
50:10
phenotype will undo the phenotype of interest?
50:12
And Rich, where I'm going, that you
50:15
and I left off was, what about
50:17
the proteome? What about the metabolism? So
50:19
you made three statements there. Broad general
50:21
statements. And I think each of the
50:23
three deserves careful... Amendment. Let's do it.
50:26
To be polite about it. The first
50:28
has to do with hommarks of aging,
50:30
which I think set the field back
50:32
dramatically. I think when you are officially
50:34
branded a homework of aging, by two
50:37
people sitting alone at their computers and
50:39
writing a review article, a homework of
50:41
aging... I thought they were walking around
50:43
a pond when they came up with
50:45
this. All right, okay, okay. Means that
50:48
somebody once said, I'm interested in aging,
50:50
that's kind of important, isn't it? Let's
50:52
put it on our list. You can't
50:54
tell if something is a hallmark of
50:56
aging. Does that mean it goes up
50:59
with age? It goes down with age.
51:01
You can change it in a way
51:03
that will extend lifespan. You can kill
51:05
a mouse or a worm by removing
51:08
it. Basically, it's something that somebody wants
51:10
to might be of interest to aging.
51:12
And the downside of that is once
51:14
you're officially branded as a hallmark of
51:16
aging, anyone who wants to write up
51:19
grant on that doesn't have to prove...
51:21
that they're a fundamental cause and effect
51:23
model has any merit. Because it's a
51:25
hallmark of aging. I don't have to
51:27
prove it anymore. Someone, I don't know
51:30
who or on what grounds, has decided
51:32
it's important. My reviewers know it's important
51:34
because they've read the hallmark of aging
51:36
paper, so I don't have to think
51:38
about whether it's important. The negative side
51:41
of that coin is that there are
51:43
lots of things that didn't make it
51:45
into the hallmark list. I really think
51:47
it's premature to close thought off on
51:49
some of those. It's easy to come
51:52
up with a dozen things that ought
51:54
to be investigated, but if you want
51:56
to investigate it, it's not on the
51:58
home marks list. What are you wasting?
52:01
So deciding which of the hallmarks is
52:03
the big daddy hallmark or whatever strikes
52:05
me is not the correct thing to
52:07
talk about in the hallmarks arena. So
52:09
maybe we should talk about that before
52:12
we go through all of these. I think
52:14
there's a lot to unpack there. You
52:16
guys could afford to give me a
52:18
little piece of paper and a pen.
52:20
I think the hallmarks is a list,
52:22
a kind of arbitrary list, not completely
52:24
arbitrary, because I had some reasons for
52:26
being there. I don't think any of
52:28
us would say that. those 12 things
52:30
are not involved in aging. But
52:32
that's a very little interest. Do any
52:34
of us want to rattle them up being
52:36
that I'm the only one that's got the
52:39
list sitting in front of me? We could
52:41
do a game where we each name one
52:43
and see who can't see if we get
52:45
to all 12. Yeah. But certainly in that
52:48
list, I would not consider epigenetics as the
52:50
key hallmark. Assuming there are such
52:52
things, I consider it to be
52:54
an interesting list. It became... biblically
52:57
sacrosan almost immediately and I've never understood
52:59
why but for some reason it did
53:01
so I'd agree with rich so conceptually
53:03
beautiful I mean so I agree completely
53:05
with rich and he knows I do
53:07
because we've talked about this before I
53:09
think the flip side is I think
53:11
the hallmarks have been immensely useful to
53:13
the field they are a very easy
53:15
way to communicate this idea of biological
53:17
aging and it helps convince some of
53:19
the scientific community that thought it was
53:21
all just hocus pocus pocus and snake
53:24
oil that there is some mechanistic research
53:26
happening. We can point to specific things
53:28
that are aging. So I think that
53:30
part of the hallmarks has been actually
53:32
really valuable and has contributed to the
53:34
popularization of longevity and at least to
53:36
the extent the science of longevity has
53:38
been popularized has contributed to
53:41
that. And it has been extremely detrimental to
53:43
the field and the way I think about
53:45
it is it just caused the field to
53:47
narrow prematurely. And this goes back to
53:49
what I alluded to before. I
53:51
don't know if we understand 80%
53:54
of biological aging or 0.205% of
53:56
biological aging. My guess is it's
53:58
closer to 0.205% and... And by and
54:00
large, the funding to look outside of
54:02
the hallmarks dried up once the hallmarks
54:05
became the dominant paradigm. And people stopped
54:07
looking. And I think we need to
54:09
go back to more discovery science and
54:11
thinking outside the box. So I think
54:14
it's been a double-edged sword. Would that
54:16
happen automatically if we could wave that
54:18
magic wand and increase funding? It would
54:20
help. I don't know that it would
54:22
help enough, but it would help. I
54:24
mean, you also kind of have to change
54:26
the mindset. about what people call fishing expeditions.
54:28
That's like a bad word in grant
54:30
review panels, fishing expedition, meaning you don't
54:33
really know what you're going to find,
54:35
but you got to go look before
54:37
you can figure out what's important. So
54:39
we have to kind of change that
54:41
mindset as well. One can usefully concreteize
54:43
this discussion. I imagine that one of
54:45
this, I don't read these papers because
54:47
they upset me, but I imagine inflammation
54:49
is on one or more of these.
54:51
Sure is. Chronic inflammation. chronic inflammation. What
54:53
that does is, if you say I'm
54:55
interested in chronic inflammation, so I'm doing
54:58
good stuff, huh? But what could
55:00
be happening is this particular set
55:02
of cytokines might be overexpressed by
55:04
some glial cells and that leads
55:06
to loss of cognitive function, whereas
55:08
this other overlapping set of cytokines
55:11
produced by the macrophages in your
55:13
fat may be due more prone
55:15
to diabetes or metabolic syndrome, whereas
55:17
this particular set of lymphocytes...
55:20
are necessary to repel COVID,
55:22
and that's why you are
55:24
more susceptible to COVID. So
55:26
learning what changes within the
55:29
extremely broad generic idea of
55:31
inflammation, what changes in what
55:33
cell types, in what people,
55:36
under what pharmacological or genetic
55:38
changes, how they are interacting
55:40
with other aspects of pathology,
55:43
that's marvelous to do, but
55:45
to say, oh, inflammation, that
55:47
gets bad when you're old. is
55:49
a way of avoiding the labor of
55:51
thinking, and that's why I'm against it.
55:54
And I think Matt brought up
55:56
a really important point, and we
55:58
scientists are to blame. is the
56:00
way that research gets reviewed. For
56:03
lazy reviewers having these 12
56:05
hallmarks is really helpful. This has
56:07
got one of the hallmarks in
56:10
it. This must be good stuff.
56:12
I do think reviewers need to be
56:14
more open to new ideas and
56:16
new approaches. I mean everybody
56:19
knows that NIH grants are
56:21
approved if they're incremental. If
56:23
they're really breakthrough, they don't
56:26
get approved. A very famous biologist,
56:28
E.O. Wilson, told me years ago,
56:30
he said, don't ever include your
56:32
best ideas in a grant. They
56:35
won't get funded. Do the standard
56:37
stuff, save your best ideas for
56:39
projects that you do on the
56:41
side. That's one of the reasons
56:43
I left academia. Draw me
56:45
nuts. Almost impossible to get the
56:47
important stuff funded. The second
56:50
of your multi-partite question was, does epigenetic
56:52
change? Is it causal? And the
56:54
third, which we may get to,
56:56
is can you reverse it, and
56:58
would that be a good thing?
57:00
So let's talk about the second
57:02
element here, is it causal. The problem
57:04
is what it means. There are some changes
57:07
that occur in this particular set
57:09
of 40 cells in the pineal,
57:11
and there are other changes that occur
57:13
in these cells in the bone marrow,
57:15
and there are other cells that change
57:17
in the gut and villus lining cells,
57:19
and the crypt cells, so they are
57:21
all epigenetic in some. are caused by
57:24
some things, and we don't really know
57:26
which, if any of these, count for
57:28
aging. If someone says, I'm going
57:30
to prove that an epigenetic
57:32
change is responsible for
57:35
aging, they haven't begun to come
57:37
to grips with the nitty-gritty.
57:39
People always ask, just as
57:41
you hinted, does your drug
57:44
change epigenetic things? And
57:46
unfortunately, that's where they stop
57:48
thinking. We're always willing to give people
57:50
tissues from our drug-treated mice if they
57:53
are keen on epigenetic changes that affect
57:55
neuron regeneration. Excellent. Their experts will send
57:57
them the brains and they can do
57:59
that. stuff, it's important. I'm not making
58:02
fun of it, but the general notion
58:04
that it's aging vaguely thought of is
58:06
due to epigenetic change, more vaguely thought
58:08
of, doesn't really get you anywhere. That's
58:11
my skeptical view. Is part of the
58:13
issue that you're saying, well, what's causing
58:15
the cause? No, it's just that the
58:18
concept of epigenetic change encompasses thousands of
58:20
changes in hundreds of cell types under
58:22
hundreds of influences. Of course, some of
58:25
that causes other stuff agreeing to that,
58:27
assenting to that notion that epigenetic change
58:29
is causal for all sorts of age-related
58:31
pathologies. Everyone can agree to that, but
58:34
it's meaningless, because what counts is to
58:36
say, this specific change is really important
58:38
in this disease. Here's an epigenetic alteration,
58:41
or this specific broad spectrum change in
58:43
multiple tissues. causes something good or bad.
58:45
You have to define what it is
58:48
before you can test it. So let's
58:50
use a specific example. When you look
58:52
at a patient with type 1 diabetes
58:54
and you look at their beta cells
58:57
in their pancreas, they look different epigenetically
58:59
than the beta cells of an age-matched
59:01
person without type 1 diabetes. And we
59:04
also know that their beta cells don't
59:06
function. So they've lost function. So let's
59:08
ask that question as a specific example.
59:11
Do you believe or what confidence would
59:13
you assign to the notion that the
59:15
epigenetic change on the beta cells of
59:17
the type 1 diabetic are indeed causal
59:20
to the loss of function of the
59:22
beta cell? My last exposure to the
59:24
causes of type 1 diabetes was on
59:27
eyes in medical school, which is more
59:29
than five years ago, but if I
59:31
vaguely remember it was an autoimmune disease,
59:33
right? So if your poor little helpless
59:36
beta cells are being attacked by antibodies
59:38
and macrophages and things... those stress reactions
59:40
are going to cause epigenetic change. And
59:43
when those epigenetic changes contribute to some
59:45
extent to the ill... fate of the
59:47
beta cells, it's possible. And if I
59:50
were an expert on diabetes pathogenesis, I'd
59:52
really want to know that. It doesn't
59:54
have anything to do with aging, but
59:56
it's an interesting question. But it's a
59:59
way to address causality. Yeah, but you
1:00:01
might equally say, no, it's the mitochondrial
1:00:03
that have changed. There are a hallmark
1:00:06
of diabetes. Yeah, or it's the glycated
1:00:08
proteins. There's a ton of things, and
1:00:10
there's no reason in the world at
1:00:13
this stage, I think, to actually give
1:00:15
epigenetics genetics primacy. over anything else. It's
1:00:17
a nice hypothesis. It's a hypothesis. You
1:00:19
can formulate these questions because a lot
1:00:22
is known about type 1 diabetes. And
1:00:24
I understand 0.05% of the biology of
1:00:26
aging. Zero zero five. Yeah. I was
1:00:29
giving... You're off by an order of
1:00:31
magnitude. He was raising you by a
1:00:33
log. Yeah. I thought you would log
1:00:36
off. Formulating the questions in exactly the
1:00:38
way Steve did. makes it clear how
1:00:40
difficult it is to evaluate the concept
1:00:42
that epigenetic change contributes to pathogenesis and
1:00:45
type 1 diabetes. And we know more
1:00:47
or less what is going on in
1:00:49
type... We don't know what's going on
1:00:52
in aging. We don't even know what
1:00:54
part of the body is going on
1:00:56
or parts more likely of the body.
1:00:59
I at least internally reframe it a
1:01:01
little bit and say, what would the
1:01:03
experiment be? What would you need to
1:01:05
do to convince yourself that either broadly
1:01:08
speaking epigenetic dysregulation causes aging, whatever that
1:01:10
means, or this specific epigenetic change that
1:01:12
is associated with chronological age causes aging?
1:01:15
And so that's an easier way for
1:01:17
me to think about it because I
1:01:19
feel like it's all a... fascinating conversation,
1:01:22
but we're never going to get to
1:01:24
the answer until somebody actually does the
1:01:26
experiment. Or decides that it can't be
1:01:28
formulated because it's too complicated. It gives
1:01:31
up. Yeah, that's right. But people are
1:01:33
trying to do both of those things.
1:01:35
I mean, people are using partial or
1:01:38
transient epigenetic reprogramming and asking can that
1:01:40
have effects on biological aging. I'm actually
1:01:42
cautiously optimistic it can. I don't think
1:01:44
it's going to be a game changer,
1:01:47
but I think you can modulate aspects
1:01:49
of biological aging. epigenetic modifications, so if
1:01:51
we think this particular epigenetic mark at
1:01:54
this particular location in the genome controls
1:01:56
aging. And I don't think it's going
1:01:58
to be that simple, but let's say
1:02:01
it is. You could go in, you
1:02:03
could modify that, and then see, do
1:02:05
you reduce disease? Do you increase lifespan?
1:02:07
Do you improve health span? So those
1:02:10
are the kinds of experiments that I
1:02:12
think would get us to where we
1:02:14
can have a lot of confidence. If
1:02:17
it's the case, if somebody, let's say
1:02:19
at Altos, at Altos, publishes a paper,
1:02:21
Three years from now, that they have
1:02:24
made a mouse live six years by
1:02:26
multiple rounds of transient epigenetic reprogramming, I'll
1:02:28
be like their biggest fan. They moved
1:02:30
the needle. That convinces me that that
1:02:33
strategy modulates biological aging. Nobody's done that
1:02:35
yet. What about something far less impressive,
1:02:37
but still with WALC? So consider, if
1:02:40
we could get to the point where
1:02:42
we could locally deliver vectors that would
1:02:44
epigenetically change chondracites... so that you could
1:02:47
take osteoarthritis in the knee and just
1:02:49
regenerate cartilage, regenerate cartilage by changing the
1:02:51
epigeno. But is that biological aging? I
1:02:53
wouldn't be convinced that's modulating the biological
1:02:56
aging process. I would be convinced that's
1:02:58
a clinically useful strategy for people who
1:03:00
benefit from that therapy. I guess it
1:03:03
kind of depends on why we think
1:03:05
an individual would be experiencing osteoarthritis. How
1:03:07
much of that is senescence? How much
1:03:10
of that is inflammation? Before we go
1:03:12
down that and ask, is it the
1:03:14
S? Yeah, yeah, let's talk about Sonecis.
1:03:16
If you think osteoarthritis of the knee
1:03:19
requires a knee joint replacement and that's
1:03:21
going to help your patient, you are
1:03:23
not rejuvenating. It's perfectly possible to do
1:03:26
great things with technology, including chondrocyte regeneration,
1:03:28
without having to decide that that's related
1:03:30
to aging. People don't age because they
1:03:33
fail to have titanium knee joints or
1:03:35
something. And one way I think about
1:03:37
this, and again this may be completely
1:03:39
wrong, but it's a useful way for
1:03:42
me to think about it, is I
1:03:44
think about age related disease as the
1:03:46
down. extreme effect of biological aging. For
1:03:49
most diseases, there becomes a point where
1:03:51
the pathology of that disease mechanistically is
1:03:53
no longer the same as biological aging.
1:03:56
In which case- He's very good. You
1:03:58
should listen to him. And one of
1:04:00
the implications of that is the interventions
1:04:02
that slow biological aging may not work.
1:04:05
Once you get past that point, but
1:04:07
things that do work for that disease
1:04:09
may have nothing to do with biological
1:04:12
aging. Does that make sense? Yeah, go
1:04:14
deeper on that idea, though. Let's use
1:04:16
the example. My favorite disease. Let's talk
1:04:18
about cancer. Cancer is an easy one.
1:04:21
We know with cancer in many cancers,
1:04:23
the process is you have one or
1:04:25
more mutations, which then often lead to
1:04:28
additional mutations. You get genome instability. Eventually
1:04:30
you get an oncogen that gets activated
1:04:32
and that leads to uncontrolled cell division.
1:04:35
There are more tumor suppression gene that
1:04:37
gets deactivated. Yeah, right. And if we
1:04:39
accept that immune surveillance is one important
1:04:41
anti-cancer mechanism. We know that immune surveillance
1:04:44
declines with age, so early on we're
1:04:46
clearing a lot of our cancers. As
1:04:48
our immune system declines, these cancers are
1:04:51
going to escape immune surveillance, they're going
1:04:53
to accumulate all these mutations, they're eventually
1:04:55
going to go into uncontrolled cell division.
1:04:58
That uncontrolled cell division at that point?
1:05:00
You can treat the cancer, but uncontrolled
1:05:02
cell division is not biological aging. it's
1:05:04
not a part of the normative aging
1:05:07
process. So the treatment there, so the
1:05:09
mechanism now is fundamentally different from normative
1:05:11
aging, and the treatment, let's just say
1:05:14
the treatment in this case is chemotherapy,
1:05:16
might benefit the cancer. It's not going
1:05:18
to do normal aging, right? And I
1:05:21
think rapamycin is a good example here
1:05:23
where I think we all believe that
1:05:25
rapamycin and inhibiting amtour slows biological aging,
1:05:27
at least in up to mice, hopefully
1:05:30
in dogs, hopefully in people. Yeah. So
1:05:32
it's a fundamental... node in the network,
1:05:34
that's the way I think about the
1:05:37
hallmarks of aging. It's a node in
1:05:39
the network that underlies the hallmarks of
1:05:41
aging. So we can manipulate amtor with
1:05:44
rapamycin, slow aging. Rapomycin is a pretty
1:05:46
good anti-cancer drug until the cancer is...
1:05:48
have evolved to ignore the M-tor break,
1:05:50
then then rapid mison doesn't work anymore.
1:05:53
And we know rapid mison doesn't work
1:05:55
for most cancers. That's an example. That's
1:05:57
been tested. We know that. Yeah, absolutely.
1:06:00
And it's because the cancers evolve to
1:06:02
bypass the M-tor break or to bypass
1:06:04
the ability of rapid mison to inhibit
1:06:07
M-tors. That's a really good point that
1:06:09
we all take for granted that I
1:06:11
think is worth noting. Rapomycin can be
1:06:13
unsuccessful as a chemotherapy agent and can
1:06:16
yet be very successful as a cancer
1:06:18
preventive agent. Absolutely. And it's exactly for
1:06:20
that reason. And I think this also
1:06:23
illustrates why traditional disease-based medicine is not
1:06:25
about the biology of aging. It's about
1:06:27
something of the biology of aging is
1:06:30
distinct and it needs to be investigated
1:06:32
in a different way. And we know
1:06:34
that in the aging field, but the
1:06:36
people in the cancer field, in the
1:06:39
cardiology field, in the neurology field, I
1:06:41
don't think they understand that. This gets
1:06:43
to, if I were health czar, this
1:06:46
is what I would do, because it
1:06:48
comes back to what Rich said at
1:06:50
the outset, which is, why is this
1:06:52
a zero-sum game? I mean, you didn't
1:06:55
ask it that way, but that's effectively
1:06:57
the problem you're dealing with, which is...
1:06:59
Why can't we study cardiology, oncology, and
1:07:02
neurology and aging without everybody feeling like
1:07:04
a thing? My way of saying that
1:07:06
in Peter terms is we need to
1:07:09
have medicine 2.0 and medicine 3.0 in
1:07:11
parallel because the tools of the medicine
1:07:13
2.0 scientist and physician, which we see
1:07:15
on display today, are putting the stent
1:07:18
in, giving the chemotherapy, lowering the cholesterol,
1:07:20
all of these things, the medicine 3.0
1:07:22
toolkit looks different. Different science. You're going
1:07:25
to use wrap a mice in here,
1:07:27
you're not going to use it over
1:07:29
here because it's too late. Instead of
1:07:32
saying one or the other, why isn't
1:07:34
it both? Why wouldn't we want both
1:07:36
of these running in parallel? Well, we
1:07:38
would, but of course, the zero-sum game
1:07:41
is a pretty good analogy for what's
1:07:43
actually going on. The amount of research
1:07:45
dollars at least available to NIH is
1:07:48
not infinitely expensible. It's set by a
1:07:50
complex political process that... allocates it amongst
1:07:52
institutions, so saying that it would be
1:07:55
a good idea to have more funds.
1:07:57
I agree with you, and I'll bet
1:07:59
these two guys do as well, but
1:08:01
it's not easy to do. Yeah, I
1:08:04
think I misspoke. It will be a
1:08:06
portfolio of reallocation. But it will be
1:08:08
worthwhile because the burden of this disease
1:08:11
will be lower. So in other words,
1:08:13
it's sort of like saying, right now
1:08:15
I spend $100,000 a year on the
1:08:18
barrier to my house to prevent anybody
1:08:20
from breaking in. and I spend $100
1:08:22
a year patrolling the neighborhood to make
1:08:24
sure there aren't too many bad guys
1:08:27
in the neighborhood. There's a scenario where
1:08:29
if your total budget is $100,000 and
1:08:31
$100. Maybe you could spend $80,000 in
1:08:34
total by spending more money patrolling the
1:08:36
neighborhood. I think we generally agree with
1:08:38
you that having a greater proportion of
1:08:41
available research dollars, both private and public,
1:08:43
going into the biology of aging and
1:08:45
its impact on late life health, would
1:08:47
be a good thing. I don't think
1:08:50
you're going to get an argument from
1:08:52
anybody in... the cardiology field, the neurology
1:08:54
field, or Alzheimer's, the Alzheimer's field. It's
1:08:57
their money. But wouldn't some of those
1:08:59
people, as the funding dollars move towards
1:09:01
the aging side, also want to move
1:09:03
and say, look, I'm going to study
1:09:06
this through the aging lens? I was
1:09:08
on the council for the National Aging
1:09:10
Institute for three years, and if at
1:09:13
any point I can swear to this
1:09:15
from personal testimony, somebody would say something
1:09:17
like, I wonder if maybe a few
1:09:20
percent of the Alzheimer's budget. might instead
1:09:22
go to studying how slow aging models
1:09:24
would have an impact on late life
1:09:26
neurodegenerative disease, the next day the director
1:09:29
of the aging institute would get a
1:09:31
call from two or three Congress people
1:09:33
who were on the Appropriations Committee stating
1:09:36
that this will not be happening because
1:09:38
there was an Alzheimer's association person who
1:09:40
got the call from the NIA staff
1:09:43
member in charge of Alzheimer's saying, tell
1:09:45
the congressman to call the director. and
1:09:47
let's put a stop to that reckless
1:09:49
idea. They're tied in to the political
1:09:52
process in ways. Well, we just need
1:09:54
to go maybe one step further because
1:09:56
those Congress people have a boss. They
1:09:59
report to somebody too. Would that be
1:10:01
at the stage? Yeah, no. I mean,
1:10:03
come on. Maybe it's because the public
1:10:06
doesn't understand this. Those people answer to
1:10:08
the public. That's a good example. These
1:10:10
are our dollars. That's right. That are
1:10:12
going on with you. But Alzheimer's association.
1:10:15
I mean, that's a patient advocacy group.
1:10:17
That is the public. Yes, although let's
1:10:19
ask the question, what have they done
1:10:22
for those patients lately? That's a different
1:10:24
question, but I mean, I'm just reinforcing
1:10:26
what you said. I think part of
1:10:29
this is educating people. If you know
1:10:31
somebody who's suffering from Alzheimer's disease, you
1:10:33
know very well that the only thing
1:10:35
we've got going for us right now
1:10:38
is prevention. We don't have too many
1:10:40
silver bullets in the treatment gun. Despite
1:10:42
massive spending. I was once in Congress
1:10:45
trying to lobby. with about six people
1:10:47
from the Alzheimer's Association in the same
1:10:49
room. I was totally ignored by staffers
1:10:52
that were in there. I mean, I
1:10:54
agree with all of this. I agree
1:10:56
with all of this. I think, again,
1:10:58
though, we should be careful not to
1:11:01
demonize people for wanting to cure Alzheimer's.
1:11:03
It's a good thing. It's a good
1:11:05
goal. I think the communication piece is
1:11:08
about the fact that it's going to
1:11:10
be much more efficient and effective to
1:11:12
keep people from getting it in the
1:11:15
first place. Once you've outpaced the biology
1:11:17
of aging with the pathology of the
1:11:19
disease, it gets a lot harder, a
1:11:21
lot harder to do anything about it.
1:11:24
So I think that communication part, honestly,
1:11:26
I don't know why we've been so
1:11:28
unsuccessful, because I think a lot of
1:11:31
us have been out there trying to
1:11:33
communicate this message for a long time,
1:11:35
but it's starting to permeate. We're at
1:11:37
that moment, I think, where people are
1:11:40
starting to get it, that biological aging
1:11:42
is a thing. It's malleable. It's going
1:11:44
to take a little while, but there's
1:11:47
reason to be optimistic. And there's also
1:11:49
the private sector is another reason, I
1:11:51
think, to be optimistic. So let's go.
1:11:54
record right now I think when we
1:11:56
if we defeat Alzheimer's disease it's going
1:11:58
to be because of the biology of
1:12:00
aging it's not going to be because
1:12:03
of the drugs that get rid of
1:12:05
absolutely yeah probably cancer probably heart disease
1:12:07
although I think Peter's more optimistic we
1:12:10
can prevent heart disease if you took
1:12:12
the tools of medicine 2.0 and just
1:12:14
applied them 30 years earlier we wouldn't
1:12:17
have ASCPD that's the one place where
1:12:19
it's word but again that's because the
1:12:21
mechanism of action is so well understood
1:12:23
with ASCD compared to Alzheimer's and cancer
1:12:26
a lot of infectious disease, a lot
1:12:28
of liver disease, a lot of kidney
1:12:30
disease, all of those things can be
1:12:33
improved dramatically by targeting the biology of
1:12:35
aging. You know, if I were to
1:12:37
write my book again, I would add
1:12:40
a fifth horseman, because I talked about
1:12:42
these four horsemen of ASCVD, cancer, neurodegenerative
1:12:44
and demanding diseases, and metabolic disease, but
1:12:46
I would actually add a fifth hallmark.
1:12:49
It's not really a hallmark of disease,
1:12:51
but it's the fifth thing that brings
1:12:53
life to a bad close, which is
1:12:56
immune dysfunction, which is immune dysfunction. And
1:12:58
I don't think I gave that enough
1:13:00
attention in the book, because of course,
1:13:03
as you said, it factors in very
1:13:05
heavily to oncogenesis, but also, as COVID
1:13:07
showed us, what a risk factor it
1:13:09
was to be old. And you know,
1:13:12
I'm reminded of this when I see
1:13:14
people my age get brutal pneumonias, and
1:13:16
like two months later, they're okay. And
1:13:19
you realize, two of my patients actually
1:13:21
in the past six months have had
1:13:23
really bad pneumonias. where you're looking at
1:13:26
the CT of their chest and you
1:13:28
cannot believe they're alive. But of course
1:13:30
they're fine. Three months later, four courses
1:13:32
of antibiotics later, they're fine. And you
1:13:35
realize, you do that to a 75-year-old,
1:13:37
it's over. And it simply comes down
1:13:39
to how their B cells and T
1:13:42
cells work. That to me is an
1:13:44
area where I'd love to see more
1:13:46
attention, which is, what would it take
1:13:49
to rejuvenate the immune system as a
1:13:51
proactive statement? I think that that's a
1:13:53
perfect example. Influenza pneumonia has never fallen
1:13:55
out of the top 10 causes of
1:13:58
death in the US. You know, it
1:14:00
used to be number two, but still
1:14:02
now it's number eight or nine, but
1:14:05
it's always there because you can't really
1:14:07
do anything about the late life immune
1:14:09
dysfunction. Just to follow this up, if
1:14:11
magically you become in charge and you're
1:14:14
able to double the amount of research
1:14:16
being done on the biology of aging
1:14:18
fundamentally, then we can afford to do,
1:14:21
let's give some mice to start with
1:14:23
a batch of anti-aging drugs. and see
1:14:25
if it makes them more resistant to
1:14:28
infectious illnesses, including pneumonias, but viral infections
1:14:30
as well and many others. I'd love
1:14:32
to know the answer to that, and
1:14:34
no one has actually really looked in
1:14:37
a serious way, because the ITP has
1:14:39
enough money to just measure lifespan. None
1:14:41
of the health span. Just hoping that
1:14:44
everybody else is now going to look
1:14:46
at the brain and the lungs and
1:14:48
the infection, the sensory systems. That really
1:14:51
ought to be done, and it's not
1:14:53
being done because of a lack of
1:14:55
a lack of money. You said something
1:14:57
a while ago, Rich, that I think
1:15:00
is timely now, which is with each
1:15:02
generation of these drugs, they get more
1:15:04
efficacious and less toxic. Not yet, but
1:15:07
that's the hope. Well, no, no, but
1:15:09
I'm going to use another example. The
1:15:11
GLP1s are the best example of this,
1:15:14
right? So you go back to the
1:15:16
very, very first generation of GLP1 agonists
1:15:18
barely lost any weight, horrible side effects,
1:15:20
horrible side effects, horrible side effects. Fast
1:15:23
forward to semi-glutide, quite a bit better
1:15:25
efficacy, still really bad side effects. Next
1:15:27
generation trusepetite, better efficacy, side effects are
1:15:30
almost gone. Now, why haven't we been
1:15:32
able to do that with these geo-protective
1:15:34
drugs? So we have this one study
1:15:37
using Evrolamus that gives us a hint
1:15:39
that says, hey, this might actually enhance
1:15:41
immune function in people in their mid-60s.
1:15:43
But we need the follow-up study, the
1:15:46
follow-up drug, the follow-up drug, imagine what
1:15:48
the fourth generation of that drug can
1:15:50
do. where it's tuned to get better
1:15:53
and better and a fewer side of
1:15:55
it. There are strong commercial motivations. You
1:15:57
know you're going to sell a lot
1:16:00
of the obesity drugs. They're very strong.
1:16:02
martial motivations to do those studies over
1:16:04
and over and over again until you
1:16:06
find one that works better. And there
1:16:09
are good preclinical models that you can
1:16:11
use that you're not wasting too much
1:16:13
of your time on clinical trials. That
1:16:16
could be done for anti-aging drugs as
1:16:18
well, although testing anti-aging drugs and people
1:16:20
is a whole separate set of tangle
1:16:23
of difficulties. I don't want to talk
1:16:25
about that right now. But I'm saying
1:16:27
it won't be quite as easy as
1:16:29
it was for anti-obacy medications, but no
1:16:32
one's doing even the first level of
1:16:34
research to find the optimal compounds for
1:16:36
efficacy without side effects or even to
1:16:39
begin to see if they have desirable
1:16:41
effects on aging rate indicators in people.
1:16:43
That's kind of a cheap and easy
1:16:45
study and no one has really tackled
1:16:48
that yet. Well, I just heard that
1:16:50
there are over 80 synolithic studies in
1:16:52
early clinical trials. It's a joke. It's
1:16:55
a joke. It's a joke. It's a
1:16:57
joke. We have to come back to
1:16:59
this. Are any of them powered for
1:17:02
anything other than safety? This is, I
1:17:04
think, a problem. Oh, it's all phase
1:17:06
one, exactly. So they're underpowered. They're almost
1:17:08
useless, in my opinion. Well, until they
1:17:11
get to phase two, phase three. Well,
1:17:13
until they get to phase two, phase
1:17:15
three, phase three. Well, until they get
1:17:18
to phase two, two, two. There's lots
1:17:20
of complicated issues here. I think endpoints
1:17:22
for clinical trials are really challenging, but
1:17:25
solvable. So there are two places I
1:17:27
wanted to go next, and I'm going
1:17:29
to let Rich decide, because he's going
1:17:31
to have the strongest point of view.
1:17:34
Can we talk about senescence? Or can
1:17:36
we talk about what biomarkers would be
1:17:38
necessary to help us study aging in
1:17:41
humans as we translate from your work
1:17:43
and Matt's work? I know what I
1:17:45
want to talk about, and it's the
1:17:48
second of those, too. I don't want
1:17:50
to spend the next three or four
1:17:52
hours explaining why senescence is silly and
1:17:54
anti-senalytics are untested at best. So there's
1:17:57
no way we're not talking about that.
1:17:59
Let's go on to item number two.
1:18:01
And I think the most important thing...
1:18:04
is to make a clear distinction between
1:18:06
biomarkers and aging rate indicators. Please explain
1:18:08
the difference to people, please. Okay, I'll
1:18:11
do my best. So a biomarker, allegedly,
1:18:13
and in real life, is something that
1:18:15
changes with age. So if you have
1:18:17
some drug that slows aging, the biomarkers,
1:18:20
many of them in the different cell
1:18:22
types and in the blood will change
1:18:24
more slowly. They are a good way
1:18:27
of looking at whether you're slowing it.
1:18:29
rate of change of biomarkers,
1:18:31
very established part of the
1:18:33
literature, and valuable. But you
1:18:35
have to wait till somebody's old, whether
1:18:38
it's a dog or a mouse or
1:18:40
a person, because only when they're old,
1:18:42
has the bio marker of aging, the
1:18:45
surrogate marker for biological aging, changed very
1:18:47
much. So in a clinical trial, certainly
1:18:49
in a human situation, no
1:18:51
one wants to wait 20 years to
1:18:53
see whether the biomarkers have changed. And
1:18:56
a one-year is such a tiny fraction
1:18:58
of a human lifespan that you
1:19:00
don't really anticipate detectable change
1:19:02
with a appropriately powered study.
1:19:04
It's like aging rate indicators, which
1:19:07
are much less well studied and much
1:19:09
less well established in principle, are things
1:19:11
you can measure that tell you whether
1:19:13
in a slow aging state or a
1:19:15
normal state. Can I just make a point
1:19:17
for the listener so they understand
1:19:19
the challenge of what we're talking
1:19:21
about? When we study blood pressure
1:19:23
drugs or cholesterol drugs, The
1:19:25
biomarkers change so rapidly
1:19:27
and we know the relationship
1:19:30
between the biomarker and the
1:19:32
disease state. So if your
1:19:34
blood pressure is 145 over 90,
1:19:36
on average, before I give you
1:19:38
this ace inhibitor, and three months
1:19:41
later, six months later, nine
1:19:43
months later, your blood pressure
1:19:45
is averaging 119 over 74, I
1:19:48
know I've done something well. Now I will
1:19:50
still probably in the phase three, in fact
1:19:52
I will in the phase three, have to
1:19:54
make sure that I also reduce some event
1:19:56
in you. But generally by the phase
1:19:58
two, I know that this drug is... not toxic
1:20:00
and that it's predictably lowering your blood pressure,
1:20:02
that's really, really valuable. A biomarker generically
1:20:04
is something that's easy to measure that is
1:20:06
informative about something that's hard to measure.
1:20:09
A classical example, a famous example is you
1:20:11
want to know how many cigarettes somebody
1:20:13
smokes a day, they'll lie to you. But
1:20:15
if you measure cotoning in their blood, that's
1:20:17
a byproduct of nicotine, you don't have to
1:20:20
ask them. You can find out how many
1:20:22
cigarettes they had in the last couple of
1:20:24
days by measuring blood. That's a biomarker of
1:20:26
cigarette consumption. Is it a marker of nicotine
1:20:28
or carbon monoxide? I don't know the answer
1:20:30
to that. I wasn't sure. In
1:20:32
principle, a biomarker of aging is, are many
1:20:34
of them, and they are measuring biological aging
1:20:36
processes, and they're useful in that regard. But
1:20:38
they don't tell you how fast you're aging. The
1:20:41
analogy I love to use is an
1:20:43
odometer is like a biomarker of aging
1:20:45
of your car. It tells you how
1:20:47
many miles your car has gone, but
1:20:49
it doesn't tell you how fast the
1:20:51
car is going. The speedometer tells you how
1:20:53
fast your car is going. What we
1:20:55
need, and what I think we're
1:20:57
just beginning now to document, is
1:21:00
things like the speedometer, aging rate
1:21:02
indicators that reliably discriminate
1:21:04
slow aging mice or people
1:21:06
from regular old mice or
1:21:08
people. We have now dozen
1:21:10
or so things that change in the fat,
1:21:13
in the blood, in the liver, in the brain,
1:21:15
and in the muscle that are always changed
1:21:17
in any slow aging mouse, whether
1:21:19
it's drug A, drug B, drug C,
1:21:21
calisthenic diet, or single gene
1:21:23
mutations. We've looked now at
1:21:25
five different single gene mutations, and
1:21:27
this whole set of 12,
1:21:29
or roughly 12, aging rate
1:21:31
indicators always changes in every slow aging
1:21:34
mouse, and it does so in youth,
1:21:36
which is the key point. So,
1:21:38
if it does so quickly after
1:21:40
an anti -aging drug is administered,
1:21:43
that's the transition. That's the bridge
1:21:45
you need for clinical studies in
1:21:47
people. If you want to know whether
1:21:49
metformin or canagliflozin or something slows
1:21:51
aging in people, and you don't want
1:21:53
to wait 20 years, but you've
1:21:55
got things that tell you whether they're
1:21:57
in a slow aging state, how
1:21:59
fast... they are aging versus normal, and
1:22:02
that's a big if. We don't yet have
1:22:04
evidence we can do that. We just have
1:22:06
hope we can do that. Then that allows
1:22:08
you quickly, quickly being within six
1:22:10
months to a year, to know whether
1:22:13
your anti-aging manipulation, alleged
1:22:15
anti-aging manipulation, has moved them
1:22:17
to a physiological status which is
1:22:20
associated with slower aging. A lot of
1:22:22
that can be done in mice with drugs,
1:22:24
with mutants. And are these all
1:22:26
proteins rich? No. No, some of them
1:22:28
are changes in the fat,
1:22:30
different classes of macrophages. The pro-inflammatory
1:22:33
macrophages, the anti-inflammatory macrophages, the good
1:22:35
ones, go up. UCP1, I recall
1:22:37
from our discussion. UCP1, goes up
1:22:40
in every one of our 10
1:22:42
different kinds, 11 now of slow
1:22:44
aging mice. Does it go up
1:22:47
in any of the mice that
1:22:49
did not receive a successful drug?
1:22:51
Well, we compare them to controls,
1:22:54
and the... Question you're asking is really
1:22:56
important. That's what we're doing in the
1:22:58
next five years. We just got a
1:23:00
grant to do that. We're going to
1:23:02
take mice and give them either a
1:23:04
good drug or a different drug that
1:23:06
doesn't work and then make those
1:23:08
comparisons. A really important thing to prove.
1:23:10
So far, our only control has been
1:23:13
untreated mice. At some point in this,
1:23:15
I had to bring this up. But
1:23:17
let's imagine that Rich is incredibly successful
1:23:19
at finding these things. That is a
1:23:22
very, very long way. from assuming that it's
1:23:24
going to be the same in people.
1:23:26
Most things that clinically
1:23:28
work in mice do not work in people.
1:23:30
It might be, and that would be
1:23:33
wonderful, but I think ultimately we're
1:23:35
going to have to find this
1:23:37
for people. And my thought is
1:23:39
the kind of evaluation that you
1:23:41
do routinely of your patients.
1:23:43
If we took a group of 65-year-olds
1:23:45
and we gave them a drug
1:23:48
that we thought was an anti-aging
1:23:50
drug, and follow them the next
1:23:52
five or six years doing these
1:23:54
evaluations, I think you could probably
1:23:57
safely say, this is slowing aging.
1:23:59
or not slowing aging. So I don't
1:24:02
think that it's going to be that
1:24:04
easy to jump from ice to people
1:24:06
in this. I've always wondered if in
1:24:08
people the easiest way to do it
1:24:11
would be to take the most obvious
1:24:13
thing that we know is going to
1:24:15
reduce the rate of aging. So it'd
1:24:18
be an interesting experiment, but you find
1:24:20
someone who is overweight diabetic and smokes
1:24:22
and has hypertension. You get hundreds of
1:24:24
these folks. You put half of them
1:24:27
on to be ethical, a plan where
1:24:29
you try to get them to stop
1:24:31
and presumably many don't. In the other
1:24:33
group you pull out all the stops
1:24:36
and you don't care because you're interested
1:24:38
not in testing the hypothesis, does this
1:24:40
thing help you? You're interested in getting
1:24:42
them to lose weight, not have diabetes,
1:24:45
stop smoking, exercise like crazy. The greatest
1:24:47
division between two groups of individuals where
1:24:49
we would I think be able to
1:24:52
agree that this group is now aging
1:24:54
slower, the group that we've... reconcile their
1:24:56
diabetes quit the smoking etc etc etc
1:24:58
and then I'd love to see riches
1:25:01
12 line up in that population that
1:25:03
would be great let me just say
1:25:05
that I think that people that study
1:25:07
animals myself included always underestimate how well
1:25:10
we can evaluate health in people with
1:25:12
a very very thorough evaluation because we
1:25:14
don't do that in our experiment. Why
1:25:17
do you think that is Steve why
1:25:19
is it? Because I was going to
1:25:21
ask about parabiosis later on in the
1:25:23
discussion. We might as well talk about
1:25:26
it now, right? Parabiosis seems to actually
1:25:28
kind of work in certain mouse models.
1:25:30
Do we have any reason to believe
1:25:32
it's going to work in humans? And
1:25:35
if not, why not? Why are mice
1:25:37
so different from people? But wait a
1:25:39
minute. I wouldn't say that just because
1:25:41
we don't have evidence that it works
1:25:44
in humans means mice or when it
1:25:46
comes to parabiosis, right? Yeah, yeah, yeah.
1:25:48
That's a different discussion. translation to people,
1:25:51
most have failed. I actually think that's
1:25:53
because those are artificial mouse models where
1:25:55
they tried to give young mice an
1:25:57
age-related disease. I'm more optimistic. I don't
1:26:00
know this. Rich doesn't have those mice,
1:26:02
yeah. I know. I'm more optimistic that
1:26:04
biological aging or normative aging is going
1:26:06
to be much more likely to translate
1:26:09
to people, both interventions and biomarkers, than
1:26:11
the specific disease interventions. I might be
1:26:13
wrong. I don't know the answer. We
1:26:15
would hope that's a case. That's fair.
1:26:18
I don't think we should rule out
1:26:20
the mice as a useful model. In
1:26:22
fact, I think there's reason to be
1:26:25
optimistic that it will. it's not a
1:26:27
pragmatic approach for population gerotherapeutics. But I'm
1:26:29
just wondering like why it wouldn't be
1:26:31
as efficacious. This is something that, I
1:26:34
mean aren't there six or eight clinical
1:26:36
trials going on right now? Different variants
1:26:38
of that, yeah. Yeah. Yeah. I haven't
1:26:40
seen them. I've seen the one that's
1:26:43
looking at it's not really a parabiosis
1:26:45
study, but it's looking at plasma ferises
1:26:47
for Alzheimer's for Alzheimer's. I consider that
1:26:50
a little bit different, but fair enough,
1:26:52
but fair enough, okay, okay, because they're
1:26:54
just using albuma. studies going on of
1:26:56
young blood. Yeah, okay, okay. But if
1:26:59
you think of peribiosis as both taking
1:27:01
away the bad stuff that accumulates with
1:27:03
age and adding in the good stuff
1:27:05
that's in young, some sort of plasma
1:27:08
exchange hits at least half that equation.
1:27:10
Okay, I'm gonna come back to this,
1:27:12
but my question was, why the difference?
1:27:14
You're saying, Matt, the difference is probably
1:27:17
amplified in disease specific cases like heart
1:27:19
disease, cancer and Alzheimer's disease. probably less
1:27:21
relevant when you're talking about aging, because
1:27:24
even a flawed mouse model still ages.
1:27:26
In fact, it's designed to age in
1:27:28
a certain way. Yeah, and I mean,
1:27:30
I think normative aging looks very similar.
1:27:33
Again, if we look for mice to
1:27:35
dogs to people, just broadly speaking, the
1:27:37
process looks pretty similar. So, I'm cautiously
1:27:39
optimistic that these things are going to
1:27:42
translate. Not to pay too much attention
1:27:44
to Steve's pessimism on this point, although
1:27:46
he's completely right. And it's for a
1:27:49
variety of reasons. Sometimes humans are different
1:27:51
from mice. Sometimes the drug has side
1:27:53
effects that are tolerable in mice, not
1:27:55
tolerable in people, etc. But I always
1:27:58
like to look at the other side
1:28:00
of the coin. That is... If your
1:28:02
goal is to develop a drug that
1:28:04
blunts pain in people and you screen
1:28:07
40 or 50 drugs and you find
1:28:09
a couple that inhibit pain in mice,
1:28:11
that's a really good start. It doesn't
1:28:13
guarantee they're going to work in people,
1:28:16
but it gives you this category of
1:28:18
snail-based neurotoxins. Let's make 40 of those
1:28:20
from 40 different snails. We'll find one
1:28:23
that actually in people works can be
1:28:25
made by a scalable process and doesn't
1:28:27
produce... serious side effects. So the mice,
1:28:29
it's not a one-to-one mapping. It works
1:28:32
in mice, it doesn't, it works in
1:28:34
people, but it's an important critical first
1:28:36
step which usually succeeds in finding a
1:28:38
set of drugs of related families or
1:28:41
with related targets at least that are
1:28:43
efficacious in people. Most drugs that are
1:28:45
used in people had useful rodent-based research
1:28:47
somewhere in their pedigree. Absolutely agree with
1:28:50
that, Rich, and nobody's saying that 100%
1:28:52
of things at work in mice do
1:28:54
not work. But I think there's a
1:28:57
critical difference for aging research, which it
1:28:59
takes four years to do one of
1:29:01
these in mice. And so if we
1:29:03
have to do 40 to find one
1:29:06
or two that works... That's why I
1:29:08
like aging rate indicators. Speed things up.
1:29:10
I'm stepping on your toes, Peter, but
1:29:12
the question I always come back to.
1:29:15
I agree. We need these aging rate
1:29:17
indicators. How do we get to the
1:29:19
point where we get to the point
1:29:22
where we get to the point where
1:29:24
we're confident that they're confident that they
1:29:26
actually work in people, How do we
1:29:28
get to the point that FDA is
1:29:31
confident that they work? That's the only
1:29:33
way you're going to be able to
1:29:35
use them in a clinical trial. I
1:29:37
don't see a path in the short
1:29:40
term. Well, I don't know that we
1:29:42
need that to tell the truth. So
1:29:44
I went to the FDA to try
1:29:46
to get them to approve a trial
1:29:49
of Metformin. And we didn't couch it
1:29:51
in aging because you're right, as soon
1:29:53
as you mention aging, their eyes glaze
1:29:56
over and they're not interested anymore. that
1:29:58
we're fine with that. But that's a
1:30:00
different end point. That's not a biomarker.
1:30:02
My question is that you've merged two
1:30:05
different difficult problems. Problem A, can we
1:30:07
find drugs that slow aging in people?
1:30:09
Problem B, can we... surmount the legal
1:30:11
and political barriers to getting them. That's
1:30:14
not what I was asking. I was
1:30:16
asking, how do we get to the
1:30:18
point? Okay. What I'm saying is that
1:30:21
you are focused on something I don't
1:30:23
have any answers to basically, which is
1:30:25
how do we get the FDA to
1:30:27
develop and approve clinical trials? I was
1:30:30
more interested in a step before that.
1:30:32
It'd be nice to have some drugs
1:30:34
that actually do work to slow aging
1:30:36
in people. But you have to trust
1:30:39
the biomarker of aging rate before you
1:30:41
can be confident that the drug that
1:30:43
moves the biomarker of aging rate works
1:30:45
in people. That's fundamentally what I'm asking.
1:30:48
How do we get to the point
1:30:50
where, let's just take FDA out of
1:30:52
the equation, the four of us would
1:30:55
sit and look at the data and
1:30:57
I'll be like, yep. Well, that's sort
1:30:59
of my thought experiment. I would have
1:31:01
to take an example in humans that
1:31:04
is so egregious, so let's say we
1:31:06
do that. Well, it would make me
1:31:08
worry. It would only show you the
1:31:10
positive signal. It would show you the
1:31:13
specificity and not the sensitivity of the
1:31:15
test. That's the problem. You might miss
1:31:17
the signal. If you found a proteomic
1:31:19
genomic, like if you found a multimodal
1:31:22
signal that detected a difference in rate
1:31:24
of aging between those two very extreme
1:31:26
sets, you might miss it with a
1:31:29
geoprotective drug, which wouldn't be as dramatic
1:31:31
as dramatic as that chain. So what
1:31:33
if I told you that there are
1:31:35
people who claim... There are epigenetic signatures
1:31:38
that do that, that correlate quite well.
1:31:40
They claim with health outcomes, 10-year mortality,
1:31:42
5-year mortality, 3-year mortality in people, and
1:31:44
are measuring the rate of biological aging.
1:31:47
Because it's out there. I mean, it's
1:31:49
in the literature. I mean, this is
1:31:51
not perfect, but it would be one
1:31:54
thing I would immediately think of, which
1:31:56
is I would take a really good
1:31:58
biobank that would have enough samples that
1:32:00
I could sample a bunch of human
1:32:03
stuff. and use an unbiased sample and
1:32:05
a biased sample. So I would determine
1:32:07
an algorithm based on one and see
1:32:09
how well it predicted on another based
1:32:12
on enough samples. That would have to
1:32:14
be true at a minimum. Yeah, I
1:32:16
think it is. I mean... Again, at
1:32:18
least it depends on how much faith
1:32:21
you put in these research studies, but
1:32:23
I mean people have published epigenetic algorithms,
1:32:25
Dunedin Pace is the one that most
1:32:28
people are going to talk about, that
1:32:30
correlate seemingly pretty well, at least with
1:32:32
mortality and with metrics of health span,
1:32:34
for lack of a better way of
1:32:37
framing it. So that exists. And Dunedin
1:32:39
Pace is using something besides epigenetic or
1:32:41
is it only epigenetic? I think it
1:32:43
uses something else. It was trained off
1:32:46
of... other biomarkers and then they found
1:32:48
epigenetic marks that correlate with those other
1:32:50
biomarkers. So it's a correlation to a
1:32:53
correlation, but there's still a correlation. What
1:32:55
do you think, Rich? Well, I wanted
1:32:57
to go back to the example you
1:32:59
gave where you took a lot of
1:33:02
people and gave them intense exercises and
1:33:04
dietary changes to improve their likely health
1:33:06
outcomes, and that's a good place to
1:33:08
start a discussion, because you said every
1:33:11
sensible person would see the treated group
1:33:13
as aging more slowly. And I would
1:33:15
want to ask before I agree to
1:33:17
that, do they also have improved cognition?
1:33:20
How are they doing in cataracts? How
1:33:22
are they doing in hearing? What happens
1:33:24
when you give them a flu shot?
1:33:27
Do they have a great flu shot?
1:33:29
So the things you've pointed to are
1:33:31
really important for both overall health and
1:33:33
for cardiovascular risk and the things linked
1:33:36
to that. So it's nice to know.
1:33:38
But to convince me that you now
1:33:40
have a slow aging group of people.
1:33:42
You need to go beyond the risk
1:33:45
factors for specific common human diseases. If
1:33:47
you could show that, then for the
1:33:49
first time I would be convinced you
1:33:51
had an effective anti-aging manipulation in people.
1:33:54
Currently I don't know that there is
1:33:56
any effective anti-aging manipulation in people. If
1:33:58
your approach got there, that would be
1:34:01
a terrific research model. Well, but now
1:34:03
we're getting into... the definition of aging
1:34:05
a little bit, which is would you
1:34:07
agree that the approach I'm describing would
1:34:10
produce a longer life? It's easy to
1:34:12
produce a longer life if you happen
1:34:14
to have a clinical condition. where you're
1:34:16
tied to a railroad track, and there's
1:34:19
a train coming, you can extend that
1:34:21
woman's life enormously by simply giving her
1:34:23
a knife and cutting the bonds and
1:34:26
letting her walk away from the track.
1:34:28
longevity promoting interventions. Well, if 80% of
1:34:30
people died as a result of trains
1:34:32
on train tracks, that might be a
1:34:35
worthwhile example. But given that 80% of
1:34:37
people die from these four chronic diseases.
1:34:39
I'm all in favor of protecting people
1:34:41
against chronic diseases. Yes, yes, yes. That's
1:34:44
good thing. And I'm glad that people
1:34:46
are doing that. No question about it.
1:34:48
Now, talking about the biology of aging,
1:34:50
there are all sorts of things that
1:34:53
also happen when you get older that
1:34:55
are not part of those chronic diseases.
1:34:57
And to make a case that you've
1:35:00
got an anti-aging manipulation, you need to
1:35:02
show that those are changed too. But
1:35:04
do all of them have to change
1:35:06
or just most of them? Don't enough
1:35:09
have to change that you increase the
1:35:11
length and quality of your life, and
1:35:13
if you still get a cataract at
1:35:15
the same rate, I'm not sure that
1:35:18
should be disqualifying. Right, but the important
1:35:20
thing I think about what Rich said
1:35:22
is all the stuff that he pointed
1:35:25
out could be easily done in humans.
1:35:27
Wouldn't be easily done in humans. Wouldn't
1:35:29
be hard to measure hearing. Well-known, famous,
1:35:31
long and slow aging dog breeds, and
1:35:34
it's true for horses too, certainly true
1:35:36
for mice, is that more or less
1:35:38
everything slows down together, the tiny dogs
1:35:40
that are very long-lived, it's not just
1:35:43
that they have a delay of cancer,
1:35:45
they have a delay in neurodegenerative disease,
1:35:47
a delay in digestive diseases, and joint
1:35:49
diseases, aging has been slowed in those
1:35:52
dogs. And if... The dogs did your
1:35:54
thing. But we might not have an
1:35:56
intervention that does that to your point,
1:35:59
Rich. I'm saying we might not have
1:36:01
a non pharmacologic method that does that.
1:36:03
It's not clear that even though exercise
1:36:05
clearly extends lifespan, it's not clear that
1:36:08
it's doing so by slowing aging. Those
1:36:10
are two different things, to your point.
1:36:12
It's not clear, but it's an interesting
1:36:14
question. Like, do you believe exercise slows
1:36:17
aging? Exercise, healthy diet, sleep? I have
1:36:19
no idea. I think so. My intuition
1:36:21
is I think so, but I can't
1:36:23
point to the evidence that tells me
1:36:26
so. Well, there's evidence to support it.
1:36:28
The question is, does it rise to
1:36:30
the level of evidence that would convince
1:36:33
Rich? I believe it probably does too,
1:36:35
but I'm not going to, say, with
1:36:37
100% certainty. I think here's where we
1:36:39
get back into health span versus lifespan.
1:36:42
The effect of exercise on longevity is
1:36:44
enormous. Somewhat depends on where you start.
1:36:46
I've always found these to be a
1:36:48
little bit problematic because I don't think
1:36:51
that defining it by the input is
1:36:53
as valuable as defining it by the
1:36:55
output. In other words, to say you
1:36:58
exercise this many minutes a week versus
1:37:00
that many minutes a week is a
1:37:02
little dirty because intensity matters, what you
1:37:04
do matters, sometimes the output is what
1:37:07
matters more, how strong you are, how
1:37:09
high your VO2 Max is, those tend
1:37:11
to be more predictive, because that's the
1:37:13
integral of the work that's been done.
1:37:16
But your point is, it's well taken.
1:37:18
The impact on health span is what
1:37:20
I tell my patients. If this amount
1:37:22
of exercise didn't make you live one
1:37:25
day longer, the quality in which your
1:37:27
life would improve would justify it. Now
1:37:29
fortunately we can move past this semantic
1:37:32
discussions because there's now molecular ways of
1:37:34
checking this. Exercises, I'll bet all of
1:37:36
you know, increases an enzyme called GPLD1
1:37:38
in the blood of exercise people and
1:37:41
in mice, and Sovieta's lap has shown
1:37:43
that if you elevate GPLD1... It does
1:37:45
great things to your brain, more neurogenesis
1:37:47
and more brain-derived protective factors, brain-driven neurotrophic
1:37:50
factors. Iresen also goes up in humans
1:37:52
and in mice. After exercise, it does
1:37:54
great things for your fat. As does
1:37:57
cloth, though? Let's leave that out for
1:37:59
a moment. Oh boy. Oh boy, I'm
1:38:01
striking all the nerves here today. All
1:38:03
right. You may be quite right. I
1:38:06
wanted to stick with the GPLD1 and
1:38:08
Iresen to make the point that they
1:38:10
also go up in all of the
1:38:12
slow-aging mice. That is, all the anti-aging
1:38:15
drugs, the calistricted diet, the isalusian restricted
1:38:17
diet, and five different single gene mutation.
1:38:19
that extend life span in mice, they
1:38:21
all elevate GPLD1. 17 Alpha Estridal? Yes.
1:38:24
Can Igaflosen? Both sexes? Well this is
1:38:26
the key question. I recent is sex-specific
1:38:28
GPL1 is in both sexes. This is
1:38:31
how one begins to answer that question.
1:38:33
This is the exact kind of question
1:38:35
one has to ask. So if you
1:38:37
are interested in the idea that exercise
1:38:40
regimes have a benefit beyond the obvious
1:38:42
exercise link physiological declines of age, Do
1:38:44
they improve cognition? And if so, how
1:38:46
these molecular changes are the things you
1:38:49
need to begin to investigate. The anti-aging
1:38:51
studies in mice show that the anti-aging
1:38:53
drugs, at least the ones we've looked
1:38:55
at so far, increase the same things
1:38:58
that exercise does. Rich, have you done
1:39:00
this experiment with an ITP cohort where
1:39:02
you run in addition to a drug
1:39:05
parallel? You know what I'm going to
1:39:07
ask? Well, you're going to ask if
1:39:09
we exercise our mice. Yes. Yeah, we've
1:39:11
never done that. So you haven't done
1:39:14
a sedentary versus exercise. You have not
1:39:16
done that. You haven't done a obesogenic
1:39:18
versus fasted. We never use obesogenic diets.
1:39:20
It's worth doing. The IDP doesn't do
1:39:23
it. We don't have the resources. We
1:39:25
have enough resources to test about five
1:39:27
drugs a year, but if we wanted
1:39:30
to test them in exercise versus non-exercised.
1:39:32
We got to get you a budget
1:39:34
increase because that will now get to
1:39:36
this question, because now we could look
1:39:39
at the soluble. Maybe it would, maybe
1:39:41
it wouldn't. In mice. I'm very agnostic
1:39:43
about what we can learn from exercising
1:39:45
mice, because mice are basically kept in
1:39:48
a jail cell their entire life. If
1:39:50
you took a bunch of people and
1:39:52
put an exercise wheel and a gel
1:39:54
cell, that we'd use it, would that
1:39:57
be the same? Would that substitute for
1:39:59
people that walk around to go inside,
1:40:01
they go outside, they go to the
1:40:04
gym, that do this? It wouldn't substitute
1:40:06
for all of it, no question. So
1:40:08
to me, it's a very low level
1:40:10
of exercise. If you didn't see anything
1:40:13
from it, then you wouldn't rule it
1:40:15
up. Right. So the molecular, testable, molecular
1:40:17
hypotheses that link the biology of aging
1:40:19
to anti-aging drugs and to exercise and
1:40:22
teasing out how those are interrelated and
1:40:24
which of your exercise regimes increase I
1:40:26
recent, increased GPLD1 and increased neurogenesis. That's
1:40:29
a research agenda that could be very
1:40:31
valuable. And then if you want to
1:40:33
screen drugs in people to see which
1:40:35
ones... deserve expensive long-term testing. The ones
1:40:38
that raised GPLD1, IRISN, and some aspect
1:40:40
of neurological function in addition to the
1:40:42
good stuff they're doing for the muscles.
1:40:44
That's an approach. I agree completely and
1:40:47
this gets back to what we were
1:40:49
talking about before with the epigenetic changes
1:40:51
is if you had a mechanistic connection,
1:40:53
which is what Rich is drawing there,
1:40:56
not only this is correlated with this
1:40:58
outcome, but here's why. We all feel
1:41:00
a lot more confident that this is
1:41:03
real that this is real. that it's
1:41:05
important, and especially if that mechanistic connection
1:41:07
is preserved in people. Do any of
1:41:09
you believe that G.L.P.1 agonists are geo-protective?
1:41:12
I'm super interested in that question. Yeah,
1:41:14
I don't know. We need to find
1:41:16
that out. They look good. I think
1:41:18
there's two parts, though. Are they geropotective?
1:41:21
I'm taking the first as a given.
1:41:23
Okay. Yeah. That's a different question is,
1:41:25
is chronic caloric restriction beneficial in normal
1:41:27
weight people? But most people taking GOP1
1:41:30
agonists aren't normal. Yes, yes. And I
1:41:32
think it's impossible at this point because
1:41:34
the studies are all done in obese
1:41:37
and patients with type 2 diabetes that
1:41:39
we can't disentangle them. So we will
1:41:41
just say that for that patient population,
1:41:43
the caloric restriction appears to be giroprotective.
1:41:46
But yes, you're right. I'm technically asking
1:41:48
the second question, which is in an
1:41:50
individual who is metabolically healthy but overweight.
1:41:52
where there's actually no evidence that weight
1:41:55
loss per se is necessary outside of
1:41:57
maybe some edge cases in orthopedic stuff.
1:41:59
Is there a giroprotective nature to this?
1:42:02
And where it's most talked about is
1:42:04
in dementia prevention right now. That's
1:42:06
where it's at least most complicated to
1:42:08
tease that out. So what do
1:42:10
you guys think? And it clearly
1:42:13
has neurological effects as effects on
1:42:15
addiction. The dementia connection is not
1:42:17
inconceivable. Right. Right. Right. I mean, Rich, this
1:42:19
is one for you to test. Yeah, why hasn't
1:42:21
the ITP tested this yet, Rich? Is it because
1:42:24
the oral ones are just not strong enough and
1:42:26
we want to... Yes. Can you break your
1:42:28
protocol? Because it's enormously laborious to
1:42:30
do weekly injections. That sounds like
1:42:32
an I need more money problem.
1:42:34
And also you need a separate
1:42:36
control group. That sounds like a
1:42:38
I need more money problem. You
1:42:40
get sham injections and are, yes,
1:42:42
if you increase our budget dramatically,
1:42:44
I think it's a worthwhile experiment.
1:42:46
But what we're waiting for is oral
1:42:49
drugs that work that you don't have
1:42:51
to do injections of drugs. I mean,
1:42:53
there is an oral semi-glutide formulation that's
1:42:55
taken daily. It was submitted to
1:42:58
us this. technically very laborious. Each
1:43:00
mouse has to be food deprived for
1:43:02
six hours. Then the material is administered
1:43:04
and then they have to have a
1:43:06
change in their water balance for the
1:43:09
next two hours. It is technically not
1:43:11
an injection, but it is not any less
1:43:13
laborious and in addition you have
1:43:15
to have your own separate control
1:43:17
group that gets all of those
1:43:20
different manipulations with a sham injection.
1:43:22
Could you do three instead of
1:43:24
five next year and make that
1:43:26
one of them? reallocate some funding?
1:43:28
Well, I'm not in charge. It's a heavy
1:43:30
lift. I'd vote against it. I would vote
1:43:32
for waiting about a year until somebody comes
1:43:34
up with a pill that you can just
1:43:36
mix into mouse food or water and give
1:43:38
it to the mice and it'll work.
1:43:40
And these are going to be mice
1:43:43
that are an incredible amount of stress
1:43:45
from all the handling, the injections. Yeah,
1:43:47
that's why the control group is necessary,
1:43:49
but the companies are putting so much
1:43:51
money into this. They understand why people
1:43:53
don't like to inject like to inject
1:43:55
to inject themselves. But I'm reasonably sure that
1:43:57
in a year or two there'll be some...
1:44:00
agent that works when you put it
1:44:02
in the food of a mouse or
1:44:04
pop it as a pill as a
1:44:06
person, those would be enormously important
1:44:09
to test. Do we know
1:44:11
if terzepotide, for instance, if
1:44:13
we're given to people of normal
1:44:15
body weight, do they also lose
1:44:17
15% of their body weight? I
1:44:19
have not seen the data on that.
1:44:21
I can tell you anecdotally, having
1:44:24
seen patients, it's going to
1:44:26
be dose dependent. So as you
1:44:28
know that drug is dosed from as low
1:44:31
as two and a half milligrams weekly to
1:44:33
as much as 15 milligrams
1:44:35
weekly? Usually people who don't
1:44:37
need to lose much weight, someone
1:44:39
who says, look, I just want to
1:44:41
lose this last 10 pounds and I've
1:44:43
done all the exercising and dieting I
1:44:46
can do, they typically just lose
1:44:48
that 10 pounds and they take a
1:44:50
very low dose. Now to your point, if
1:44:52
they took the 15 milligrams, would
1:44:54
they become sarcopenic? I don't know. how
1:44:56
constraining lack of resources are. I mean,
1:44:58
there are probably like 15 or 20.
1:45:01
And I mean, every time I hear
1:45:03
Rich talk about this stuff, it just
1:45:05
pisses me off. Because there's a bunch
1:45:07
of stuff that should be tested, should
1:45:09
have been tested by now, that hasn't
1:45:11
been tested, not because it's not a
1:45:14
good idea, but because there just isn't
1:45:16
any resources to do it. Well,
1:45:18
I think what's really frustrating as
1:45:20
well is that. These are the
1:45:22
types of experiments that would allow
1:45:24
us to actually start to economically
1:45:26
model the impact of these drugs
1:45:28
outside of just kind of a
1:45:30
disease state. For example, if
1:45:32
drugs like these are indeed neuroprotective
1:45:35
and people can work three years
1:45:37
longer or five years longer because
1:45:40
they're healthier, think of the impact
1:45:42
on that over at OMB. What does that
1:45:44
mean to tax take? What does that
1:45:46
mean to delaying Medicare? What does that
1:45:48
mean to reduce health care spending at
1:45:51
the time when it is most expensive?
1:45:53
So last estimate I saw was 38
1:45:55
trillion a year for every year of
1:45:57
health span. Wow. That was a McKenzie.
1:46:00
That's 38? I'll send you the link.
1:46:02
Not 3.8? No, 38. That's analysis by
1:46:04
Andrew Scott, his British economist. That's bigger
1:46:06
than I would have guessed. Wow. Can
1:46:08
we just, because I'm in the mood
1:46:10
to see you get spicy, can we just talk
1:46:12
about senescence for a minute? Senescent cells
1:46:15
he means, Rich. You know, the things
1:46:17
that drive aging. What do you mean?
1:46:19
Do you want me to talk about senescent
1:46:21
cells? Okay, yes, I'll be glad to do
1:46:23
that. It's a terrible historical
1:46:26
historical historical accident. Leonard Hayflick
1:46:28
way back found that human
1:46:30
cells would only divide 50 times
1:46:32
and stop. One of his colleagues, a
1:46:34
guy named Victoria Defending, made a joke
1:46:36
at lunch and said to him, hey
1:46:38
Len, maybe they're getting a hold. Ha ha
1:46:41
ha ha. And Len did not understand
1:46:43
it was a joke. He thought it
1:46:45
was a serious scientific hypothesis. It's clearly
1:46:47
nuts because we don't get old in
1:46:49
a way that it is modeled by
1:46:51
having embryonic lung fiber blasts stop
1:46:53
growing. But at the time, the hottest
1:46:56
technique in modern medicine was you could
1:46:58
grow cells in culture. I was really
1:47:00
so cool. You could do stuff with
1:47:02
them. So all the cell biologists who
1:47:05
really wanted to use the cool as
1:47:07
new toys wanted to have a way
1:47:09
of studying aging without all these messy
1:47:11
mice and rats and having to wait
1:47:13
and stuff. They could do it in
1:47:15
vitro, because this was in vitro aging.
1:47:17
This is in vitro senescence. And
1:47:19
the field to skip 30 or 40 years.
1:47:21
The field went ahead with this metaphor.
1:47:23
without ever questioning it. It's now such
1:47:26
an industry that the people who review
1:47:28
these grants and papers and advise billionaires
1:47:30
and advise startup companies, they all were
1:47:32
trained in labs that just do senescence
1:47:34
for a living so they never stopped
1:47:37
to question. One of the most famous
1:47:39
and best scientists in this area
1:47:41
is a woman named Judy Campisi. She
1:47:43
recently passed away. She and I were
1:47:46
assistant professors together at Boston University. She
1:47:48
and I were going to send in
1:47:50
a program project with a third-person Barbara
1:47:52
Gilcrest. I was going to study immunity in
1:47:54
aging, Barbara's going to study skin cells,
1:47:57
we talked, Judy, you want to study
1:47:59
cells in essence. So she read the literature,
1:48:01
she came back to us and she said,
1:48:03
it has nothing to do with aging.
1:48:05
I mean, it's good cell biology, it's
1:48:08
good about cancer biology, but of course
1:48:10
there's nothing to do with aging.
1:48:12
And we told Judy, of course it
1:48:14
has nothing to do with aging. And
1:48:16
we told Judy, of course it has
1:48:18
nothing to do with aging. We understand
1:48:21
that, but the reviewers think it is
1:48:23
aging. So if you can just keep a
1:48:25
straight face for the three hours of
1:48:27
the site visit, when she moved to
1:48:29
Berkeley. She took her grant with her, and
1:48:31
after a year or two,
1:48:33
she had apparently convinced herself
1:48:35
that it was aging. It was close
1:48:38
enough to aging. So the
1:48:40
notion that aging is due
1:48:42
to senescent cell accumulation is bad
1:48:44
for two reasons. It's a
1:48:47
grotesque oversimplification. The evidence for
1:48:49
this is awful. But even worse,
1:48:51
it again cuts off productive
1:48:53
thinking. There almost certainly are changes
1:48:55
that occur in some glial cells in
1:48:58
the brains that as you get older,
1:49:00
they start making bad cytokines is bad
1:49:02
for your brain. There probably are changes
1:49:05
in some bone marrow cells or some
1:49:07
cells in the lineage that leads to
1:49:09
the beta cells in the pancreas that
1:49:11
lose the ability to divide. And that's
1:49:13
bad for you. And finding out how
1:49:15
it happens is really important. But
1:49:18
once you convince yourself, that's all
1:49:20
the same thing. This cytokine, this
1:49:22
sort of proliferation. This change in
1:49:25
ability to make specific fibrous
1:49:27
connective tissue, let's call that
1:49:29
senescence. It's the same thing. You've
1:49:31
lost what you need to
1:49:33
think of good, careful,
1:49:36
well-defined experiments with well-defined
1:49:38
endpoints. If you say that senescent,
1:49:40
there is a thing called a
1:49:42
senescent cell, the thing that's happening
1:49:44
in this gli, and in this
1:49:46
marrow cell, and this pancreas, it's
1:49:48
due to the senescent cell accumulating.
1:49:51
You've blocked off productive
1:49:53
generation of research hypotheses. The
1:49:55
last point I'll mention in this
1:49:58
rant has to do... with senelitic
1:50:00
drugs. So the ITP was asked
1:50:03
to test an allegedly senelitic
1:50:05
drug called Faisitan. It was
1:50:07
given to us by someone who
1:50:09
is using this now for
1:50:11
clinical trials and who has
1:50:13
a company that's interested in
1:50:15
senelitic drugs. So we gave it to
1:50:17
mice. It had no beneficial effect whatsoever.
1:50:20
What's the mechanism of this
1:50:22
drug's action? Has no action or
1:50:24
had no effect? It had no effect.
1:50:26
Okay. What is it supposed to do?
1:50:29
It was supposed to kill senescent cells
1:50:31
or something. So we told this
1:50:33
guy sorry it had no effect.
1:50:35
He said, well, let's prove that
1:50:38
whether it had any change in senescent
1:50:40
cells. So we gave him
1:50:42
blind tissues from each of
1:50:44
the treated and untreated mice and
1:50:46
he tried a test and there
1:50:49
were no changes in senescent cells.
1:50:51
So then he said, well, send the brain
1:50:53
and the liver and the muscle, maybe
1:50:55
the senescent cells have been changed in
1:50:57
the brain, so we send blind samples
1:51:00
to a colleague of his. There were
1:51:02
no changes in senescent cells by any
1:51:04
of the markers that these folks looked
1:51:06
at. So this drug, which is now
1:51:08
being marketed in clinical trials, and you
1:51:11
can buy it, I'm sure. It's a
1:51:13
natural product. There's no evidence as far
1:51:15
as I know that it... either has
1:51:17
an anti-aging effect or removes senescent
1:51:20
cells. But once you've got a
1:51:22
commercial company pushing this stuff, and your
1:51:24
whole brand, your whole lab, your whole
1:51:26
program project, and all the people
1:51:28
who are reviewing you are convinced
1:51:30
senescent cells exist, they're bad and
1:51:32
drugs can kill them, it's a
1:51:34
snowball rolling downhill and a rant of
1:51:37
the sort of just delivered has no impact
1:51:39
on the field. So can I
1:51:41
give a counter example? Because there's
1:51:43
good experimental data. that these things can
1:51:46
be at least partially eliminated and
1:51:48
when you do that there's an
1:51:50
improvement in health and this has
1:51:52
been done both in a genetic
1:51:54
treatment which genetically would say prime
1:51:56
these cells to be genetically killed
1:51:59
and it's also been done with drugs,
1:52:01
not with FISA, I hasten to say.
1:52:03
So I think there's strong evidence
1:52:05
that getting rid of these P16 positive
1:52:07
cells, which is really what it's all
1:52:10
based on, can have an improvement
1:52:12
in health and in longevity. Is the
1:52:14
Van Derson paper you're talking about in
1:52:16
which they were allegedly to please? Yeah,
1:52:18
yeah, yeah, yeah. Let me tell you
1:52:20
about that, because I was on the
1:52:22
program project. Two papers. Okay, one was
1:52:24
with the short live mice and once.
1:52:26
Okay, so talk about the one that
1:52:29
is not the short live mice. There's
1:52:31
a paper, a famous paper, by Van
1:52:33
Derson Kirkland and several other colleagues Darren
1:52:35
Bay. Are these the guys at Mayo?
1:52:37
Yes, they would. I remember this. They've
1:52:39
left. Two of them have left, but
1:52:41
yes. They allege that they could remove
1:52:43
senescent cells by taking genetically modified mice,
1:52:46
giving them a drug. All the senescent
1:52:48
cells would go away. and the mice
1:52:50
lived longer, according to the paper. It was
1:52:52
on the cover of nature. It was on
1:52:54
the cover of nature. I remember this one.
1:52:56
I was a part of the program
1:52:59
project, so was Judy Kempisie. And
1:53:01
my job was to do the
1:53:03
lifespan experiment. We got the mice
1:53:05
from Kirkland and Vendersen. We got
1:53:08
Kempisie's mice. We got the drugs from
1:53:10
them, and we gave the drugs to the
1:53:12
mice at 18 months. And you know, they
1:53:14
had no effect on senescent cells.
1:53:16
Not one. We tried seven times. to
1:53:19
show depletion of senescent cells
1:53:21
in their mice using their
1:53:23
drug, and went zero for seven.
1:53:25
We then took the tissues,
1:53:27
blinded, and sent them to Judy's
1:53:29
lab, Judy Campesi's lab, so she
1:53:31
could measure P16 cells, but she
1:53:33
didn't know which ones were from
1:53:36
treated and which ones were untreated,
1:53:38
when we undid the code. There
1:53:40
was no effect on senescent cells
1:53:43
whatsoever. So I remained
1:53:45
somewhat skeptical. I asked Van
1:53:47
Derson. Had he measured the number of
1:53:49
senescent cells in his treated mice, no, we're
1:53:51
planning to do that. But what was the
1:53:54
phenotypic change in the mice when you did
1:53:56
this experiment? Oh, when I... I didn't want
1:53:58
to do an experiment. of lifespan
1:54:01
experiment with an alleged
1:54:03
anti-senolithic drug until I knew
1:54:05
that it was depleting senescence. So how
1:54:07
long did you treat for? I used their
1:54:10
protocol and I asked them, I asked
1:54:12
Darren Baker, what is the dose, how
1:54:14
long do you treat the mice, and
1:54:16
how long after you add the drug
1:54:18
should you wait before you detect the
1:54:20
removal of senescent cells? And his answer
1:54:23
astonishingly was, we don't know. We've
1:54:25
never looked at that. But the nature
1:54:27
mice were treated for how long. They
1:54:29
were a long time, a long time.
1:54:31
Yeah, treated, I think they started treatment
1:54:33
at middle age, right? And I mean,
1:54:36
in the published papers, they do show
1:54:38
a reduction in P16 positive cells, and
1:54:40
you're saying you couldn't replicate that in
1:54:42
your lab. But we're conflating a
1:54:44
bunch of different issues here. We're
1:54:46
conflating the genetic model with the
1:54:49
drugs and do-so-nescent cells even exist,
1:54:51
and I feel like, I mean,
1:54:53
I think Rich's skepticism skepticismicism is
1:54:55
valid in many ways. Whether we
1:54:57
agree on the definition of senescence,
1:55:00
what people are calling senescent cells
1:55:02
do accumulate in multiple tissues with age
1:55:04
in mice and people, and if you get
1:55:06
rid of them, you can see some health
1:55:08
benefits. Am I convinced they have big effects
1:55:10
on lifespan? No, I'm not, because the data
1:55:13
is mixed and even that genetic model other
1:55:15
people haven't been able to reproduce, so it's
1:55:17
messy. But I think partly maybe start
1:55:19
with what is the definition of a
1:55:21
senescent cell, because that's where a lot
1:55:23
of this confusion comes from. That's what
1:55:25
I was saying, that there is no
1:55:27
satisfactory definition. Satisfactory to you? I mean,
1:55:29
is your issue rich that we talk
1:55:31
about it like it's one cell? Yeah,
1:55:33
that's a big part of it. You
1:55:35
can't think about it clearly if you
1:55:37
imagine that these many, many different kinds
1:55:40
of cell intrinsic changes with
1:55:42
potential pathological impacts. are all aspects
1:55:44
of the same phenomenon. But we
1:55:46
do that with other things. We
1:55:48
have mitochondrial dysfunction. There's lots of
1:55:50
different ways to get to mitochondrial
1:55:52
dysfunction. So the NIH has just
1:55:54
put about $600 million into a
1:55:56
network of researchers to study cells
1:55:58
in essence. And I'm on the advice. group
1:56:00
for that and to the extent that
1:56:02
Rich is saying these are many
1:56:04
many different things all pretending to
1:56:06
be the same thing that's clearly
1:56:08
true but they're coming up
1:56:10
with bigger and bigger and broader
1:56:13
definitions of what a senescent
1:56:15
cell is but on the other
1:56:17
hand they're also coming up with
1:56:19
more more interesting things that those senescent
1:56:21
cells do either in tissue culture
1:56:23
which I don't put much or in mice
1:56:26
I don't think the NIH would put
1:56:28
that kind of money into something if
1:56:30
they didn't feel there was a valid
1:56:32
basis. I think part of this is
1:56:34
we're calling it senescence and I think
1:56:36
none of us, to me that's stolen
1:56:38
a really good word out of the
1:56:40
vocabulary. Senescent just means aging and it
1:56:42
used to be, you could talk about
1:56:44
calendar aging, you could talk about senescence
1:56:46
which is what we now think of
1:56:48
as aging and now you can't use
1:56:50
this anymore because any time you do
1:56:52
they think you're talking about these cells.
1:56:54
Is this what they call the zombie
1:56:56
cell? I keep trying to tell you.
1:56:58
I keep trying to purge that from
1:57:01
my memory. I mean, the most common
1:57:03
definition, I think, is just an irreversibly
1:57:05
arrested cell that doesn't die and
1:57:07
typically gives off a pattern of
1:57:09
inflammatory cytokines and other factors, which
1:57:11
is a catch-all for a lot
1:57:13
of different ways to get there
1:57:15
and a lot of different states that these
1:57:17
irreversibly arrested cells can exist in. Yeah,
1:57:19
but even neurons, they're not considering senescent
1:57:21
neurons, and neurons are post-mytotic. Right, but
1:57:23
they don't always give off this pattern
1:57:25
of signals, right? That's, I mean, again,
1:57:28
this is part of the problem, as
1:57:30
you mentioned, P16. I think the, even
1:57:32
at the molecular level, the catalog of
1:57:34
markers that people are using to define
1:57:36
a senescent cell is changing, and it
1:57:38
seems to change. Yeah. Yeah. Yeah. Yeah.
1:57:40
I agree with much of what you're
1:57:42
saying, I just don't think we should.
1:57:44
throw the baby out with the bathwater
1:57:46
here and say there's nothing to this. I
1:57:48
think there is something to it and I think
1:57:50
there's lots of evidence that are there enough
1:57:52
similarities between all the different classes of
1:57:54
senescent cells that people are studying now
1:57:56
that they should be categorized as one
1:57:58
thing? I think that's a... valid conversation
1:58:01
to have. It's a good discussion point.
1:58:03
I don't think we know the answer.
1:58:05
And they discussed this a lot in
1:58:07
the synect, because even the sasp, even
1:58:09
these things that are oozing out of
1:58:12
the cells, varies quite a bit, depending
1:58:14
on the nature of the cell. That's
1:58:16
the problem, of course. You referred
1:58:18
to it, as almost anyone would, as
1:58:21
the sasp, the setup, the set-ups, and
1:58:23
essence associated proteins, secretory proteins.
1:58:25
And once you think of it as
1:58:27
the sasp, you've lost. Because the key
1:58:30
point is not to do that. The
1:58:32
key point is, here's a set of
1:58:34
set of kinds that this cell has
1:58:36
begun to make. That's really interesting. Here's
1:58:38
another set, overlapping probably. They make it
1:58:41
when you've made them stop dividing for
1:58:43
a separate reason. That's interesting. We should
1:58:45
study that. But to think you've proven
1:58:47
something about this cell type when you've
1:58:49
actually been looking at this cell type
1:58:52
because the sasp has been changed. But
1:58:54
do you think it's possible that a drug
1:58:56
such as rapomycin has part of
1:58:58
its effect? on aging through a
1:59:00
broad inhibition of a subset of
1:59:02
the sasps? I think it's very
1:59:05
likely that rapomice and changes cytokine
1:59:07
production by many different cell types
1:59:09
and that some of those changes probably
1:59:11
have health benefits. I would like
1:59:13
to know what it does to
1:59:15
the cytokine production from the macrophages
1:59:17
in the fat and the glial
1:59:19
cells in the brain and cells
1:59:21
that are in charge of protecting
1:59:23
you from viral infections. But the mistake
1:59:25
is to say, yes, it's affecting the
1:59:28
SAS. It's easy to see an analogy. If
1:59:30
I said, here's a drug and
1:59:32
it helps you because it affects
1:59:34
neurons, you'd laugh at me. Because
1:59:36
what you really want to know
1:59:38
is, is it motor neurons, sympathetic
1:59:40
neurons, parasympathetic neurons, neurons, and your
1:59:42
hypothalamus, what part of the hypothalamus,
1:59:44
the ones that control appetite? And
1:59:46
I said, no, no, no, it
1:59:48
affects neurons. I've got a drug
1:59:50
that affects neurons. But I mean,
1:59:53
people are aware of these complications
1:59:55
and are studying these complications now.
1:59:57
It seems to me that it's
1:59:59
the terminology. you object to and I
2:00:01
can appreciate that. It's thinking that I
2:00:03
object to you. The terminology is problematic
2:00:05
because it makes people stop thinking about
2:00:08
the important details and start imagining
2:00:10
that they've had a thought when
2:00:12
they say I have a drug that removes
2:00:14
senescent cells. The problem is that the
2:00:16
words trap you into patterns of thought
2:00:18
that are in this case non-productive and
2:00:20
misleading. Maybe inefficient, but the field is making,
2:00:22
I would say, quite a bit of progress.
2:00:24
And I think the way you learn about
2:00:26
the complexities, you start with a simple model,
2:00:28
you study it, and then your model gets
2:00:30
more complicated. So I totally get the frustration
2:00:33
rich, because I get as frustrated as you
2:00:35
are about senescent cells about other things. But
2:00:37
I think this is also part of the natural process
2:00:39
here. And I think what Steve said is really
2:00:41
important. The fraction of the NIH budget
2:00:43
that goes to study the biology of aging
2:00:45
of aginging through NIA has remained tiny. But
2:00:47
senescent cells are actually a really good
2:00:50
example of how a bunch of people
2:00:52
in other institutes are studying aging,
2:00:54
and they don't even know it. They're
2:00:56
studying senescence in cancer, or senescence in
2:00:58
kidney disease. So it actually has had
2:01:01
an impact in broadening the appeal and
2:01:03
scope of the field outside of NIA
2:01:05
in ways that I certainly didn't anticipate.
2:01:07
Do you think that going back to the
2:01:09
meta problem at the beginning of our discussion,
2:01:12
do you think that's maybe a better way
2:01:14
to think about... Allocating funds.
2:01:16
So for example, the NCI
2:01:18
obviously receives the most funding
2:01:20
within NIH. Maybe some of the
2:01:23
NCI funding goes to the NCI
2:01:25
to study cancer prevention through geo
2:01:28
protection. Right? If the turf war
2:01:30
is what matters. That's a good idea.
2:01:33
No, no, no. We've actually, a group
2:01:35
of us who are lobbying Congress have
2:01:37
actually asked the NIH to
2:01:39
tell us exactly this. How much
2:01:42
work in geroscience is going on?
2:01:44
in all these other institutes. Of
2:01:46
course, they're going to have
2:01:48
some motivation to minimize that or
2:01:50
maximize it or something, but at least
2:01:52
it will give us an idea. Right
2:01:54
now we have no idea how much
2:01:56
of the NCI budget is going to
2:01:58
this or NIDDK or... anything else. They
2:02:01
already have produced a report that told
2:02:03
us how much they were spending in the
2:02:05
NIA, but we already knew that. We
2:02:07
wanted to know how much they're spending
2:02:10
in the other institutes. I mean, I
2:02:12
think that could alleviate some of the
2:02:14
turf war issues, but I think what
2:02:16
you really need is the change in
2:02:18
leadership and leaders who actually recognize why
2:02:20
this is important. And that's where it
2:02:23
starts. We can have a conversation about
2:02:25
how much power does the NIH positions
2:02:27
who get it. It's going to have
2:02:29
an impact. Let's talk a little bit about Metformin. Rich,
2:02:31
do you think Metformin is geo-protective? In humans, I
2:02:34
know it doesn't appear to be in your mice. I think the
2:02:36
evidence is uncertain. There's a famous paper from Bannister that alleged that
2:02:38
diabetics on Metformin had lower mortality risks than age-like. You don't listen
2:02:40
to my podcast, do you don't listen to my podcast? I do,
2:02:42
actually, no, no, you know, you know, you know, it was a
2:02:44
different, it was a different, it was a different, it was
2:02:46
a different, it was a different, it was a different, it
2:02:48
was a different, it was a different, it was a different,
2:02:50
it was a different, it, it was a different, it, it
2:02:53
was a different, it, it, it, it was, it was, it
2:02:55
was, it was, it was, it was, it was, it was,
2:02:57
it was, it was, it was, it was, I did a
2:02:59
very lengthy treatise in a journal club comparing the Bannister paper
2:03:01
to the Keys paper and came to the conclusion that the
2:03:04
Bannister paper had too many methodology flaws to be valid. That's
2:03:06
exactly what I was going to say. As a matter of
2:03:08
fact, Keys, Christensen, who's the senior member of the group, and
2:03:10
I have just written a review article, which says exactly that.
2:03:12
You've just, that's the title of one paper. Is it
2:03:14
out yet? That's under review. Okay. Yes. So you
2:03:16
know exactly what I know exactly what I was going
2:03:18
to what I was going to what I was going to
2:03:21
what I was going to say. The question is
2:03:23
to whether Metformin would be
2:03:25
giroprotective, that is... In a
2:03:27
non-diabetic. In humans, I think,
2:03:30
is interesting, unanswered. It's not
2:03:32
the drug. I would have looked at
2:03:34
myself, if I had a big set
2:03:36
of dogs, for instance, and I wanted
2:03:38
to give them a drug that modified
2:03:40
their glucose homeostasis, I
2:03:42
would probably start with
2:03:45
something like canaglithosin that actually
2:03:47
does work in mice, which is known
2:03:49
to be safe over the long term.
2:03:51
in people. Metform and is safe over
2:03:53
the long term in people, but I
2:03:55
don't think there's much evidence that it's
2:03:57
anti-aging, leaving aside how great it is
2:03:59
for... What do you think Steve? I
2:04:01
think it's very promising. I'm skeptical
2:04:03
because I'm always skeptical in the
2:04:05
absence of evidence, but the observational
2:04:07
evidence, ignoring the banister paper, just
2:04:09
the consistency of the observational data,
2:04:11
that it reduces dementia, cancer, cardiovascular
2:04:14
disease, suggests to me there's enough
2:04:16
smoke there to look to see
2:04:18
if there's fire or not. I'll
2:04:20
send you the keys review article
2:04:22
and then you can rethink that.
2:04:24
But sorry Steve, you're saying it
2:04:26
does all of those things in
2:04:28
diabetics. Well, most of the studies
2:04:30
have been done in diabetics, absolutely.
2:04:32
And how much of that is
2:04:34
just because you're carrying the diabetes
2:04:36
is an open question. And how
2:04:38
much of that is a selection
2:04:40
for people in diabetes that are
2:04:42
progressing much less slowly because they're
2:04:44
the ones that stay on a
2:04:46
single agent as opposed to the
2:04:48
ones that progress into... Right, which
2:04:50
is why you have to do
2:04:52
the study. Yeah. Where is Tame
2:04:54
in the world of... Tame is
2:04:56
in a very preliminary state. There's
2:04:58
now enough money to get it
2:05:00
started. It has not enrolled? Anything
2:05:02
else? It's enrolling right now. Okay.
2:05:04
Previously they didn't want to start
2:05:06
it until they had enough money
2:05:08
to do the whole thing. It's
2:05:10
been impossible to get that. But
2:05:12
yeah, it's been around for eight
2:05:14
years now. And I was in
2:05:16
on the original discussion about, do
2:05:18
we do rapid mice and do
2:05:20
we do met foremen? And it
2:05:22
was all about cost and safety.
2:05:24
That was the whole thing. I
2:05:26
went in strongly advocating for rapid
2:05:28
mice and I came out saying,
2:05:30
okay, there are these cost issues.
2:05:32
And I think it was important
2:05:34
because when we went to the
2:05:36
FDA, I didn't want them to
2:05:38
think that we were trying to
2:05:40
make a bunch of money with
2:05:42
this trial and nobody's going to
2:05:44
get rich. from Metform. Why is
2:05:46
generic sorolum is so expensive still?
2:05:48
I think it's supply and demand,
2:05:50
honestly. If there was demand, yeah,
2:05:52
I think so. But coming back
2:05:54
to the Metform in question, I
2:05:56
mean, I think, first of all,
2:05:58
we don't know the answer. I
2:06:00
mean, Rich is right. We don't
2:06:02
know. So... What are our opinions?
2:06:04
My opinion is diabetes probably accelerates
2:06:06
biological aging and Metformin is effective
2:06:08
at reducing diabetic symptoms and probably
2:06:10
reduces biological aging in that context.
2:06:12
Probably doesn't. And people who are
2:06:14
not diabetic. That's my intuition. Let
2:06:16
me push back on that for
2:06:18
a second, which is diabetes is
2:06:20
an artificial diagnosis in that we
2:06:22
just make a cutoff. We say
2:06:24
you're hemoglobin A1C is 6.5. But
2:06:26
there are data that we've looked
2:06:28
at that suggest a monotonic improvement
2:06:30
in all-cause mortality as average blood
2:06:32
glucose goes down, measured by hemoglobin
2:06:34
A1C, in the non-diabetic range. Meaning,
2:06:36
people with an A1C of 5
2:06:38
live longer than people with an
2:06:40
A1C of 6, all of whom
2:06:42
are non-diabetic. Point being, if Metformins
2:06:44
gero protection comes through the regulation
2:06:46
of glucose in the patient with
2:06:48
diabetes, Does it stand to reason
2:06:50
that even in patients without diabetes
2:06:52
further attenuation of hepatic glucose output
2:06:54
is going to improve all-cause mortality?
2:06:56
Maybe. I don't know the answer,
2:06:58
obviously. I think the question is,
2:07:00
is the biomarker in this case
2:07:02
A1C, what is that actually reflecting?
2:07:04
Is that presumably reflecting some aspect
2:07:06
of metabolic homeostasis? And so, first
2:07:08
of all, does metformin in non-diabetics
2:07:10
have the desired effect or the...
2:07:12
effect we would associate with reduced
2:07:14
mortality in non-diabetics consistently, would be
2:07:16
question number one. I don't know
2:07:18
the answer to that, you probably
2:07:20
do. And I don't want to
2:07:22
speak for near, because it's been
2:07:24
a while since we've spoken, but
2:07:27
the last time I had near
2:07:29
on the podcast, his rationale for
2:07:31
why Metformin was neuroprotective, had nothing
2:07:33
to do with glucose homeostasis and
2:07:35
a non-diabetic. It was, and I
2:07:37
know you're going to love this,
2:07:39
I mean Rich, you're really going
2:07:41
to love this, there, you're really
2:07:43
going to love this. But my
2:07:45
point being not to say that
2:07:47
that's incorrect correct or anything it's
2:07:49
that there was something much more
2:07:51
primal about Metformin's actions. Now, here's
2:07:53
my pushback on that. Metformin requires
2:07:55
an organic cation transporter to get
2:07:57
into cells, as I've learned somewhat
2:07:59
recently, that muscles don't have. So,
2:08:01
if you look at the tracer
2:08:03
studies, Metformin does not get into
2:08:05
muscles. It gets into enterocytes and
2:08:07
the liver. It's very concentrated in
2:08:09
the liver. Gets in the gut.
2:08:11
Unclear from these tracer studies, if
2:08:13
it's getting into immune cells. So
2:08:15
Navcendel tells me that he believes
2:08:17
they are getting into immune cells
2:08:19
as well. So the question is,
2:08:21
at least I think we need
2:08:23
to ask ourselves the question, if
2:08:25
it's working, which cells is it
2:08:27
working on and how? And so
2:08:29
the liver part's easy. Everybody gets
2:08:31
big concentration of metformin shows up
2:08:33
here. We sort of understand that
2:08:35
that reduces hepatic glucose output. After
2:08:37
that, I'm sort of scratching my
2:08:39
head going, I don't know how
2:08:41
it works. Well, we know it
2:08:43
has a target in the mitochondrial
2:08:45
complex one, it inhibits, we know
2:08:47
it affects. Well, that's my point,
2:08:49
like it's not in the muscle.
2:08:51
That's a question. And we also
2:08:53
know that it activates AMPK. But
2:08:55
those mechanisms are probably related. This
2:08:57
is why Neur points at two
2:08:59
of the hallmarks. I just have
2:09:01
to tell you this. But here's
2:09:03
interesting thing. Good friend of ours,
2:09:05
George Martin, George Martin, George Martin,
2:09:07
who died a couple of ours,
2:09:09
George Martin, who died a couple
2:09:11
of a couple of a couple
2:09:13
of a couple of a couple
2:09:15
of years ago. once went through
2:09:17
and catalogued all the human diseases
2:09:19
he could and tried to look
2:09:21
at the similarities of their phenotypic
2:09:23
changes relative to what happens with
2:09:25
normal aging. He came up with
2:09:27
diabetes as having the most similarities
2:09:29
to accelerated aging of any of
2:09:31
the groups that he looked at
2:09:33
which in this sentence the glycosylation,
2:09:35
the hyper growth factors like insulin,
2:09:37
IGF1, all these things. I mean,
2:09:39
there's logic to that. Let me
2:09:41
agree with the... emphasis you were
2:09:43
just putting on organ specific and
2:09:45
tissue specific changes and I think
2:09:47
it's about time to get away
2:09:49
from what does Metforman do to
2:09:51
the body or any of these
2:09:53
drugs for that matter and start
2:09:55
to think what does it do
2:09:57
to each of the interesting players
2:09:59
and how they talk to one
2:10:01
another. Someone in my lab has
2:10:03
been looking at the enzymes related
2:10:05
to de novo lipogenesis, and she's
2:10:07
been looking at a couple of
2:10:09
different kinds of slow aging mice,
2:10:11
and it has major effects in
2:10:13
the liver, and it has major
2:10:15
effects on white and brown adipose
2:10:17
tissue, and they go in different
2:10:19
directions, and which is primary, which
2:10:21
is reactive, whether any of these
2:10:23
are related to the effects of
2:10:25
the mutations on the muscle or
2:10:27
the brain, is now an open
2:10:29
question. having a diagram of hallmarks
2:10:31
which are changed by a drug
2:10:33
is much less useful than asking
2:10:35
what specific changes in what cell
2:10:37
types of which organs that talk
2:10:40
to each other are being changed
2:10:42
by this drug as a primary
2:10:44
or as a secondary or as
2:10:46
a compensatory effect. That's how you'll
2:10:48
start to get into first mechanisms,
2:10:50
but also start to be able
2:10:52
to think clearly about ways of
2:10:54
targeting therapy so that it has
2:10:56
a benefit with fewer and fewer
2:10:58
side effects. Let's use Kanaga Flosen
2:11:00
as an example. We've demonstrated, and
2:11:02
I use we very liberally here,
2:11:04
you've demonstrated that it reduces all-cause
2:11:06
mortality in your mice in males.
2:11:08
And we know exactly what Kanaga
2:11:10
Flosin does in the kidney, and
2:11:12
we know that those mice lived
2:11:14
longer. Do you believe that the
2:11:16
longevity benefit came through glycemic control?
2:11:18
Because there was no difference in
2:11:20
weight, if I recall. They actually
2:11:22
lost weight. Males and females lost
2:11:24
weight on Kanaga Flosin. Was the
2:11:26
difference in weight statistically significant between
2:11:28
the long-lived males and the normal
2:11:30
males? The mice treated with the
2:11:32
drug were lighter in weight than
2:11:34
controls and that's true of both
2:11:36
sexes. So the weight loss wasn't
2:11:38
necessarily what explained. The cat actually
2:11:40
lost more weight in females than
2:11:42
in males. So the question is
2:11:44
very valid and we do not
2:11:46
know the answer, SGL2, is on
2:11:48
many other cell types and it's
2:11:50
quite possible, very plausible. that canagglithosin
2:11:52
had an effect principally through controlling
2:11:54
peak daily blood glucose, not average,
2:11:56
but peak. And it's also possible
2:11:58
that it had effects on cells
2:12:00
of unknown origin in the brain
2:12:02
and all of these are... very
2:12:04
valid and I don't think anyone
2:12:06
knows the answer. It's well worth
2:12:08
evaluating. There are other inhibitors of
2:12:10
SGLT to an SGLT one that
2:12:12
have differential cell specificities and differential
2:12:14
effects on different cell types and
2:12:16
looking at those would help give
2:12:18
you glimpses into this question. We
2:12:20
guessed it had to do with
2:12:22
glucose but we might be... So
2:12:24
what is your intuition Steve? Going
2:12:26
back to Metformin? My intuition is
2:12:28
that it might work. I don't
2:12:30
have a strong opinion. There's enough
2:12:32
suggestive evidence that I think it's
2:12:34
worth a trial. I think that
2:12:36
if we wait until we figure
2:12:38
out exactly what each drug does
2:12:40
in each cell type, it will
2:12:42
take us forever to get any
2:12:44
therapies and in medicine, there have
2:12:46
been many, many advances that came
2:12:48
about before we understood the mechanistic
2:12:50
underpinning. And if there's enough suggestive
2:12:52
evidence and there's not a lot
2:12:54
of side effects, suggest me that
2:12:56
it's worth digging into now because
2:12:58
the benefits are so enormous. Like
2:13:00
we said, one year healthy aging,
2:13:02
38 trillion dollars. That should talk
2:13:04
to Congress if nothing else does.
2:13:06
Well, and I would also say,
2:13:08
tame could be successful independent of
2:13:10
whether Metformin is effective at slowing
2:13:12
biological aging. By getting others into
2:13:14
the field, do you mean? Well,
2:13:16
also, even just hitting the endpoints,
2:13:18
right? So the end point is
2:13:20
multimorbidity or comorbidity. So it's quite
2:13:22
possible that the trial will be
2:13:24
successful, even if Metformin is not
2:13:26
effective, gerotherapeutic. It's also true. It
2:13:28
may not succeed. for a variety
2:13:30
of reasons the clinical trials don't
2:13:32
succeed. I sort of agree with
2:13:34
Steve, like I'm supportive of doing
2:13:36
the trial. I also agree, I
2:13:38
think, with probably both Steve and
2:13:40
Rich, that it's not what I
2:13:42
would pick if I was going
2:13:44
to do one trial, if we
2:13:46
could only do one trial, but
2:13:48
we have to start somewhere. Matt,
2:13:50
why do you think that the
2:13:53
ITP studies for rapamice and always
2:13:55
worked? Because inhibiting Mtor, Increases lifespan
2:13:57
and slow as aging. I know
2:13:59
what you're at. Yeah, you know
2:14:01
where I'm going. Because most people
2:14:03
who are using rapid mice and
2:14:05
off label have moved to once
2:14:07
weekly or some sort of cycling
2:14:09
like that. So I think one
2:14:11
question is, would that increase lifespan
2:14:13
in mice as much or more
2:14:15
than daily? We don't effectively know
2:14:17
the answer to that question. I
2:14:19
don't think you guys do some
2:14:21
intermittent. It can increase lifespan, but
2:14:23
it's never been dose optimized, right?
2:14:25
I think this is the question.
2:14:27
Is the metabolic rate of the
2:14:29
mouse so fast that giving the
2:14:31
mouse daily RAPA is not the
2:14:33
same as giving the human daily
2:14:35
RAPA? Yes, and the RAPA in
2:14:37
the ITP study is in the
2:14:39
food. So it's not a single
2:14:41
dose, or it's not a single...
2:14:43
They're just chowing on it all
2:14:45
day. Well, at least during the
2:14:47
period of day that they're eating
2:14:49
and have access. I'll let Rich
2:14:51
talk about what they know about
2:14:53
the blood levels, but it is
2:14:55
a fundamentally way of delivering the
2:14:57
drug. I mean, I guess in
2:14:59
2008 or seven when you did
2:15:01
the first study, maybe it wasn't
2:15:03
clear this idea of Mtor 1
2:15:05
versus Mtor 2 and the constitutive
2:15:07
dosing. Maybe we should ask how
2:15:09
many people at this table actually
2:15:11
believe that model. Yeah, that's kind
2:15:13
of where I want to go.
2:15:15
Yeah, that's kind of where I
2:15:17
want to go. I want to
2:15:19
understand what I want to go.
2:15:21
I want to understand what I'm
2:15:23
talking about. That the bad side
2:15:25
effects come from Mtor 1. I
2:15:27
don't know enough to say. Many
2:15:29
of our slow-aging mice, actually, M-tore
2:15:31
Complex 1 function is down in
2:15:33
all of them, but M-tore Complex
2:15:35
2 is often up. And it's
2:15:37
up in an interesting way. Mice
2:15:39
eat mostly at night, and they
2:15:41
more or less fast during the
2:15:43
day. In our slow-aging mice, M-tore
2:15:45
Complex 2 is elevated, but it
2:15:47
no longer responds in the fasting
2:15:49
period, but it doesn't respond to
2:15:51
food in the same way. So
2:15:53
their complex changes. in both its
2:15:55
baseline state and its response to
2:15:57
food weatheries, what happened in people,
2:15:59
what happened in people taking it
2:16:01
every other day, every fifth day,
2:16:03
whether they are beneficial or harmful
2:16:05
or a mixture. really don't know.
2:16:07
The Emtor complex two-story is trickier.
2:16:09
The other thing is that is,
2:16:11
I think, important but not really
2:16:13
appreciated is that it's not only
2:16:15
Emtor complex one drugs like rapomice
2:16:17
and not only lower the overall
2:16:19
effect, but it also changes the
2:16:21
substrate specificity so that the kinase
2:16:23
that is susceptible to torque inhibition
2:16:25
that looks at a ribosomal protein,
2:16:27
S6, that goes down and doesn't
2:16:29
work nearly as well as inhibited.
2:16:31
For how long? I don't know.
2:16:33
But the other aspect of tour
2:16:35
downstream is on a protein called
2:16:37
40 BP1 that's involved in translation.
2:16:39
It does not change that kinase.
2:16:41
What it does is it changes
2:16:43
the total amount of the protein.
2:16:45
So the proportion of the protein
2:16:47
that's phosphorylate drops down, but the
2:16:49
actual kinase that adds the phosphate
2:16:51
to that substrate is unchanged. So
2:16:53
whether that's important, that it's having
2:16:55
at least two different pathways. that
2:16:57
are being influenced in one case
2:16:59
by changing the substrate and in
2:17:01
the other case by changing the
2:17:04
kinase. No one's really looked at
2:17:06
that. They say it's a drug
2:17:08
that blocks and torque kinase one
2:17:10
function. And downstream is where a
2:17:12
lot of the action is. I
2:17:14
know your lab at one point
2:17:16
was interested in cell type specific
2:17:18
inhibitors of the torque complex one.
2:17:20
I don't know whether you... Everything
2:17:22
you just said, rich occurs in
2:17:24
what cell? Mouse liver. What about
2:17:26
muscle? Well, the overall decline in
2:17:28
the ratio of phosphorylated versus substrate.
2:17:30
We also published that, I think,
2:17:32
in Muscle and Kidney. I would
2:17:34
have to go back to the
2:17:36
papers and see whether we also
2:17:38
found the elevation of the substrate
2:17:40
before ABP1 in both of those
2:17:42
tissues. I vaguely recall that it
2:17:44
was the substrate that changed, not
2:17:46
the kinase in those tissues as
2:17:48
well, but I'd rather look it
2:17:50
up before I sign my name
2:17:52
to it. But even what Rich
2:17:54
is saying is, wow, I mean,
2:17:56
really important and informative, also only
2:17:58
a tiny piece of all the
2:18:00
downstream things that Amtor affects, and
2:18:02
I think the point is we
2:18:04
just really don't have a... understanding
2:18:06
of how rapomycin or fasting or
2:18:08
other drugs that hit emptor are
2:18:10
affecting all of the things that
2:18:12
are downstream of. I agree completely.
2:18:14
Let me give you an example.
2:18:16
Linda Partridge just published in bio-arcove
2:18:18
at least a nice paper. Rapomycin
2:18:20
increased lifespan of her mice. If
2:18:22
she added an inhibitor of a
2:18:24
different kinase called irk, it did
2:18:26
better. The inhibition by irk worked
2:18:28
by itself, but it actually improved
2:18:30
on rapomycin. So two people in
2:18:32
my lab are looking at that.
2:18:34
And it turns out that the
2:18:36
Erk-Kainase inhibitor is working in an
2:18:38
entirely different pathway. It's affecting the
2:18:40
proteome by increasing the degradation through
2:18:42
a chavron immediate autophagee mechanism, which
2:18:44
is not affected by Raphamison. At
2:18:46
least at the dose they used,
2:18:48
right? At least at the dose
2:18:50
they used, right? At least at
2:18:52
the dose they used, right, that's
2:18:54
right. Sorry, what model was this?
2:18:56
This was mice. Nice. Yeah, it
2:18:58
was a black six. Oh, okay.
2:19:00
So it's agreeing with an amplifying
2:19:02
the question, there may well be
2:19:04
multiple cell intrinsic pathways, some of
2:19:06
which are tour-dependent, some of which
2:19:08
are map kinase, erk-dependent, which can
2:19:10
synergize, as in the Partridge case
2:19:12
for lifespan, but also potentially synergize
2:19:14
for health impact. Yeah, and here's,
2:19:16
I mean, I think an important,
2:19:18
again, limitation to what's been done.
2:19:20
There are drugs out there that's
2:19:22
been done. There are drugs out
2:19:24
there, ATP competitive inhibitors that have
2:19:26
different affinities for different types of
2:19:28
kinases. Haven't been tested for longevity.
2:19:30
These dual kinase inhibitors. In fact,
2:19:32
in the restor bio trial, the
2:19:34
last one, the phase three, which
2:19:36
did not get to completion, they
2:19:38
substituted. They took the wrapalog out
2:19:40
and used an ATP competitive drug.
2:19:42
Didn't know that. So what is
2:19:44
your belief, Matt, around? dosing wrapa
2:19:46
in humans than or even in
2:19:48
your dogs. You're doing it. We're
2:19:50
doing it once a week now.
2:19:52
We've moved to once a week.
2:19:54
So, I mean, maybe it's worth
2:19:56
at least talking about how that
2:19:58
evolved and this is my... understanding
2:20:00
of how we got to where
2:20:02
we are today, which is that
2:20:04
most people using wrapamison off label
2:20:06
for potential health span effects, most
2:20:08
doctors prescribing it, are recommending once
2:20:10
weekly dosing in the three to
2:20:12
six, sometimes eight 10 milligram range.
2:20:14
So the first place I'm aware
2:20:17
of in the literature where this
2:20:19
was showing to have a potential
2:20:21
benefit for anything related to aging
2:20:23
was Joan Manick's work. when she
2:20:25
was first at Novartis and then
2:20:27
at Restor Bio looking at flu
2:20:29
vaccine response in elderly people, and
2:20:31
they were using everlimas, so a
2:20:33
derivative of rapomycin, and they found
2:20:35
that for vaccine response, it was
2:20:37
most effective and had the least
2:20:39
side effects at once weekly dosing
2:20:41
at five milligrams, and they tested.
2:20:43
daily, one or two migs, five
2:20:45
migs once a week, 20 minutes
2:20:47
a week. Yeah, it was once,
2:20:49
it was a milligram a day,
2:20:51
five once a week, 21s a
2:20:53
week. Now I've had both Lloyd
2:20:55
Clixtine and Joan Manic on the
2:20:57
podcast. It's been so long that
2:20:59
I don't recall if I asked
2:21:01
them why. They designed the trial
2:21:03
with those forearms. So my understanding
2:21:05
is that Novartis had internal data
2:21:07
at that point on side effects
2:21:09
and had an internal hypothesis that
2:21:11
if you let the trough levels
2:21:13
bottom levels bottom out. that reduced
2:21:15
side effects. The side effects in
2:21:17
organ transplant patients were largely driven
2:21:19
high troughs. High troughs. Yep. And
2:21:21
then after that they developed, based
2:21:23
off of David Sabatini's work and
2:21:25
then Dudley Laming, after he left
2:21:27
David's lab, a hypothesis that chronic
2:21:29
treatment with rapomycin, which maybe would
2:21:31
be equivalent to daily dosing in
2:21:33
people, this was all done in
2:21:35
cells, led to off-target effects on
2:21:37
Mtor Complex 2, that were driving
2:21:39
the side effects. So that got...
2:21:41
sort of dogmatized as the truth,
2:21:43
actually don't think there's a ton
2:21:45
of evidence beyond those initial papers
2:21:47
to support the idea that the
2:21:49
side effects are all through Mtor
2:21:51
Complex too. The idea is, if
2:21:53
you dose once a week... You
2:21:55
let the trough levels bottom out.
2:21:57
You don't get the off-target effects
2:21:59
on Mtor Complex too. You avoid
2:22:01
the side effects. Again, we don't
2:22:03
have definitive data. The data I've
2:22:05
seen seem consistent with that idea.
2:22:07
People dosing daily seem to be
2:22:09
more likely to have side effects.
2:22:11
Mostly things like bacterial infections or
2:22:13
the really severe mouth stores, but
2:22:15
sort of anecdotal and I don't
2:22:17
know for sure how strong that
2:22:19
data is in people. It did
2:22:21
hold up in all of the
2:22:23
restor biocclinical clinical trials that I'm
2:22:25
aware of. really didn't show any
2:22:27
side effects different from placebo. In
2:22:29
the dog study, you're using a
2:22:31
slow release formulation. It's an enteric
2:22:33
coded. It's a different formulation than
2:22:35
what the ITP uses, but all
2:22:37
of the human cirrolumus formulations have
2:22:39
some way to get to the
2:22:41
small intestine. So it's not substantially
2:22:43
different, I don't think, than wrap
2:22:45
immune or the generic cirrolumus you
2:22:47
would get. Let's do the closest
2:22:49
thing that a group like this
2:22:51
could do in terms of a
2:22:53
speed round. I'm going to go
2:22:55
through a couple of other ideas.
2:22:57
I just want to get the,
2:22:59
what are you thinking about this?
2:23:01
Can we say anything positive about
2:23:03
risveratrol? No. Rich? No. Why does
2:23:05
this thing not die? Why is
2:23:07
there still a hundred different risveratrols
2:23:09
being sold on Amazon? Why do
2:23:11
I still get people asking me,
2:23:13
do you take resveratrol? Should I
2:23:15
be taking resveratrol? It has a
2:23:17
good PR team. I think it's
2:23:19
really hard to prove something doesn't
2:23:21
work. So once it gets in
2:23:23
the consciousness as improving health, I
2:23:25
mean, even in the longevity field,
2:23:27
Jesus Christ, I was saying the
2:23:30
resveratrol stuff was garbage for 10
2:23:32
years before people believed it. Now
2:23:34
everybody believes it. But it takes
2:23:36
a really long time. studying resveratrol
2:23:38
in the aging field anymore. Think
2:23:40
if you went to a conference
2:23:42
and asked scientists, what do you
2:23:44
think about resveratrol? You'd get the
2:23:46
same answer here with maybe one
2:23:48
exception. But I think. I think
2:23:50
it takes a really hard time.
2:23:52
Just one exception. It takes a
2:23:54
long time to change. Bad ideas
2:23:56
don't die hard. That's right. And
2:23:58
that's true in the scientific literature.
2:24:00
And it's especially true when there's
2:24:02
a profit motive to continue selling
2:24:04
this stuff. And I'm not 100%
2:24:06
convinced that there are no health
2:24:08
benefits from resveratrol. Pretty convinced there's
2:24:10
no reason to believe it affects
2:24:12
the biology of aging or is
2:24:14
a longevity drug. But I can't
2:24:16
say for sure that. Nobody would
2:24:18
ever benefit from any dose of
2:24:20
resveratrol. Yeah, but we couldn't say
2:24:22
that about anything. I agree Yeah,
2:24:24
yeah. Now we could say that
2:24:26
if you were force-fed the highest
2:24:28
fat diet in the world such
2:24:30
that your liver encroached on your
2:24:32
lungs through your diaphragm, isn't there
2:24:34
a chance rich that under that
2:24:36
situation resveratrol might help? I have
2:24:38
no idea. Wasn't that the one
2:24:40
and only one experiment that worked?
2:24:42
Yeah, the famous experiment which was
2:24:44
published as resveratrol, the first drug
2:24:46
ever found to extend mouse lifespan,
2:24:48
to extend mouse life spent. It
2:24:50
turns out that the mice die
2:24:52
because they were on a 60%
2:24:54
coconut oil diet. It's poisonous to
2:24:56
the extent that it causes the
2:24:58
liver to fill with fat and
2:25:00
compresses the thorax so that they
2:25:02
cannot inhale. Three or four papers
2:25:04
later, they published as an obscure
2:25:06
paragraph in a discussion section on
2:25:08
a paper. Pearson was the first
2:25:10
author of the second paper that,
2:25:12
oh, by the way, all these
2:25:14
mice on the coconut oil diet.
2:25:16
Finally, we've looked at them. They're
2:25:18
all dying because of lung compaction
2:25:20
due to expansion of the liver.
2:25:22
So the notion that their drug
2:25:24
had slowed aging because on the
2:25:26
60% coconut on our diet, it
2:25:28
temporarily extended lifespan was due to
2:25:30
the prevention of this extremely bizarre
2:25:32
phenomenon. I just cannot get enough
2:25:34
of that story. Let's all documented
2:25:36
in the literature. I believe I
2:25:38
know it won't. Two separate papers.
2:25:40
All right, let's have a word
2:25:42
on NAD, NR, NMMN. Steve, what
2:25:44
is your point of view on
2:25:46
this? Well, the current state of
2:25:48
evidence. I'm skeptical. It's one of
2:25:50
those things that makes a great
2:25:52
deal of conceptual sense, but the
2:25:54
evidence at this point is not
2:25:56
very compelling. And we have the
2:25:58
ITP evidence that is, I think,
2:26:00
the strongest. And there was strongest
2:26:02
negative evidence. Yes. Yes. Yeah. Just
2:26:04
make it fair. I assume that
2:26:06
people knew that I guess I
2:26:08
should. And is it your view,
2:26:10
Steve, that this stuff probably does
2:26:12
not extend lifespan? Maybe there is
2:26:14
some other health span benefit out
2:26:16
there that has just not been
2:26:18
studied. The right experiment hasn't been
2:26:20
done. It hasn't been powered. Pick
2:26:22
your favorite excuse. I think NED
2:26:24
is very, very interesting molecule. And
2:26:26
I don't think NED is very,
2:26:28
very interesting molecule. And I don't
2:26:30
think we could throw out manipulating
2:26:32
NED is something that could be
2:26:34
important for agents. I just don't
2:26:36
think the evidence is there at
2:26:38
this point. Do you think if
2:26:40
you're going to manipulate it? That,
2:26:43
I don't know. I will express
2:26:45
complete ignorance on that. Matt, what
2:26:47
is your point of view on
2:26:49
all of this? Yeah, well, I
2:26:51
think the way you framed that
2:26:53
question to Steve is indicative of
2:26:55
why it's so hard to disprove
2:26:57
something, especially when there are people
2:26:59
out there who have money to
2:27:01
make, who really want to make
2:27:03
the case that you should buy
2:27:05
this stuff. Because it's always possible
2:27:07
that there's some way that this
2:27:09
could be beneficial. Having said that,
2:27:11
NAD, like Steve said, central molecule
2:27:13
in thousands of chemical reactions of
2:27:15
chemical reactions of chemical reactions of
2:27:17
chemical reactions, really important. Good reason,
2:27:19
I don't know about good reason,
2:27:21
some reason to believe that NAD
2:27:23
homeostasis declines with age, like lots
2:27:25
of many other things, so it's
2:27:27
plausible that if you fix that,
2:27:29
you can get benefits from it.
2:27:31
The data is decidedly mixed, both
2:27:33
in the literature, preclinical literature, and
2:27:35
in people as to whether or
2:27:37
not boosting NAD increases lifespan, improves
2:27:39
health span. So I think there's
2:27:41
lots of issues. What's the most
2:27:43
positive data you would point to?
2:27:45
Well, for Lifespan, the original study
2:27:47
by Johann Auerx's lab, where they
2:27:49
started treating, I think at 20
2:27:51
months of age, was published in
2:27:53
science, I believe, showed an effect
2:27:55
that was reasonable. good size except
2:27:57
the controls were short-lived, which is
2:27:59
a different issue. Yeah, that's different
2:28:01
issue, right? There's a number of
2:28:03
cases where something was reported to
2:28:05
increase lifespan when the controls were
2:28:07
short-lived and then when the study
2:28:09
was repeated and longer-lived, you didn't
2:28:11
see an longer-lived, you didn't see
2:28:13
an effect. I don't know why
2:28:15
there was a difference between that
2:28:17
study and the ITP. But that's
2:28:19
probably the best case you can
2:28:21
point to. There's studies in C.
2:28:23
But then I think when you
2:28:25
talk about the precursors, it's even
2:28:27
more complicated than maybe boosting NAD
2:28:29
could slow aging because can you
2:28:31
get the right doses in people?
2:28:33
You talked about bioavailability. Is there
2:28:35
any difference between NMMN, NNR, Niacin,
2:28:37
nicotine, or when you take it
2:28:39
orally, the data suggests that it
2:28:41
all gets broken down to Niacin
2:28:43
in the gut? So why are
2:28:45
people taking 70-dollar NMM? Or NNR?
2:28:47
Why are people selling it? Some
2:28:49
of them are scientists. dodge that
2:28:51
question. It's complicated. I don't personally
2:28:53
believe there is enough evidence to
2:28:55
think that NED precursors as they
2:28:57
are being marketed today are likely
2:28:59
to benefit most people. Some people,
2:29:01
probably people who have conditions of
2:29:03
dysregulated NED, could get a benefit.
2:29:05
I don't think there's any difference
2:29:07
between the various molecules that are
2:29:09
being marketed right now. And there's
2:29:11
at least one study in mice
2:29:13
that giving NMMN to aged mice
2:29:15
causes kidney inflammation and potentially kidney
2:29:17
pathology. I'm not saying NMM is
2:29:19
dangerous, but when you try to
2:29:21
weigh the risk reward, if it
2:29:23
causes kidney pathology in aged mice,
2:29:25
at least at high doses, could
2:29:27
it do the same thing in
2:29:29
dogs or people? Yeah, it could.
2:29:31
And it bothers me, particularly in
2:29:33
the companion animal space, that people
2:29:35
are marketing NMM. for people's pets
2:29:37
when they know that it might
2:29:39
cause kidney disease in people's dogs
2:29:41
and cats. That's problematic to me.
2:29:43
We talked briefly about bit. See,
2:29:45
is there going to be a
2:29:47
day when the substance found in
2:29:49
the blood of someone much younger
2:29:51
than you when infused into you
2:29:53
whilst some of your old blood
2:29:56
is removed is going to, assuming
2:29:58
we figure out what frequency that
2:30:00
has to be done in fact
2:30:02
your life? Yeah, I think this
2:30:04
is an incredibly interesting question and
2:30:06
it really deserves to be investigated.
2:30:08
in detail because if it's true
2:30:10
it's a real game changer because
2:30:12
we do transfusions I mean this
2:30:14
is not exotic medicine I think
2:30:16
we very much need to know
2:30:18
whether this works the same way
2:30:20
in people and also it would
2:30:22
be nice to know how much
2:30:24
of it is due to the
2:30:26
taking young blood versus how much
2:30:28
of it is getting rid of
2:30:30
the old book but the evidence
2:30:32
from mice is very very compelling
2:30:34
it is Steve if we could
2:30:36
design the perfect experiments that would
2:30:38
try to ask these questions. Let's
2:30:40
just say we started by doing
2:30:42
just the one experiment, which was
2:30:44
the full peribiosis, so the putting
2:30:46
in the taking out, we didn't
2:30:48
try to disentangle the effect, and
2:30:50
there was no benefit in humans.
2:30:52
What would be your best hypothesis
2:30:54
as to why it would have
2:30:56
failed, assuming it was statistically powered
2:30:58
correctly, and there was no methodology
2:31:00
error. If this was a biologic
2:31:02
result, why would you think, given
2:31:04
how favorable this has been in
2:31:06
mice, it would not occur in
2:31:08
humans? products that ended up in
2:31:10
the circulation of humans was a
2:31:12
very different nature than in mice.
2:31:14
The number of things that differ
2:31:16
between humans and mice and blood
2:31:18
would be enormous. So pinning it
2:31:20
down would be. But I think
2:31:22
there probably is some reason to
2:31:24
suspect that it may work. I'm
2:31:26
very impressed. I mean if it
2:31:28
does work, this is an opportunity
2:31:30
that we had the technology to
2:31:32
do this 50 years ago, right?
2:31:34
Right. And it may not work
2:31:36
in young people. But it may
2:31:38
work in older people. I think
2:31:40
there's a lot of drugs that
2:31:42
could affect aging. That because young
2:31:44
people haven't aged as much might
2:31:46
not have minimal effect, but you
2:31:48
give it to somebody, you know,
2:31:50
50 years later, might have a
2:31:52
big effect. I find myself frustrated
2:31:54
by the question, rather than by
2:31:56
the answer, because... You've got a
2:31:58
horrible question, Asker here, Rich, is
2:32:00
the problem. I think you are
2:32:02
well above average, but this particular
2:32:04
one, I think, is illustrative, because
2:32:06
the reason like people like probiosis
2:32:08
is that they've seen it in
2:32:10
a sci-fi movie. It sounds exactly
2:32:12
like what you do in sci-fi
2:32:14
and they're flashing lights and it's
2:32:16
so sexy and it's just so
2:32:18
great and you can take the
2:32:20
blood of young virgins and give
2:32:22
it to old people and they
2:32:24
stand up and they can get
2:32:26
on. I didn't realize they had
2:32:28
to be virgins. But none of
2:32:30
that is pertinent. Pertinent is, is
2:32:32
there something that is in the
2:32:34
blood of old people that it
2:32:36
would be good to remove and
2:32:38
if so, what is it? And
2:32:40
is there something, a cell, a
2:32:42
molecule, a set of three molecules?
2:32:44
that's in the blood of young
2:32:46
people or mice that would be
2:32:48
good for you. The only virtue
2:32:50
of this parabiosis circus is to
2:32:52
suggest that, you know, the answer
2:32:54
might be yes. There might be
2:32:56
something you could remove from old
2:32:58
blood a cell or some plasma
2:33:00
molecule, and there might be something
2:33:02
good in the blood of young
2:33:04
individuals. So the challenge now is
2:33:06
to find out what those things
2:33:09
are, and then you can do
2:33:11
real-life science. Real life science is
2:33:13
not done by taking blood from
2:33:15
young people and putting it into
2:33:17
old people. That's medieval science where
2:33:19
there's a complex mixture of dozens
2:33:21
and hundreds of potential. In other
2:33:23
words, you might start with that
2:33:25
and no one thinks that if
2:33:27
you do that experiment where you
2:33:29
literally take blood out of an
2:33:31
old person and discard it and
2:33:33
take blood out of a young
2:33:35
person and put it in. and
2:33:37
you get a favorable result nobody
2:33:39
thinks that that's what's going to
2:33:41
the FDA that is the proof
2:33:43
of concept what experiments would be
2:33:45
worth you've a limited amount of
2:33:47
volunteers, doctors, and money, what experiments
2:33:49
are most informative? And in my
2:33:51
view, by far the most informative
2:33:53
experiments are, what is in the
2:33:55
blood of young mice that is
2:33:57
so good, and what is in
2:33:59
the blood of an- But I
2:34:01
don't know, would you want to
2:34:03
go on that fishing expedition until
2:34:05
you at least saw a signal?
2:34:07
Yes, people are doing it. I
2:34:09
mean, there are companies doing it,
2:34:11
and on the basic research side.
2:34:13
Of course they are. I'm asking
2:34:15
a different question, though, which is...
2:34:17
Yes, that's the only way you
2:34:19
can turn your idea into science.
2:34:21
Well, on the other hand, if
2:34:23
it has a positive effect, I
2:34:25
don't think it really matters. That's
2:34:27
something to be investigated later. My
2:34:29
thought is it's not simple. It's
2:34:31
not GDF 11, for sure. If
2:34:33
it were simple, there's enough people
2:34:35
looking at it, they would have
2:34:37
figured it out. My guess is
2:34:39
some combination. If there's something there,
2:34:41
there's some combination. I mean, why
2:34:43
can't you do both? I think
2:34:45
Peter and I are saying the
2:34:47
same thing. Would we love to
2:34:49
understand the mechanism? Yeah, absolutely. Do
2:34:51
we have to understand the mechanism?
2:34:53
Yeah, absolutely. Do we have to
2:34:55
understand the mechanism? Do we have
2:34:57
to understand the mechanism? Yeah, absolutely.
2:34:59
Do we have to understand the
2:35:01
mechanism? loves to test individual chemical
2:35:03
compounds, even sometimes ones where the
2:35:05
mechanism of action is not known.
2:35:07
And that's very sensible. We are
2:35:09
very dubious about, let's take a
2:35:11
little vis and a little of
2:35:13
that and a little of that,
2:35:15
and we're really dubious about taking,
2:35:17
let's grind up a little dubious
2:35:19
about, and a little of that,
2:35:21
a little of that, and we're
2:35:23
really dubious about taking, let's grind
2:35:25
up the asparagus, who's really dubious
2:35:27
about taking, let's grind. And we're
2:35:29
really dubious of each drug. What
2:35:31
I'm saying is that if you
2:35:33
have a very complex mixture of
2:35:35
hundreds of molecules and something happens,
2:35:37
you don't know what to do
2:35:39
next because it could be any
2:35:41
one or two or eight or
2:35:43
ten of those and you haven't
2:35:45
really decided you have troubles and
2:35:47
then with standardization with mechanistic tests
2:35:49
and with transferring to... a key
2:35:51
species like something. But my thought
2:35:53
is we still wouldn't be using
2:35:55
anesthesia if we had to wait
2:35:57
until we figured out how it
2:35:59
worked. Yeah, and it doesn't have
2:36:01
to be peribiosis. It doesn't have
2:36:03
to be taking blood from young
2:36:05
people and putting it into old
2:36:07
people, right? There are other variants
2:36:09
of this that can be done
2:36:11
clinically, and there's some evidence to
2:36:13
support things like therapeutic plasma exchange
2:36:15
or things like that. So should
2:36:17
we test it? I think so.
2:36:20
And my gut feeling is, yeah,
2:36:22
probably will have some benefits in
2:36:24
benefits in people. So if you
2:36:26
could only do one experiment, would
2:36:28
you do a plasma for recess
2:36:30
experiment? And if so, would you
2:36:32
test? The simplest one is you
2:36:34
literally just exchange old plasma for
2:36:36
albumin. That's what they're typically doing
2:36:38
in these studies. Yeah. First of
2:36:40
all, I don't know enough about
2:36:42
this area to be confident in
2:36:44
my answer, but yeah, that's probably
2:36:46
where I would look to start
2:36:48
simply because it's going to be
2:36:50
logistically easier to do from a
2:36:52
clinical trial perspective. So scientifically then,
2:36:54
the hypothesis is it's the presence
2:36:56
of something bad. Well, it's both.
2:36:58
That is worse than the absence
2:37:00
of something good. Because the albumin's
2:37:02
not going to give you the
2:37:04
young person. That's the problem with
2:37:06
that experiment to me. We don't
2:37:08
know now. We don't. If it's
2:37:10
young blood is good, old blood
2:37:12
is bad or some combination. We
2:37:14
would automatically, if we only did
2:37:16
the plasma ferasis, we would only
2:37:18
be testing part of that. I'd
2:37:20
push back on that. I think
2:37:22
we do have reason to believe
2:37:24
it's a combination of both. There's
2:37:26
data in both directions. That's why
2:37:28
I proposed starting with that experiment.
2:37:30
Yeah. I think that's again, as
2:37:32
much as anything, sure, in this
2:37:34
field. That's not as sure as
2:37:36
rapid mice and mice, but there's
2:37:38
at least evidence to support that
2:37:40
idea. Last thing I'll say you
2:37:42
asked why might it fail might
2:37:44
fail in fail in humans. the
2:37:46
peribiosis experiment itself shortens lifespan in
2:37:48
rodents. And so just the fact
2:37:50
that you're surgically connecting these animals
2:37:52
together. So it may be that
2:37:54
the benefit from peribiosis true peribiosis
2:37:56
in that context is somehow related
2:37:58
to the shortening of lifespan due
2:38:00
to the procedure. I don't think
2:38:02
that's the case because there's other
2:38:04
lines of evidence that argue against
2:38:06
that, but there may be something
2:38:08
about the procedure itself that is...
2:38:10
That increases muscle repair and improves
2:38:12
cardiac function? It just seems to
2:38:14
me that... I agree. I'm just
2:38:16
saying that may be an alternative
2:38:18
explanation for something that's limiting in
2:38:20
those mouse experiments. Just seems like
2:38:22
there's not enough time and not
2:38:24
enough money to do the work.
2:38:26
Hopefully some of that's changing. If
2:38:28
we were to do another longevity
2:38:30
roundtable next year, which is problematic
2:38:32
because this table, you guys are
2:38:34
going to have to get awfully
2:38:36
cozy, any nominations for folks you'd
2:38:38
want to invite to a longevity
2:38:40
roundtable next time? There's so many
2:38:42
people we could do this with,
2:38:44
right? And I'm guessing nobody wants
2:38:46
to give their seat up next
2:38:48
year. Let's make this table bigger.
2:38:50
I think it would be good
2:38:52
to invite Vadym Gladyship. I disagree
2:38:54
with some of what he says.
2:38:56
I think he always has something
2:38:58
interesting to say. Who's your nominee?
2:39:00
I've made some more time to
2:39:02
think about it. All right. Matt,
2:39:04
anybody? I mean, I think we
2:39:06
would all agree there are tons
2:39:08
of great people in the field.
2:39:10
I mean, I think Brian Kennedy,
2:39:12
and I think Brian is going
2:39:14
to be on your podcast in
2:39:16
an upcoming date, is somebody who
2:39:18
also thinks broadly and deeply about
2:39:20
the science and is fantastic. So
2:39:22
he would be great to have
2:39:24
great to have some differing. I
2:39:26
mean we differ. sometimes on opinions,
2:39:28
but I think more or less
2:39:30
are aligned. Be interesting to have
2:39:33
some different voices as well. All
2:39:35
right. So we think we'll do
2:39:37
another longevity roundtable around the oval
2:39:39
table? Sure. Let's do it. Let's
2:39:41
see where we are a year
2:39:43
from now. I think there's going
2:39:45
to be a lot of new
2:39:47
stuff. That's what's new in aging
2:39:49
research. Rate of progress. The derivative
2:39:51
is... Very much positive. You know
2:39:53
who else I want to throw
2:39:55
out there is Morgan Levine? I
2:39:57
think she'd be really interesting to
2:39:59
have because she while she is
2:40:01
an expert in epigenetics and biomarkers
2:40:03
I think takes a pretty clear-eyed
2:40:05
view of that space. Now is
2:40:07
Morgan at Yale still? She's at
2:40:09
Altus. She's at Altus. Yeah, okay,
2:40:11
I wasn't sure if she was
2:40:13
there full-time. Got it. Yeah, I
2:40:15
would second that. That's an excellent
2:40:17
idea. All right. Well, Rich, you
2:40:19
can get back to me on
2:40:21
your nominees as well. I will
2:40:23
definitely do that. All right, gentlemen.
2:40:25
Thank you for making the committee
2:40:27
on to this. Thank you. Thank
2:40:29
you for listening to this week's
2:40:31
episode of The Drive. Head over
2:40:33
to Peter atiamd.com/show notes if you
2:40:35
want to dig deeper into this
2:40:37
episode. You can also find me
2:40:39
on YouTube, Instagram, and Twitter, all
2:40:41
with the handle Peter Atia MD.
2:40:43
You can also leave us review
2:40:45
on Apple podcasts or whatever podcast
2:40:47
player you use. This podcast is
2:40:49
for general informational purposes only and
2:40:51
does not constitute the practice of
2:40:53
medicine, nursing, or other professional health
2:40:55
care services, including the giving of
2:40:57
medical advice. No doctor-patient relationship is
2:40:59
formed. The use of this information
2:41:01
and the materials linked to this
2:41:03
podcast is at the user's own
2:41:05
risk. The content on this podcast
2:41:07
is not intended to be a
2:41:09
substitute for professional medical advice, diagnosis,
2:41:11
or treatment. users should not disregard
2:41:13
or delay in obtaining medical advice
2:41:15
from any medical condition they have,
2:41:17
and they should seek the assistance
2:41:19
of their health care professionals for
2:41:21
any such conditions. Finally, I take
2:41:23
all conflicts of interest very seriously.
2:41:25
For all of my disclosures and
2:41:27
the companies I invest in or
2:41:29
advise, please visit Peter atiamd.com/about where
2:41:31
I keep an up-to-date and active
2:41:33
list of all disclosures.
Podchaser is the ultimate destination for podcast data, search, and discovery. Learn More