E116 - What are Mental Disorders? (with Dr. Awais Aftab)

E116 - What are Mental Disorders? (with Dr. Awais Aftab)

Released Friday, 21st March 2025
Good episode? Give it some love!
E116 - What are Mental Disorders? (with Dr. Awais Aftab)

E116 - What are Mental Disorders? (with Dr. Awais Aftab)

E116 - What are Mental Disorders? (with Dr. Awais Aftab)

E116 - What are Mental Disorders? (with Dr. Awais Aftab)

Friday, 21st March 2025
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

that I think points to this

0:02

kind of binary tendency that either

0:04

a problem is a problem of

0:07

biology or we want to say

0:09

that if the problem is not

0:11

a clear biological problem or a

0:14

problem biological abnormality, then you know,

0:16

nothing much is to be gained

0:18

by understanding the biological dimension of

0:21

it. Mental health problems, mental disorder,

0:23

exists precisely in this middle ground

0:25

where they are not primarily problems

0:28

of biology gone wrong. Mental health

0:30

problems are problems of

0:33

behavior gone wrong and,

0:35

you know, psychological experiences

0:38

gone wrong, but they

0:40

nonetheless have a biological

0:42

dimension. and then they're,

0:45

you know, because they

0:47

have a biological dimension,

0:50

we can study

0:53

that, we can

0:55

intervene on that,

0:58

you know, often

1:01

in, you

1:03

know, remarkably

1:05

effective ways

1:08

at times. And it's

1:10

not actually me conducting today's conversation,

1:12

but my co-host, Dr. Anya Borisova.

1:15

For those of you newer to the podcast,

1:17

she's a clinical and academic registrar

1:19

at the South London and Multis

1:21

Trust, and today she's in conversation with

1:23

Dr. Ayr's Aftab. Dr. Aftab is a

1:26

psychiatrist working in Ohio in the

1:28

US and is also the clinical

1:30

assistant professor of psychiatry at the

1:32

Case Western Reserve University. Dr. Aftab

1:34

is interested in philosophy and history

1:36

of psychiatry and writes about these

1:38

topics. on his sub-stack, psychiatry at

1:41

the margins. He's also the author

1:43

of the recently published Conversations

1:45

and Critical Psychiatry. Today's Conversation

1:47

focuses on the philosophy of

1:49

psychiatry, what assumptions and frameworks

1:51

shape the field and how

1:54

this impacts things like diagnosis

1:56

or decisions about treatment or

1:58

understanding mental disorders. They discuss

2:00

what defines a mental disorder,

2:02

how biological, psychological and social

2:04

factors interact with mental health,

2:06

the evolution of psychiatric diagnoses,

2:08

and how these have shifted

2:10

over time, the benefits and

2:12

limitations of psychiatric medications, their

2:14

potential harms, and some of

2:16

the public relations challenges that

2:18

psychiatry has faced and continues

2:21

to face in the modern

2:23

day. As ever, it's really

2:25

helpful if you can give

2:27

us a rating or a

2:29

comment or a review. We

2:31

really enjoy hearing from you.

2:33

and you can also give

2:35

us feedback directly at our

2:37

email at Thinking Mind podcast@gmail.com.

2:39

This is The Thinking Mind,

2:41

a podcast all about psychiatry,

2:43

psychology, self-development and related topics.

2:45

We hope you enjoy and

2:47

as always thank you for

2:49

listening. for this week's episode

2:51

of the Thinking Mind podcast.

2:53

Dr. Aftup, welcome and thank

2:55

you for joining us. Thank

2:57

you for having me. You

2:59

are a psychiatrist and you

3:01

write blogs. You do a

3:04

lot of thinking about what

3:06

the practice of psychiatry should

3:08

be like and how it

3:10

can be improved. What got

3:12

you thinking about this? What

3:14

made this important for you?

3:16

I had been interested in

3:18

in philosophy for a long

3:20

time even before I started

3:22

med school. I was interested

3:24

in philosophy and and in

3:26

fact I was I was

3:28

considering the possibility of going

3:30

into philosophy as an academic

3:32

profession, but I was in

3:34

Pakistan at the time and

3:36

you know philosophy as a

3:38

kind of profession was just

3:40

not really a viable option.

3:42

So I ended up in

3:44

medical school and in medical

3:46

school I realized pretty quickly

3:49

that the psychiatry was a

3:51

subject that interested me, it

3:53

felt a great fit for

3:55

my temperament and also for

3:57

my interests. And so I

3:59

started pursuing psychiatry. To my

4:01

delight I discovered that there's

4:03

this huge overlap between philosophy

4:05

as a discipline and the

4:07

subject better of psychiatry and

4:09

there's this burgeoning field of

4:11

philosophy of psychiatry which is

4:13

inhabited by both by medical

4:15

professionals, by psychologists, by philosophers.

4:17

So I started reading up

4:19

on that and started getting

4:21

familiar with the literature and

4:23

I really got interested and

4:25

curious about fundamental issues in

4:27

psychiatry, various kinds of concepts

4:29

that drive our clinical work

4:31

and our scientific work. and

4:34

I started realizing how fundamental

4:36

they are to do clinical

4:38

practice and scientific practice. So

4:40

my profession as a psychiatrist,

4:42

my career as a psychiatrist,

4:44

I have used the opportunity

4:46

to explore these basic questions

4:48

with the help of the

4:50

philosophical literature. What as a

4:52

quite a novice to philosophy

4:54

literature? and I imagine a

4:56

lot of our listeners are

4:58

as well, could you give

5:00

us more of an idea

5:02

of what kind of questions

5:04

those are, even what philosophy

5:06

means, particularly when applied to

5:08

psychiatry? Yes, so in, you

5:10

know, the range of questions

5:12

that are subject to philosophical

5:14

analysis or philosophical inquiry is

5:17

pretty broad. But a helpful

5:19

way to think about is

5:21

what are some of the

5:23

assumptions and ideas that are

5:25

guiding clinical work and what

5:27

are some of the basic

5:29

concepts that we rely on

5:31

in the field. So a

5:33

basic concept that that is

5:35

ubiquitous is the idea of

5:37

mental disorder or mental illness.

5:39

So we can ask about

5:41

when we characterize a behavioral

5:43

state as being disordered or

5:45

when we characterize a behavioral

5:47

state as being an illness

5:49

or a medical problem in

5:51

a certain sense. What kind

5:53

of judgments are we making?

5:55

Due to what extent are

5:57

these judgments grounded in facts

5:59

about biology? Are facts about

6:02

physiology? And to what extent

6:04

are these judgments grounded in

6:06

various values that guide human

6:08

decision making? To what extent

6:10

are these values social cultural

6:12

in nature? To what extent

6:14

are these driven by perceptions

6:16

of distress and judgments of

6:18

harm? And what is the

6:20

end? and it'll end product

6:22

of this confluence of judgments.

6:24

When we classify conditions within

6:26

the realm of mental disorders

6:28

and within the realm of

6:30

mental health problems, when we

6:32

let's say draw a line

6:34

between depression as a syndrome

6:36

and anxiety as a syndrome,

6:38

what is the nature of

6:40

that classification? What exactly are

6:42

we carving? Are we the

6:45

kind of categories that we

6:47

have separated? What is their

6:49

metaphysical status? Do these categories

6:51

present an essence, a unique

6:53

singular category that is particular

6:55

to or specific to that

6:57

group? Or are these just,

6:59

for example, practical groupings that

7:01

we're using for our practical

7:03

purposes and they don't necessarily

7:05

capture the underlying structure of

7:07

the problems or mechanisms of

7:09

the problem? So these are

7:11

metaphysical debates around what we

7:13

call essentialism and pragmatism. And

7:15

then when we look at

7:17

the nature of psychiatric explanation

7:19

and nature of psychiatric knowledge,

7:21

we can use a philosophy

7:23

of science perspective and we

7:25

can think about how science

7:27

progresses, how is it that

7:30

scientific knowledge is generated, what

7:32

kind of limitations biases it

7:34

is subject to, what is

7:36

the relationship between our... to

7:38

practical understanding of a phenomena

7:40

and the phenomena as did

7:42

exist independent of human concerns

7:44

or perception. So we can

7:46

apply a lot of those

7:48

philosophy of science questions about

7:50

realism, anti-realism, and epistemology and

7:52

the basis of scientific knowledge

7:54

to psychiatry as well and

7:56

think about how is it

7:58

that psychiatric knowledge is generated,

8:00

what is it that we

8:02

are trying to study, what

8:04

kind of inferences we can

8:06

make, and we can also

8:08

look at the methods of

8:10

knowledge production. what kind of

8:12

perspectives are being excluded, for

8:15

example, from the way we

8:17

currently generate psychiatric knowledge and

8:19

what are the consequences of

8:21

those things. And because psychiatry

8:23

deals with behavioral phenomena, but

8:25

also deals with brain mechanisms

8:27

and processes, we inevitably come

8:29

up with these philosophical issues

8:31

around the mind-body relationship. So

8:33

what is the relationship between

8:35

mental health symptoms, behavioral disservice

8:37

experiences, what is happening in

8:39

the brain, to what extent

8:41

we can reasonably think of

8:43

mental disorders as being brain

8:45

disorders? And what are the

8:47

advantages and disadvantages of thinking

8:49

of them in that kind

8:51

of manner? can conceivably be

8:53

the limits of biology in

8:55

explaining these problems. So that's

8:58

just, you know, I'm going

9:00

through a list of some

9:02

of the questions that, you

9:04

know, if psychiatry is interested

9:06

in philosophy and others working

9:08

in this area, you have

9:10

tackle, but it's just a

9:12

subset of things that are

9:14

being discussed. I guess to

9:16

go back to one of

9:18

the specific examples, so you

9:20

talk about, for instance, using

9:22

a philosophical viewpoint to inquire

9:24

around. diagnosis like depression anxiety

9:26

and to what extent these

9:28

might be separate entities to

9:30

what extent they're categories that

9:32

are sort of pragmatic to

9:34

use. What I mean what

9:36

what do you think about

9:38

that question? Yeah so the

9:40

The people involved in these

9:43

specific debates, you'll hear them

9:45

talk about natural kinds, social

9:47

kinds, and practical kinds. Natural

9:49

kinds are... categorizations that reflect

9:51

the structure of the of

9:53

the natural world as it

9:55

exists independent of human interests.

9:57

So a good example of

9:59

that is the periodic table

10:01

of elements in chemistry and

10:03

physics. It's it captures something

10:05

genuine about the way elements

10:07

exist and they're distributed. It

10:09

corresponds to the structure of

10:11

the atomic nuclei. It has

10:13

tremendous explanatory and predictive power.

10:15

on the position of the

10:17

element in the periodic table,

10:19

we can predict a lot

10:21

of things about them. And

10:23

it is the, it is

10:25

the correct, objectively correct way

10:28

of categorizing and classifying elements

10:30

as well. If we imagine

10:32

different chemists, you know, starting

10:34

from different theoretical ideas about

10:36

what elements are. if they

10:38

do empirical work good enough

10:40

if they're doing scientific work

10:42

correctly over time they would

10:44

all converge onto the same

10:46

model so it's it's it's

10:48

exist independently captures it's the

10:50

you know it has powerful

10:52

explanatory value and independent of

10:54

human interest versus in think

10:56

of So those are natural

10:58

kinds. The complete opposite are

11:00

social kinds, and these are

11:02

categorizations and distinctions that are

11:04

almost completely dependent on human

11:06

interests. So a good example

11:08

of this would be something

11:11

like the political borders. They're

11:13

entirely dependent on human perceptions

11:15

and human negotiations. They exist

11:17

in our minds, so to

11:19

speak. There's nothing in the

11:21

natural, you know, geographical landscape

11:23

that corresponds to the border

11:25

itself. And if humans were

11:27

to suddenly despair, you know,

11:29

the political borders would no

11:31

longer exist. And as human...

11:33

conflicts occur, we change these,

11:35

you know, border space based

11:37

on our negotiations. So that's

11:39

an example of something that

11:41

depends on human interests, you

11:43

know, almost completely with no

11:45

correspondence to the natural world.

11:47

And in between natural and

11:49

social kinds, there is this

11:51

messy gray area of what

11:53

we call practical kinds. And

11:56

these are these are phenomena

11:58

that that are enmeshed with

12:00

human interests and that we

12:02

have multiple ways of looking

12:04

at this phenomena from different

12:06

perspectives, different angles, depending on

12:08

what it is that we

12:10

want to achieve. And depending

12:12

on what we want to

12:14

achieve, we can make categorizations

12:16

that are useful to us,

12:18

but they are not the

12:20

one objectively correct way of

12:22

looking at it in the

12:24

same way as the periodic

12:26

table of elements is. So

12:28

a lot of medications in

12:30

medicine and a lot of

12:32

them in psychiatry tend to

12:34

fall into this area of

12:36

practical kinds. Now there are

12:39

exceptions to it. So for

12:41

example infectious diseases are a

12:43

clear example of you know,

12:45

natural kinds in medicine, you

12:47

know, whether some, whether a

12:49

syndrome is, is caused by

12:51

a spirokeyed infection, you know,

12:53

we can identify, we can,

12:55

you know, we can discover

12:57

that it's a, it's a

12:59

well-defined entity and things like

13:01

autosomal genetic disorders, you know,

13:03

can come pretty close to

13:05

something like that too. But

13:07

for other conditions like depression,

13:09

anxiety, psychosis that are highly

13:11

heterogenous and where we're kind

13:13

of, you know, the boundaries

13:15

are fuzzy and the actual

13:17

distribution of symptoms is very

13:19

dimensional as well. Any boundaries

13:21

and thresholds that we draw

13:24

and any distinctions that we

13:26

make, they are not going

13:28

to be the one objective.

13:30

correct way of looking at

13:32

it, but rather it's going

13:34

to question of what it

13:36

is that we that we

13:38

want to achieve. If we

13:40

want to achieve, for example,

13:42

a schema that corresponds to

13:44

response to treatment, that is

13:46

going to look very different

13:48

from some classification that optimizes,

13:50

let's say, genetic associations. And

13:52

if you want to optimize

13:54

something like neuroimaging findings, a

13:56

schema based on neuroimaging finding

13:58

is going to look very

14:00

different from you know, from

14:02

clinical description, for example. And

14:04

that's what we see. So,

14:06

in a, in a, in

14:09

a, in to tell, to

14:11

speak of this in other

14:13

terms, people talk of validators

14:15

and convergence of validators. When,

14:17

when we are dealing with

14:19

natural kinds, the converge, the

14:21

validators tend to converge. They

14:23

all tend to point in

14:25

the same direction. Versus when

14:27

we are dealing with practical

14:29

kinds, there's a misalignment between

14:31

different validators and different validators.

14:33

point or somewhat different things

14:35

and and you know how

14:37

to optimize that depends on

14:39

what it is that we

14:41

want to accomplish so so

14:43

there there is it's not

14:45

so we cannot make things

14:47

up it's not it's not

14:49

arbitrary you know it but

14:52

the optimal solutions will depend

14:54

on goals that we have

14:56

set for ourselves okay so

14:58

how you think about depression

15:00

as a psychiatrist or how

15:02

you think about finding the

15:04

boundary between depression anxiety will

15:06

depend on whether you're for

15:08

example thinking about what kind

15:10

of treatment to prescribe versus

15:12

how to explain that to

15:14

the patient or how the

15:16

patient might understand it themselves?

15:18

Yes, so the first thing

15:20

to note is that even

15:22

in terms of, you know,

15:24

symptoms themselves, depression and anxiety

15:26

share symptoms. So the boundaries

15:28

between them are fuzzy for

15:30

you know, based on that

15:32

reason alone. So, so many,

15:34

many. things like you know,

15:37

sleep disturbances, distractibility, irritability, you

15:39

know, other changes can be

15:41

seen in both. And then

15:43

depression and anxiety, as syndromes

15:45

are highly comorbid as well.

15:47

People oftentimes, you know, have

15:49

both of them. And in

15:51

fact, when you look at

15:53

the way these symptoms cluster

15:55

together, due to the hydrogenate

15:57

and in high comorbidity, depression

15:59

and anxiety do not show

16:01

up as distinct syndromes in

16:03

the statistical sense, in the

16:05

psychometric sense, but rather they

16:07

show up in kind of

16:09

in mesh together in this

16:11

larger subfactor known as the

16:13

distress subfactor, which is one

16:15

of the, you know, one

16:17

of the kind of dimensions

16:19

in a classification known as

16:22

hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology. you

16:24

know, which is based on

16:26

statistical association. So even statistically

16:28

speaking, they do not separate

16:30

out as distinct syndromes. And

16:32

another way to think about

16:34

is you can think of

16:36

it of what happened in

16:38

the United States in terms

16:40

of diagnostic practices. So if

16:42

you look at 1960, 1960s,

16:44

very commonly, we're the most

16:46

common kind of, you know,

16:48

psychiatric diagnosis and, you know,

16:50

utilized and given to patients

16:52

and, and common treatments at

16:54

that time were benzodazepine medications.

16:56

So, so people were getting

16:58

diagnosed with anxiety and they

17:00

were, a lot of them

17:02

were being prescribed benzodazepines. And

17:05

at that time, psychiatrists are

17:07

also working with this general

17:09

idea of neurosis, which kind

17:11

of heavily in which anxiety

17:13

symptoms featured pretty prominently. When

17:15

DSM-3 was published in 9-4,

17:17

1980, the people behind the

17:19

DSM-3 had a somewhat kind

17:21

of anti-psychodynamic bias. And as

17:23

a result of that, they

17:25

took the category of neurosis

17:27

and they divided that up

17:29

into several distinct anxiety disorders.

17:31

So general anxiety disorder became

17:33

its own thing, panic disorder

17:35

began its own thing, obsessive

17:37

compulsive disorder began its one

17:39

thing. And they set the

17:41

criteria relatively conservatively. So for

17:43

general anxiety disorder, you have

17:45

to meet a threshold of

17:47

six months in order in

17:50

order to feed. diagnosed. Now

17:52

prior to DSM 3, depression

17:54

did not exist as a

17:56

unified entity or a unified

17:58

category in the DSM. In

18:00

DSM 1 and 2, you

18:02

see manic depressive, a kind

18:04

of, manic depressive, insanity, depressive

18:06

episodes. You had psychotic depression

18:08

separately. You had psychotic depression

18:10

separately. And you even had

18:12

a depressive personality kind of

18:14

separate, you know, and separate.

18:16

And then... convolutional melancholia which

18:18

was kind of depression in

18:20

the old age that that

18:22

that was a separate category

18:24

and people sometimes spoke of

18:26

this primarily as being a

18:28

distinction between melancholic endogenous styles

18:30

of depression and and neurotic

18:33

styles of depression so but

18:35

depression was a was fragmented

18:37

across the manual in DSM

18:39

1 and 2 in DSM

18:41

3 it is all brought

18:43

together in the in a

18:45

unified category of major major

18:47

major depression and and and

18:49

compared to anxiety disorders, the

18:51

threshold is set relatively low.

18:53

You just need two weeks

18:55

of, you know, peer, you

18:57

know, symptom period in order

18:59

to meet depression criteria. And

19:01

we, and in 1980s, it

19:03

also when we, when we

19:05

start seeing the development of

19:07

antidepressant medications. Prior to 1980,

19:09

depression as a diagnosis was

19:11

rare, because it was only

19:13

being given to people who

19:15

had severe melancholic symptoms, and

19:18

anyone who had milder forms

19:20

because they tend to have

19:22

comorbid anxiety too, they were

19:24

diagnosed with an anxiety disorder.

19:26

Post 1980, we see this

19:28

large shift that, you know,

19:30

depression starts becoming a more

19:32

common diagnosis. People who would

19:34

have been diagnosed with anxiety

19:36

in 1960, 1970s, 70s, are

19:38

now being diagnosed depression and

19:40

the treatment of selection also

19:42

changes and becomes, you know,

19:44

as the surreiser being developed

19:46

and others, so anti-depression start

19:48

getting utilized more. symptoms have

19:50

not changed. You know, people

19:52

were still experiencing the same

19:54

mix of depression and anxiety

19:56

as they were all always

19:58

experiencing. But our understanding and

20:00

conceptualization of that changed dramatically.

20:03

You know, the same person,

20:05

you know, who would be

20:07

classified as, you know, anxiety

20:09

disorder in one decade is

20:11

classified as as depressive disorder

20:13

in another decade, but the

20:15

same exact mix of symptoms.

20:17

And what has changed is

20:19

what... symptoms we are emphasizing,

20:21

what thresholds we are using,

20:23

and in kind of how

20:25

we are approaching the treatment.

20:27

So that's a good example

20:29

of how we're not dealing

20:31

with some kind of objective

20:33

essence of something that we

20:35

are identifying through our diagnostic

20:37

schemas, but rather different diagnostic

20:39

approaches emphasize different cardinal symptoms,

20:41

they emphasize different thresholds, between

20:43

these conditions in a different

20:46

way. So you could, you

20:48

know, divide up, you know,

20:50

syndromes into smaller, you know,

20:52

categories, like, as we did

20:54

with anxiety disorders, or you

20:56

could combine smaller things, previously

20:58

smaller things, into one big

21:00

larger category, and suddenly a

21:02

lot more people would fit

21:04

into that. What do you

21:06

think about the present in

21:08

terms of how depression is

21:10

diagnosed, how anxiety is not...

21:12

is often not thought about

21:14

in people who have depression.

21:16

What do you, what are

21:18

your thoughts on that? Yeah,

21:20

I think so. I think

21:22

that the first thing we

21:24

have to recognize are the,

21:26

is the fluid fuzzy nature

21:28

of these diagnostic constructs. You

21:31

know, we are working with

21:33

these symptom level descriptions and

21:35

we have to recognize that

21:37

these are highly heterogeneous conditions,

21:39

different people are presenting with

21:41

very different combinations of symptoms.

21:43

And so, and that is

21:45

what we see in depression

21:47

is that people... you know,

21:49

present, you know, with depressive

21:51

states and I'm right right

21:53

here away. Some people have

21:55

very severe psychomotor retardation, agitation,

21:57

they have very severe cognitive

21:59

problems, they have severe anedonia,

22:01

sometimes kind of, you know,

22:03

severe, severe existential flavor to

22:05

suicidality, versus we see, there

22:07

are other people whose depression

22:09

is very enmeshed with their

22:11

life circumstances. They're experiencing a

22:13

very stressful time, they're going

22:16

through a divorce, they're having...

22:18

you know, job problems or

22:20

they have other, they have

22:22

financial, you know, stressors or

22:24

unstable housing and because of

22:26

that excessive stress, you know,

22:28

that they're experiencing a lot

22:30

of low mood and dysphoria.

22:32

And then there are other

22:34

situations where someone by their

22:36

temperament is predisposed to high

22:38

neuroticism and that high neuroticism

22:40

makes them vulnerable to experiencing

22:42

low mood and idonia at

22:44

kind of various points in

22:46

their life and perhaps... they

22:48

live with a certain amount

22:50

of low mood and an

22:52

anedonia that that you know

22:54

fluctuates with life stressors but

22:56

you know remains elevated at

22:59

all times. So these are

23:01

these are very different styles

23:03

of depression you know everyone

23:05

manifesting but in current diagnostic

23:07

schemas they're not gonna sort

23:09

it out very very well

23:11

and we know everyone is

23:13

being treated in a somewhat

23:15

similar kind of manner. So

23:17

I think we have to

23:19

recognize that our I can't.

23:21

schemas don't do justice to

23:23

this heterogeneity and also the

23:25

boundaries that we have made

23:27

between kind of disard different

23:29

conditions disorders at the symptom

23:31

level these boundaries are not

23:33

respected at the mechanistic level

23:35

the the mishmash of mechanism

23:37

that processes that exists between

23:39

you know it does not

23:41

respect DSM or ICD boundaries

23:44

you know we see this

23:46

with genetics too the you

23:48

know the genetic associations don't

23:50

respect whether this is schizophrenia

23:52

or better is bipolar or

23:54

schidzab effect, there were some

23:56

other kind of thing. So

23:58

we have to start thinking

24:00

that our boundaries, whether they

24:02

might be useful in terms

24:04

of description in a certain

24:06

clinical context, but they're not

24:08

going to be that particularly

24:10

helpful when we're looking at

24:12

things from a mechanistic standpoint,

24:14

or when we are looking

24:16

at things from a psychological

24:18

process standpoint, that is why

24:20

when we look at things

24:22

from, for example, from a

24:24

psychoanalytic or psychodynamic perspective, the

24:27

usual. descriptive categories are less

24:29

important and what matters more

24:31

is the is the pattern

24:33

of psychological experiences. So so

24:35

being mindful of that you

24:37

know we're dealing with a

24:39

multifaceted phenomena and and the

24:41

boundaries we use from one

24:43

perspective are not going to

24:45

hold, you know, looking at

24:47

things on another perspective. And

24:49

then secondly, we have to

24:51

be scientifically smarter about even

24:53

at the descriptive level about

24:55

examining these things. If the

24:57

traditional GSM ICD syndromes and

24:59

boundaries are kind of semi-arbitrary

25:01

in the sense that they

25:03

are built on clinical impressions

25:05

that people have had over

25:07

years and there's a kind

25:09

of semi-consensus around them, we

25:12

have to look at other

25:14

approaches about how we can

25:16

approach them in a sophisticated

25:18

matter. And that has been

25:20

happening in the psychology world

25:22

with the classification system known

25:24

as hierarchical text on me

25:26

of psychopathology, where people are

25:28

taking statistical data. seriously and

25:30

they're asking you know what

25:32

happens if we look at

25:34

the at the co variation

25:36

and co-occurrence between symptoms if

25:38

you know how do we

25:40

what kind of latent factors

25:42

emerge you know in this

25:44

classification and the answer first

25:46

is that you know these

25:48

kind of the The dimensions,

25:50

we see dimensions emerge rather

25:52

than categories. So the phenomena

25:54

are distributed in a population

25:57

in a latently continuous manner

25:59

rather than there being discontinuities.

26:01

One, and second, we see

26:03

a hierarchy of dimensions where

26:05

we have symptoms and traits

26:07

as being narrow dimensions, you

26:09

know, at the bottom that

26:11

that cluster together to form

26:13

larger dimensions such as distress

26:15

of factor, fears of factor,

26:17

that then cluster together to

26:19

form things like. internalizing disorders

26:21

and externalizing disorders and thought

26:23

disorders. And then there's this

26:25

very intriguing thing at the

26:27

top called the general factor

26:29

of psychopathology or the P

26:31

factor that it seems to

26:33

statistically speaking explain a shared

26:35

variance among all mental health

26:37

disorders and also kind of

26:40

is a reflection of the

26:42

fact that all mental disorders

26:44

are comorbid with each other

26:46

at a certain level. So

26:48

just as all aspects of

26:50

intelligence are. are in a

26:52

sense linked to each other

26:54

such that we can talk

26:56

about a G factor, a

26:58

general factor of intelligence. All

27:00

aspects of psychopathology are related

27:02

to each other through mechanisms

27:04

we don't understand very well

27:06

and we see a single

27:08

statistical factor emerged called the

27:10

P factor. And there's a

27:12

very interesting debate in the

27:14

scientific literature around how to

27:16

understand that, whether the P

27:18

factor is just some statistical

27:20

way of talking about things

27:22

or whether it represents some

27:25

kind of, you know, set

27:27

of processes that that confer

27:29

some kind of general vulnerability

27:31

to mental health. problems. So

27:33

the thing I want to

27:35

emphasize is that we have

27:37

to take the kind of

27:39

the pragmatic practical nature of

27:41

our classification seriously, which means

27:43

being humble about their limitations

27:45

and looking at these things

27:47

from a variety of different

27:49

perspectives and optimizing our classifications

27:51

for a variety of things.

27:53

Let me give you another

27:55

example. So take take blood

27:57

pressure. blood pressure is a

27:59

continuum. Everyone has a systolic

28:01

blood pressure, you know, and

28:03

there's nothing magical about our

28:05

current threshold for essential hypertension

28:07

as a diagnosis. So saying

28:10

that, oh, a 140 millimeter

28:12

mercury of the systolic blood

28:14

pressure, you know, we're using

28:16

it at a diagnostic threshold.

28:18

It's got some arbitrary. You

28:20

know, you could have said

28:22

139 or 141 and it

28:24

wouldn't be much different. So,

28:26

but there's no natural discontinuity

28:28

continuity at 1. It's just

28:30

a smooth line. So why

28:32

is it that we go

28:34

with 140 millimeter of mercury

28:36

thresholds? It's because we have

28:38

looked at the data and

28:40

we are interested in reducing

28:42

the risk of future cardiovascular

28:44

negative events. So we are

28:46

interested in reducing the risk

28:48

of heart attacks and we're

28:50

interested in reducing the risk

28:53

of heart attacks and we're

28:55

interested in reducing mortality. And

28:57

if we look at, so

28:59

we identify a goal, we

29:01

look at the data and

29:03

we say what can threshold

29:05

would make the most sense

29:07

to try to optimize mortality

29:09

reduction and that gives us

29:11

the answer that if you

29:13

want to achieve this then

29:15

you know try to keep

29:17

the blood pressure lower than

29:19

140 and that also gives

29:21

us the wiggle room to

29:23

revise it so for diabetics

29:25

we know that even with

29:27

140 the mortality risk can

29:29

be higher so for diabetics

29:31

we set the blood pressure

29:33

threshold lower right so our

29:35

thresholds are going to be

29:38

sensitive to what it is

29:40

that we want to accomplish.

29:42

The problem with DSM-ICD is

29:44

that they haven't had. had

29:46

clarity on what it is

29:48

that they're trying to optimize.

29:50

Let's say we have the

29:52

threshold for depression in DSM

29:54

of five out of nine

29:56

symptoms for a two-week period.

29:58

what is that threshold trying

30:00

to optimize? I mean it

30:02

doesn't optimize treatment response because

30:04

you know people people still

30:06

respond to treatment even if

30:08

they don't meet that criteria.

30:10

It doesn't it doesn't optimize

30:12

you know near imaging finding

30:14

that we know it. It

30:16

doesn't optimize you know a

30:18

long course of illness. So

30:21

you know we have a

30:23

variety of things in mind

30:25

and we're you know we're

30:27

going about them in a

30:29

somewhat muddled manner versus if

30:31

we had clarity on what

30:33

what was the practical aim,

30:35

what was the clinical aim,

30:37

a scientific aim, we can

30:39

come up with different classification

30:41

schemes that optimize those goals

30:43

and, you know, make more

30:45

progress, you know, with that

30:47

kind of clarity. Is this

30:49

something that you apply in

30:51

your clinical practice at the

30:53

moment? This kind of thing,

30:55

or does this kind of

30:57

thinking modify how you practice?

30:59

In a certain amount of

31:01

way, in a certain sense,

31:03

I think one is that

31:06

my conceptualization of what a

31:08

person is going through, what

31:10

they're experiencing, is informed by

31:12

that. So I'm not just

31:14

thinking in terms of, hey,

31:16

you know, I'm going to...

31:18

you know, this person meets

31:20

X, Y, Z, you know,

31:22

DSM ICD, criteria, and, you

31:24

know, and then I'm just

31:26

going to blindly follow the

31:28

treatment algorithm, but rather, I'm

31:30

thinking, you know, in this,

31:32

all right, you know, what

31:34

are the different ways in

31:36

which I can conceptualize this

31:38

person's problem? And what way

31:40

of thinking makes the most

31:42

sense, you know, for this

31:44

particular patient in this particular

31:46

sense? And sometimes the diagnostic

31:48

category, the diagnostic category, is

31:51

a very useful way of

31:53

talking about, you know, you

31:55

know, you know, ADHD or

31:57

autism, sometimes it really, you

31:59

know, really can be a

32:01

life-changing explanation for them to

32:03

think of their problem in

32:05

that sense. In other cases,

32:07

it's, you know, whether I

32:09

call this major depression with

32:11

anxious distress or I call

32:13

this, you know, GAD, you

32:15

know, it... that doesn't matter

32:17

that much because the person's

32:19

problem is very linked to

32:21

a certain life circumstances and

32:23

it's very linked to their

32:25

personality and temperament and it's

32:27

it's much more fruitful for

32:29

me to think about this

32:31

person as having hey this

32:34

is a person who is

32:36

high in neuroticism who is

32:38

experiencing this acute life stressor

32:40

and this interaction is generating

32:42

a current you know syndrome

32:44

of distress for them. So

32:46

it you know it so

32:48

it allows me to adopt

32:50

a kind of flexible conceptualized

32:52

And then second thing is

32:54

that I try to communicate

32:56

that to patients as well

32:58

that do not attribute more

33:00

reality to these diagnostic labels

33:02

than they actually possess. They

33:04

are symptom level descriptions of

33:06

you know, of these problems

33:08

and they have fuzzy boundaries

33:10

and they can change over

33:12

time and they don't they

33:14

don't capture some kind of

33:16

essence that exists in your

33:19

brain. And even that, you

33:21

know, superficial level explanation can

33:23

be very helpful for patients

33:25

because the average lay person,

33:27

their understanding of these diagnostic

33:29

categories is very, very reductive

33:31

and biomedical, you know, it's

33:33

informed by decades of this

33:35

language of chemical imbalance in

33:37

brain diseases. So date and

33:39

to think that these categories

33:41

actually reflect the structure of,

33:43

you know, how brain functions

33:45

or how brain goes wrong

33:47

or, you know, or what

33:49

happens never scientifically. So being

33:51

clear about the nature of

33:53

these diagnostic, thresholds and diagnostic,

33:55

you know, mapings can be

33:57

very helpful for patients to

33:59

understand their own problems in

34:01

a better way. It's a

34:04

tricky point that you bring

34:06

because it's... speaks to sort

34:08

of a side of the

34:10

debate around diagnosis that can

34:12

become quite binary. And I

34:14

can hear that in the

34:16

explanations that you give to

34:18

your patients, it's in trying

34:20

to convey to them that

34:22

a diagnostic category is not

34:24

like a periodic table element

34:26

that is a hard natural

34:28

fact. But I think that

34:30

can sometimes go into an

34:32

area of the debate that

34:34

says Well, then these conditions

34:36

just don't exist and actually

34:38

they're not worth diagnosing at

34:40

all and they aren't biological

34:42

things in any sense of

34:44

the word and they are

34:47

just social constructs. How do

34:49

you tread that line? How

34:51

do you think about those

34:53

kind of questions? Yeah, and

34:55

I think we have been

34:57

seeing this kind of dynamic

34:59

play out in both. popular

35:01

and as well as academic

35:03

discussions around the nature of

35:05

mental illness. And my whole,

35:07

the direction of my academic

35:09

and clinical work has been

35:11

in challenging these binaries and

35:13

in showing that this dichotomous

35:15

way of thinking about mental

35:17

health problems, in fact, it

35:19

doesn't even apply very well

35:21

to medicine, you know, let

35:23

alone to psychiatry. And things

35:25

are much more complicated than

35:27

that, you know, than that.

35:29

and we have to be,

35:32

we have to, you know,

35:34

use better conceptual tools than

35:36

thinking of this in this

35:38

biomial fashion. And again, I

35:40

think the problem comes down

35:42

to the kind of biomedical

35:44

reductive idea that people have

35:46

about medicine generally. They tend

35:48

to think of all medical

35:50

diagnostic categories as being like

35:52

infectious diseases or being like

35:54

autosomal genetic disorders, when in

35:56

fact, most chronic conditions. in

35:58

medicine are highly multifectorial. and

36:00

you know, and can be,

36:02

you know, described and classified,

36:04

you know, with, you know,

36:06

some degree of kind of

36:08

debate and wiggle room, you

36:10

know, things like that. So,

36:12

the, and what happens is

36:15

that if that is the

36:17

conceptualization we work with, that,

36:19

that medical diagnosis have essences

36:21

that that are neurobiological dysfunctions,

36:23

then it, you know, we,

36:25

we, we, we, we either.

36:27

We either fit a mental

36:29

health problem into that kind

36:31

of category or we just

36:33

reject that understanding and we

36:35

say that, oh, there's, you

36:37

know, the biology has nothing

36:39

to do with this. This

36:41

is, you know, kind of

36:43

completely some kind of psychosocial

36:45

kind of issue that medical

36:47

diagnostic terminology is completely unsuitable

36:49

for this. So it sets

36:51

people up for this kind

36:53

of flip-flopping between two extreme

36:55

views. Versus, you know, If

36:57

we understand that many problems

37:00

exist in medicine that do

37:02

not have an essence in

37:04

the traditional sense, and we

37:06

can identify problematic behaviors based

37:08

on the negative impact it

37:10

has on a person's life,

37:12

if a person is persistently

37:14

and severely depressed to a

37:16

point where they're unable to

37:18

function, they're unable to function.

37:20

Let's say they're, you know,

37:22

they are so, their psychomotor

37:24

functioning as slow down to

37:26

a point, they can barely

37:28

get out of bed, they

37:30

can barely brush or, you

37:32

know, change clothes and their

37:34

appetite has gone down and

37:36

they're losing weight. That represents

37:38

a significant state of impairment

37:40

and there are many different.

37:42

biological, physiological ways in which

37:45

that state can be produced

37:47

or realized, which accounts for

37:49

the mechanistic hydrogenity of it.

37:51

So there is. there's not

37:53

going to be, you know,

37:55

there's no single, singular cause

37:57

or a final common pathway

37:59

or a fine, you know,

38:01

or a common biological deficit

38:03

in all of that. But

38:05

that state is clearly a

38:07

state of impairment of harm

38:09

of suffering. And if we

38:11

have, you know, medical tools

38:13

to alleviate that suffering, at

38:15

least for some people, you

38:17

know, in the form of

38:19

medications and neurostim therapies, then

38:21

withholding those tools. simply because,

38:23

you know, that condition does

38:25

not satisfy some kind of

38:28

abstract idea of what a

38:30

medical disorder is, is just,

38:32

you know, is just the

38:34

wrong thing to do, in

38:36

my view. So we have

38:38

to expand our understanding of

38:40

what it is that we

38:42

mean when we call something

38:44

to be a medical condition.

38:46

When we encounter states of

38:48

suffering that are kind of

38:50

clearly outside of the norms,

38:52

of our expected social cultural

38:54

norms and other norms of

38:56

functioning. And they are amenable

38:58

to be described in medical

39:00

clinical terms. And we have

39:02

clinical ways of helping at

39:04

least some people with that.

39:06

Then in my view, that

39:08

becomes a bona fide medical

39:10

medical conditions, which again, you

39:13

know, emphasizes the kind of

39:15

pragmatic approach that I have

39:17

been advocating, that these things

39:19

are pragmatic constructs. When we

39:21

say it. that depression is

39:23

a medical condition or anxiety

39:25

is a medical condition. We're

39:27

not making a claim that,

39:29

oh, there is some hidden

39:31

medical cause behind these syndromes

39:33

that is causing it, and

39:35

that's what we're trying to

39:37

fix. But we're rather simply

39:39

making a very practical claim

39:41

that these are states of

39:43

immense suffering that are out

39:45

of the ordinary by some

39:47

standard, and we have things

39:49

we can do, you know,

39:51

in our medical toolkit that

39:53

can help. person in that

39:55

kind of state of suffering.

39:58

it's useful or necessary to

40:00

identify the brain basis for

40:02

those states of suffering? I

40:04

think it's useful and in

40:06

some ways even necessary to

40:08

try to have a scientific

40:10

line of inquiry that seeks

40:12

to understand what the brain

40:14

mechanism and processes involved are.

40:16

you know, acting on those

40:18

mechanisms is one of the

40:20

ways in which we can,

40:22

you know, better improve the,

40:24

you know, the lives for

40:26

those individuals. And because we

40:28

are, you know, our minds

40:30

are embodied, you know, we

40:32

don't have free floating mental

40:34

substance, you know, that exists

40:36

independent, all of our behaviors

40:38

are mediated by the brain.

40:41

So even extreme... impairing disabling

40:43

behavioral states, they are somehow,

40:45

you know, the brain processes

40:47

through their, you know, in

40:49

their complex direction are somehow

40:51

producing them, and those states

40:53

are somehow emerging in the

40:55

ways that brains interact with,

40:57

you know, with the environment,

40:59

and then the way that

41:01

brains interact with other brains,

41:03

you know, in our interaction

41:05

with other people. So there's

41:07

a brain basis to that,

41:09

you know, to be discovered

41:11

and to be talked about.

41:13

Now, it's not going to

41:15

eliminate the need. for us

41:17

to you know rely on

41:19

psychological behavior language to describe

41:21

stuff that is going on

41:23

because when we when we

41:26

use psychological cognitive language we

41:28

are talking we are describing

41:30

we're approaching these phenomena at

41:32

a higher level we are

41:34

talking about how kind of

41:36

you know the you know

41:38

like we're not just talking

41:40

about a brain in isolation

41:42

but we are we're talking

41:44

about a brain in interaction

41:46

with the environment we are

41:48

talking about multiple brains so

41:50

the the language of neurology

41:52

for example cannot capture those

41:54

interactions because the language of

41:56

neurology is focused on what

41:58

is happening inside one brain

42:00

versus the language of psychology

42:02

and psychodynamics is captures those

42:04

higher level dynamics, higher level

42:06

interactions quite well. And many

42:09

times behavioral health problems are

42:11

linked with those higher order

42:13

things. We are dealing with

42:15

how we are perceived by

42:17

other people and things that

42:19

other people say to us

42:21

and our status within, you

42:23

know, asserting things. And we're

42:25

talking about things in the

42:27

estates that we have acquired

42:29

through learning. and behaviors, you

42:31

know, so that all of

42:33

that remains essential. But for

42:35

certain, you know, for many

42:37

of these conditions, learning more

42:39

about how is it that,

42:41

you know, the brain mediates

42:43

these behaviors. Why is it

42:45

that some people get stuck

42:47

in certain depressive states? You

42:49

know, even when they desperately

42:51

want to change their own

42:54

behaviors, they're not able to?

42:56

And what role, for example,

42:58

does neuroplasticity play in that?

43:00

Why is it that in

43:02

certain cases there's a very

43:04

strong family history of mental

43:06

health problems? And why is

43:08

it in some people genetics

43:10

seems to confer a huge

43:12

degree of vulnerability? And the

43:14

thing with genetics is that

43:16

genetics factors cannot directly manifest

43:18

in psychological terms. Any expression

43:20

of genes has to go

43:22

through cellular pathways and expression

43:24

in brain network, etc. So

43:26

any link between behaviors and

43:28

gene is gonna be mediated

43:30

through intervening neurobiological pathways, which

43:32

means that there are things

43:34

we can say. How is

43:36

it that, you know, genes,

43:39

for example, confer high vulnerability

43:41

to schizophrenia or bipolar disorder?

43:43

And understanding what those mechanisms

43:45

are, you know, has the

43:47

potential for us to develop

43:49

new... interventions. Now it does

43:51

not mean that that the

43:53

reason a person, let's say,

43:55

you know, dwell is bipolar

43:57

disorder or depression or schizophrenia

43:59

is because they have abnormal

44:01

genes. You know, that's a

44:03

very different thing. That would

44:05

be a false way of

44:07

understanding this. You know, it

44:09

would be a mistake to

44:11

say that, oh, that, you

44:13

know, bipolar disorder is a

44:15

disorder of abnormal genetics in

44:17

the same way as Huntington's

44:19

disease that is a disorder

44:22

of normal genetics. You know,

44:24

that's a different kind of...

44:26

But it nonetheless remains the

44:28

case that there is a

44:30

genetic component to a bipolar

44:32

disorder, to schizophrenia, to a

44:34

smaller extent even in depression.

44:36

And that there are coherent

44:38

ways in which that genetic

44:40

contribution is being expressed. And

44:42

in trying to understand that

44:44

means that we better understand

44:46

at the brain level what

44:48

is happening to a depressed

44:50

person. And then, you know,

44:52

hopefully, the hope is that

44:54

that gives us more tools

44:56

to understand. more tools to

44:58

intervene on that and help

45:00

a person. So again, that

45:02

I think points to this

45:04

kind of, you know, this

45:07

binary tendency that we want

45:09

to say that either a

45:11

problem is a problem of

45:13

biology. you know, and that's

45:15

why we would take biologically

45:17

seriously, or we want to

45:19

say that if the problem

45:21

is not a clear biological

45:23

problem or a problem by

45:25

logical abnormality, then, you know,

45:27

nothing much is to be

45:29

gained by understanding the biological

45:31

dimension of it. And so,

45:33

and, you know, versus mental

45:35

health problems, mental health problems

45:37

are not primarily problems of...

45:39

you know biology gone wrong.

45:41

Mental health problems are problems

45:43

of behavior gone wrong and

45:45

you know psychological experiences gone

45:47

wrong but they nonetheless have

45:49

a biological dimension and then

45:52

they you know because they

45:54

have a biological logical dimension,

45:56

we can study that, we

45:58

can intervene on that, you

46:00

know, often in, you know,

46:02

remarkably effective ways at times.

46:04

A couple thoughts jump up,

46:06

one of which is moving

46:08

away from the conversation and

46:10

depression, but I think you

46:12

drew to some of the

46:14

nuances that we can also

46:16

use to think about diagnoses

46:18

like bipolar, schizophrenia, because I

46:20

think sometimes we focus a

46:22

lot of the conversation about

46:24

the psychosocial contributors to... to

46:26

conditions like anxiety and depression.

46:28

I mean, I feel like

46:30

the more that I work,

46:32

the more patients that I

46:35

see, it feels unagnorable that

46:37

patients who have conditions like

46:39

schizophrenia have so many different

46:41

stresses and factors in their

46:43

lives that either contribute to

46:45

relapses over their lifetime, but

46:47

also when you see the

46:49

story of the initial illness,

46:51

it feels like you... you

46:53

can't ignore the psychosocial aspect.

46:55

What, I guess, do you

46:57

see that coming through more

46:59

in psychiatry nowadays? Did it

47:01

used to be thought about

47:03

more before and then biology

47:05

took over for a time?

47:07

What's your view? Yeah, I

47:09

mean, I completely agree with

47:11

you. You know, psychosocial factors,

47:13

you know, both, you know,

47:15

in terms of, you know,

47:17

individual psychological factors in terms

47:20

of their... personality structure in

47:22

terms of a temperament, in

47:24

terms of their psychological developmental

47:26

history, in terms of their,

47:28

you know, the traumatic experience

47:30

that they have had and

47:32

their perceptions of those inoid

47:34

verse experiences, as well as

47:36

the larger societal organization around

47:38

them, you know, their state

47:40

of poverty. kind of financial

47:42

stress, you know, housing instability,

47:44

you know, access to food,

47:46

those kind of things, all

47:48

of them are extremely important.

47:50

The contributions vary from, you

47:52

know, from person to person

47:54

and in case, you know,

47:56

kind of condition to condition,

47:58

but they are absolutely vital

48:00

and essential. And in many

48:03

cases, the mental health problems

48:05

and psychopathological problems, they are

48:07

enmeshed with these psychological and

48:09

social kind of stressors and

48:11

issues. So any psychiatry that

48:13

ignores them or minimizes them

48:15

or neglects them, it's going

48:17

to be a highly impoverished

48:19

psychiatry and is not going

48:21

to serve patients very well.

48:23

What happened in the... in

48:25

the 90s was that there

48:27

was this tremendous exuberance around

48:29

the potential for neuroscience and

48:31

genetics and there was this

48:33

tendency to just conceptualize these

48:35

problems as these are brain

48:37

diseases, these are genetic disorders,

48:39

and there was this strong

48:41

optimism that just any day

48:43

now we're going to find

48:45

the genes, any day now

48:48

we're going to find the

48:50

brain circuits, and then we'll

48:52

be able to fix these

48:54

problems in a particular kind

48:56

of way. Now that in

48:58

the in you know that

49:00

that hope did not kind

49:02

of was not successful, you

49:04

know, by early 2000s, it

49:06

became pretty clear that the,

49:08

you know, biology is much

49:10

more complicated than that, but

49:12

it also became clear that

49:14

that some of the kind

49:16

of, you know, most important

49:18

drivers and contributors of these

49:20

problems are in fact things

49:22

that are happening outside the

49:24

brain. It is these childhood

49:26

adverse experiences, it is these,

49:28

you know, terf like, you

49:30

know, tribal life circumstances, stressful

49:33

life events. happening. And it's,

49:35

you know, in order to

49:37

improve clinical outcomes, both at

49:39

the individual level and at

49:41

the population level, we have

49:43

to tackle those, those kind

49:45

of, you know, larger psychosocial

49:47

psychosocial dynamics. And medicine, I

49:49

think, has struggled to do

49:51

that in psychiatry as well,

49:53

but also, you know, medicine

49:55

generally. And this is, I

49:57

think, in order to take

49:59

effect. psychosocial action, we need

50:01

a certain kind of political

50:03

movement, we need a certain

50:05

kind of political action, and

50:07

we need by and from,

50:09

from, you know, social forces

50:11

and social stakeholders. And the

50:13

political climate has not been

50:16

conducive to the, to, you

50:18

know, producing the kind of

50:20

effective public health changes that

50:22

people really, really need. And

50:24

so the emphasis has been

50:26

more and more on individual-centric

50:28

interventions. What can we do

50:30

to improve the functioning of

50:32

this particular individual while, you

50:34

know, leaving their, you know,

50:36

their, you know, environment? surroundings,

50:38

you know, as much, you

50:40

know, intact as much as

50:42

possible. And this kind of

50:44

basic dynamic, you know, shows

50:46

up in you know, in

50:48

all areas of medicine, you

50:50

know, not just in psychiatry.

50:52

If you look at kind

50:54

of epidemiological data around metabolic

50:56

disorders and cardiometabolic disorders, you

50:58

see this tremendous, you know,

51:01

association with social determinants of

51:03

health, with poverty, with access

51:05

to food, with access to

51:07

exercise, you know, and so

51:09

there's a there's a case

51:11

to be made that, you

51:13

know, a public health. approach

51:15

is necessary for all health,

51:17

not just mental health and

51:19

physical health, but it is

51:21

certainly much more important in

51:23

psychiatry than even it is

51:25

in general medicine. So there's

51:27

a difference of degree, but

51:29

not a difference of quality

51:31

when it comes to these

51:33

issues in psychiatry versus medicine.

51:35

I think the trick again

51:37

is in holding both, accepting

51:39

both truth at the same

51:41

time. We can acknowledge that

51:43

yes, there are factors within

51:46

an individual and there are

51:48

factors outside an individual and

51:50

we have to tackle both.

51:52

And worse than instead. we

51:54

see this weird polarity in

51:56

like IT where one group

51:58

just tries to insist that,

52:00

oh, you know, let's just

52:02

focus on the individual and

52:04

forget everything that's outside, and

52:06

the other group said, let's

52:08

just focus on the society

52:10

and, you know, forget what's

52:12

happening in the person. And

52:14

the reason to point out

52:16

is that Again, you know,

52:18

because these things are highly

52:20

biologically heterogenous, you know, we

52:22

have not been very successful

52:24

in mapping traditional DSM ICD

52:26

categories to specific biological processes.

52:29

But if you look at,

52:31

you know, what are some

52:33

of the biggest predictors for

52:35

the development of these conditions

52:37

and for the course, these

52:39

are actually the kind of

52:41

broader temperamental factors and personality

52:43

factors like neuroticism. that can

52:45

heavily predict whether a person,

52:47

for example, is going to

52:49

have depressive disorder, anxiety disorder,

52:51

other things now. And oftentimes

52:53

the contribution of personality factors

52:55

like neuroticism tends to be

52:57

greater than the contribution we

52:59

see from adverse childhood experiences

53:01

in general. So at the

53:03

very least, even if you

53:05

put aside questions of biological

53:07

mechanisms, we are seeing a

53:09

very strong interaction between individual

53:11

factors in the form of

53:14

their personality structure and life

53:16

events in the form of

53:18

both childhood adversity as well

53:20

as acute stressors later in

53:22

life. So any kind of

53:24

binary that tries to split

53:26

the two, that tries to

53:28

say either we focus on

53:30

the individual, either we focus

53:32

on the society, is actually

53:34

going to miss the interaction

53:36

that drives psychopathology to begin

53:38

with. And I think that

53:40

interaction feels very intuitive. probably

53:42

to most people, you know,

53:44

whether you practice in this

53:46

field or if you know

53:48

people who have experienced difficulties

53:50

with their mental health. What

53:52

do you think is at

53:54

the root of the fact

53:57

that nonetheless I guess we

53:59

struggle with? either biological

54:01

empirical evidence for

54:03

this or that we struggle

54:05

to turn it into interventions

54:08

that work for the majority of

54:10

people rather than just, you know,

54:12

40-50% of people. Yeah, I think

54:14

that the causes are, you

54:17

know, complex and have a

54:19

rich history. I think part

54:21

of it is because the

54:23

mental health fields is professionally

54:25

very fragmented. So it's not,

54:28

it's not a pure, you

54:30

know, pure medical system. So

54:32

for example, cardiologists, you know,

54:34

when they're, you know, they're

54:37

dealing with hard problems, they

54:39

are the main specialty devoted

54:41

to that. we have, you

54:43

know, people with psychiatric background, so

54:46

we have sex psychiatrists, you know,

54:48

who are into this. We also

54:50

have general practitioners and family doctors

54:52

in all this. We have people

54:55

from psychology background, so kind of,

54:57

you know, people who have a

54:59

psychology degrees, who might even have

55:01

PhDs in psychology, you know, involved

55:04

in this. And then we have,

55:06

you know, social people with social

55:08

work background, and especially in the

55:10

US, most people providing some form

55:12

of... or psychotherapy actually have a

55:15

social work background rather than a

55:17

psychology background. So we have multiple

55:19

kind of you know different disciplinary

55:22

backgrounds all you know engaged in

55:24

and trying to help people and

55:26

working with people and and they

55:29

all bring different assumptions and different

55:31

background ideas and different preferences and

55:33

different methodologies and they're in a

55:36

sense they're all competing for for

55:38

resources that the system has to

55:40

offer and they're competing. for prestige

55:43

that the system has to offer.

55:45

And if there are asymmetries

55:47

in how those resources and

55:49

how that prestige and how

55:51

the language that the public

55:53

gets to hear, how those

55:55

are distributed, then we set

55:57

up the scene for conflict

55:59

between. different disciplines and we set

56:02

up the scene for kind of

56:04

competition between disciplines for that which

56:06

ultimately ends up you know hurting

56:08

patients because they don't they don't get

56:10

the integrated care we need so especially

56:12

I think in the in the UK

56:15

with you know with the heavy reliance

56:17

on and the chess funding, you know,

56:19

there's a kind of zero-sum mindset where,

56:21

you know, you have a fixed pot

56:23

of money and it can either go

56:26

here, it can either go here. So

56:28

people in that effort to try to

56:30

obtain access to resources, that they have

56:32

to kind of bolster the narrative that

56:34

they're offering, they have to kind of

56:37

oversell, you know, what these problems

56:39

are and they have to exaggerate

56:41

their side of their perspective, you

56:43

know, so that they can secure.

56:45

for the kind of funding and

56:47

access to care that people need.

56:49

So I think, so that sets

56:51

up the scene where I think

56:53

different stakeholders are incentivized to promote

56:55

one side of the story rather

56:57

than, you know, try to produce

56:59

an integrated picture that recognizes everything.

57:02

And I think that the second

57:04

thing is that, you know, at

57:06

the philosophical level, again, you know,

57:08

because we're dealing with the

57:10

mind-body relationship and it is

57:12

so easy. for people to fall

57:14

into this mind body split

57:16

tendencies and you know what

57:18

locally you can call the dualism.

57:21

And it is so difficult

57:23

to talk about mind body

57:25

in an integrated sense because you

57:27

know the Western world doesn't

57:29

really have the language to

57:31

talk about that. We have to

57:34

tackle these centuries of bias

57:36

towards dualism as well as

57:38

reductionism, that is very hard

57:40

to overcome, especially for a

57:42

layperson. So there are these

57:44

kind of, you know, broader

57:46

philosophical challenges than we face.

57:48

And then finally, the reliance

57:50

on these symptom, heterogenous symptom

57:52

categories, I think, has obscured

57:54

the underlying complexity. It is

57:56

so easy to just say,

57:58

oh, you have... generalized anxiety

58:00

and here, you know, we have

58:03

medication or CD for that, that,

58:05

you know, relying on just that,

58:07

you know, actually ignores all the

58:10

messiness and none of that really

58:12

gets conveyed to the public and

58:14

sometimes not even, not even to

58:17

the clinicians. And that has held

58:19

back, you know, kind of appreciating

58:21

the nuanced nature of these problems

58:24

too. because I guess in

58:26

conducting, even just thinking to

58:29

the very basics of conducting

58:31

research, if you're recruiting

58:33

people with, you know,

58:35

recruitment will be people with

58:38

a diagnosis of major

58:40

depressive disorder as made by,

58:42

you know, the clinical

58:44

psychologist or the psychiatrist

58:47

who's running the study, but

58:49

to the often to the

58:51

exclusion of other diagnoses,

58:53

even... though that is not necessarily

58:55

the reality of that patient's existence

58:58

in the world, you know, they

59:00

will likely be experiencing

59:02

obsessive thoughts that perhaps don't

59:04

meet criteria for a diagnosis

59:07

of compulsive disorder, but they

59:09

might still be quite bothered

59:11

by thoughts when they're feeling

59:14

particularly distressed, particularly on edge,

59:16

that they've left something turned

59:18

on or that something terrible

59:20

is going to happen or

59:22

that... you know if they don't take a

59:24

certain action they'll be responsible

59:26

for all sorts of ills. But yeah

59:29

so if when research is so limited

59:31

to to doing the categories and

59:33

like you say actually in the large

59:35

part that's because of funding you know

59:37

you get funding to research a

59:39

disorder it's hard to move forward

59:42

I guess. Yes, yeah, and that's why

59:44

I think, you know, the scientific

59:46

community has been realizing that too,

59:48

and that's why there has been

59:51

this big push towards trans-diagnostic dimensional

59:53

research, because people have realized that

59:55

just having this methodology where you

59:57

take one DSM-ICT category and that

1:00:00

to healthy controls. That's actually not

1:00:02

the ideal kind of, you know,

1:00:04

study design because, you know, heterogeneity

1:00:06

and non-specific of the mechanisms, you

1:00:09

know, both at psychological and biological

1:00:11

level, and that it makes much

1:00:13

more sense to study these conditions

1:00:15

at different levels of specificity. You

1:00:17

know, looking at larger clusters of

1:00:20

these problems like intern, like what

1:00:22

it is that internalizing disorders all

1:00:24

share with each other, and in

1:00:26

some cases drilling down on specific

1:00:28

symptoms, example, an anedonia, what mechanisms

1:00:31

might be involved in anedonia

1:00:33

versus like a motor retardation,

1:00:35

versus just talking about a

1:00:37

depression-level syndrome. So looking at

1:00:39

these things that, you know,

1:00:41

in the trans-diagnostic manner and

1:00:43

also at a kind of

1:00:45

multi-dimensional manner, is very important

1:00:47

if you want to make

1:00:49

progress. And right now, it

1:00:51

is the case that our

1:00:53

treatments have generally limited efficacy.

1:00:55

This stands for both medical

1:00:57

treatments and psychological. treatments. The,

1:00:59

you know, our psychotherapies and our

1:01:01

medications are not as effective as we

1:01:03

would like them to be. It would

1:01:05

be great if we had more effective

1:01:08

treatments, but, you know, but we're not

1:01:10

there yet. And it's important to be

1:01:12

transparent with patients about that, that the

1:01:14

tools we have right now are limited

1:01:16

and somewhat imperfect, and they come with,

1:01:18

you know, fair degree of risks and

1:01:20

problems, too. And that they're not going

1:01:22

to be a magic fix. They're going

1:01:24

to, you know, you know, for a

1:01:26

lot of people, you know, they can

1:01:28

make the symptoms more tolerable and bearable

1:01:31

enough that you know there's

1:01:33

an improvement in functioning and

1:01:35

a smaller subset of people

1:01:37

experience more dramatic improvements

1:01:39

but for a lot of people

1:01:41

the symptoms are not going to go

1:01:43

away. They might get reduced to

1:01:45

more kind of bearable functional levels and

1:01:48

that You know, we should not over

1:01:50

promise or we should not present a

1:01:52

hyped up version of what what our

1:01:55

current tools can can accomplish and I

1:01:57

think the problem has been that we

1:01:59

We have not been sufficiently

1:02:02

humble as a profession.

1:02:04

The narrative that we

1:02:06

have told the public

1:02:08

is quite disconnected from

1:02:11

the scientific and medical

1:02:13

reality of what we

1:02:15

can actually accomplish. Yeah, and

1:02:17

I think it's refreshing and

1:02:20

I think luckily perhaps

1:02:22

starting to become

1:02:24

increasingly common to discuss

1:02:26

this level of nuance. in

1:02:28

terms of the limitations of

1:02:31

our treatments. And I think it's

1:02:33

again an area where binaries

1:02:35

often arise in saying that

1:02:37

if the treatments aren't helpful

1:02:39

then they aren't helpful at all

1:02:42

or how dare you say that

1:02:44

the treatments aren't helpful, they're

1:02:46

extremely helpful, there

1:02:48

aren't any problems with them. I

1:02:51

mean can we can we think

1:02:53

about some of the issues that

1:02:55

there are with treatments? both if

1:02:57

we think in the group of depression

1:02:59

anxiety type conditions but

1:03:01

also conditions where people

1:03:04

might experience disorders of thoughts,

1:03:06

psychosis, what are some of the problems

1:03:08

that you find and how do you

1:03:10

manage them when working with patients?

1:03:12

Yeah, I think so it varies quite

1:03:14

a, you know, quite a bit,

1:03:17

especially with regards to the problems

1:03:19

that people tend to experience in

1:03:21

the short term or in the

1:03:23

cute term, you know, when perhaps

1:03:25

medication is new when it is

1:03:27

being started, versus when they have

1:03:29

been on medications for for some

1:03:31

time and you know and you

1:03:33

know they're in a more maintained

1:03:35

stage of treatment and so the

1:03:37

nature of problems kind of often

1:03:39

differs based on that in the

1:03:41

short term we tend to see

1:03:43

a variety of what you can

1:03:45

call paradoxical reactions where let's say

1:03:48

you know you have someone with

1:03:50

depression and anxiety and you start

1:03:52

them on an antidepressant and they'll

1:03:54

suddenly have a flare up there

1:03:56

their anxiety will get worse you

1:03:59

know on soon after starting an

1:04:01

intebress and they might experience states

1:04:03

of agitation, they might experience states

1:04:05

of irritability, they'll have this intense

1:04:08

dysphoria that can sometimes even, you

1:04:10

know, turn into suicidality at times.

1:04:12

So we have to be mindful

1:04:15

of those that, you know, sometimes,

1:04:17

you know, we start a medication

1:04:19

and instead of things immediately starting

1:04:21

getting better, we get a paradoxical

1:04:24

response where things suddenly get worse.

1:04:26

And so we have... to be

1:04:28

any kind of, you know, when

1:04:30

we're starting a new medication or

1:04:33

we're increasing the dose, you have

1:04:35

to be mindful for those possibilities.

1:04:37

Then we have to discuss them

1:04:39

with patients that, you know, something

1:04:42

like this happened beyond the, you

1:04:44

know, beyond the lookout for that.

1:04:46

And then there are various kinds

1:04:48

of physical side effects, you know,

1:04:51

that post-dolerability challenges, you know, where

1:04:53

that's a surreise, for example, you

1:04:55

get a lot of gastrointestinal side

1:04:58

effects, people can have sexual dysfunction

1:05:00

issues, you know, with kind of,

1:05:02

you know, ongoing use, that can

1:05:04

become distressing, people can have things

1:05:07

like headaches, other things, and so

1:05:09

there are various kinds of tolerability

1:05:11

issues that that that... we need

1:05:13

to be mindful of. When we

1:05:16

start entering into the kind of

1:05:18

long-term maintenance therapy, then the metabolic

1:05:20

effects start becoming problems. So weight

1:05:22

gain over time, increased risk of

1:05:25

diabetes, increased risk of hyperlipidemia. And

1:05:27

that often poses, you know, a

1:05:29

challenge that if a person has

1:05:32

responded very well to a particular

1:05:34

medication, but now six months later

1:05:36

they, you know, they have gained

1:05:38

a lot of weight and their

1:05:41

kind of HPA1C is creeping up,

1:05:43

you know, we can't. just leave

1:05:45

it like that and you know

1:05:47

and you know increase their risk

1:05:50

of cardiometabolic problems. So the considerations

1:05:52

change so we have to do

1:05:54

that with with antesagotic medications as

1:05:56

you're well aware the risk of

1:05:59

more you know motor movement. disorders,

1:06:01

becomes prominent, you know, kind of

1:06:03

like, you know, as we increase

1:06:06

duration of treatment. And then something

1:06:08

that has been generally neglected historically

1:06:10

has been the risk of antidepressant

1:06:12

withdrawal, you know, and discontinuation issues

1:06:15

and not just with antidepressants, but

1:06:17

with other forms of psychotropics as

1:06:19

well, because people stay on these

1:06:21

medications for years, you know, when

1:06:24

they do decide to come off

1:06:26

them, historically they were, they were,

1:06:28

they were being. taken off very

1:06:30

fast and then they would have

1:06:33

severe withdrawal related effects. And because

1:06:35

we didn't have enough research investigating

1:06:37

that, there was a general skepticism

1:06:39

in the medical community about whether

1:06:42

these things even caused that, you

1:06:44

know, that's where a protected kind

1:06:46

of withdrawal. So it took a

1:06:49

lot of effort by kind of

1:06:51

service users and other other kind

1:06:53

of patient communities who had been

1:06:55

through really negative experiences. and they

1:06:58

raised enough of a human cry

1:07:00

that the medical community began to

1:07:02

pay attention and began to recognize

1:07:04

this as a serious problem. So

1:07:07

I think with all psychotropic use

1:07:09

long term, withdrawal issues become pretty

1:07:11

significant and we have to be

1:07:13

mindful of them. Why do you

1:07:16

think there was such a reluctance

1:07:18

for professionals to hear? the concerns

1:07:20

of patients when it came to

1:07:23

the issue of medication withdrawal symptoms?

1:07:25

It was a, I think it

1:07:27

was a combination of different factors.

1:07:29

One was that we did not

1:07:32

have good research data on this

1:07:34

phenomena. We, you know, there wasn't

1:07:36

any major funding agency that was

1:07:38

interested in exploring you know what

1:07:41

happens with long-term medication use what

1:07:43

kind of problems emerges so there

1:07:45

was a big gap for example

1:07:47

in the in the US Institute

1:07:50

of Mental Health is the biggest

1:07:52

public funder of research and in

1:07:54

the last three decades they have

1:07:57

been much more interested in doing

1:07:59

basic science research or doing in

1:08:01

or developing innovative medical treatments and

1:08:03

they have left clinical trials largely

1:08:06

to pharmaceutical companies. pharmaceutical companies obviously

1:08:08

are more interested in developing a

1:08:10

medication and studying its acute efficacy.

1:08:12

They're not interested in studying long-term

1:08:15

side effects. So there was a,

1:08:17

we had this problem with incentive

1:08:19

that no major research funder was

1:08:21

really that interested in the question.

1:08:24

You know, ideally, a national history

1:08:26

of mental health should have been.

1:08:28

but they set their priorities kind

1:08:30

of differently. And in similar kind

1:08:33

of ways, I'm sure that, you

1:08:35

know, among European funding agencies, no

1:08:37

one had really made this a

1:08:40

priority. So because there was no

1:08:42

research money going in that direction,

1:08:44

it was never studied. So there

1:08:46

was no general knowledge base to

1:08:49

link to. And because these problems,

1:08:51

you know, the severe versions of

1:08:53

them were sufficiently that the average

1:08:55

clinician was not seeing them. So

1:08:58

a person could treat hundreds of

1:09:00

cases of depression and not come

1:09:02

across a very severe case of

1:09:04

withdrawal. And the milder cases, when

1:09:07

they would come across milder cases

1:09:09

of withdrawal, they might just attribute

1:09:11

that to, oh, this is the

1:09:14

person's depression returning or this person's

1:09:16

anxiety returning or it has just

1:09:18

kind of had a change in

1:09:20

quality. So there was a kind

1:09:23

of an inherent bias towards interpreting

1:09:25

these symptoms. withdrawal related symptoms as

1:09:27

you know in relapse terms rather

1:09:29

than rather than withdrawal terms. So

1:09:32

when when people did kind of

1:09:34

start you know raising this concern

1:09:36

more fiercely you know the physicians

1:09:38

would look to their own experience

1:09:41

and they would say you know

1:09:43

I've been treating these patients I

1:09:45

myself I'm not seeing this and

1:09:48

then they would look to the

1:09:50

literature and like they were saying

1:09:52

you know there's no there are

1:09:54

no studies on this so they

1:09:57

would they would start from a

1:09:59

place of skepticism given given given

1:10:01

given that and and they would

1:10:03

they would the tendency would be

1:10:06

to you know there's something unusual

1:10:08

going on here that you know

1:10:10

but it's it's probably not the

1:10:12

mess versus the situation would have

1:10:15

been quite different if you know

1:10:17

if we had paid attention to

1:10:19

to long-term you know effects from

1:10:22

the very beginning then then people

1:10:24

would have started from an informed

1:10:26

place. Should there be a responsibility

1:10:28

for pharmaceutical companies to study more

1:10:31

than the acute effects of their

1:10:33

drugs? I think so. I would

1:10:35

personally say so, but you know,

1:10:37

but... forcing them to do it,

1:10:40

you know, it's going to, it's

1:10:42

in places like the US and

1:10:44

also Europe too, would require legislative

1:10:46

action. And pharmaceutical companies have far,

1:10:49

you know, stronger lobbying than most

1:10:51

positions. And so the general tendency

1:10:53

is that pharmaceutical companies tend to

1:10:55

only do what they're strictly obligated

1:10:58

to do. But you know, so

1:11:00

for example, FDA requires, you know,

1:11:02

you need to do large placebo

1:11:05

control trials. So, you know, they

1:11:07

can compel to do it. If

1:11:09

the, for example, if it was

1:11:11

mandated that, you know, you have

1:11:14

to do long-term safety study to,

1:11:16

you know, you know, things, and

1:11:18

there are certain, there are certain

1:11:20

minimal post-marketing requirements even now in

1:11:23

the US, but they were not.

1:11:25

strong enough to pick up these

1:11:27

kinds of issues. So there's certainly

1:11:29

I think a responsibility on the

1:11:32

pharmaceutical company side and we should

1:11:34

look into getting them to do

1:11:36

it. But I think at this

1:11:39

point, what I think we really

1:11:41

need is public. funding agencies to

1:11:43

make hydrogenic harm a priority. They

1:11:45

need to recognize that this is

1:11:48

getting to a point where this

1:11:50

is a crisis in many different

1:11:52

ways. It's a crisis of clinical

1:11:54

care, but it is also kind

1:11:57

of a crisis. of you know,

1:11:59

you know, public relations too around,

1:12:01

you know, around psychiatry medicine, etc.

1:12:03

And there's genuine suffering. So many

1:12:06

people, you know, are dealing with

1:12:08

this problem and feel abandoned by

1:12:10

the medical community. So this needs

1:12:13

to be a funding priority in

1:12:15

my view and by, you know,

1:12:17

made a priority by by medical

1:12:19

research organizations. With the public relations

1:12:22

crisis in mind. And again, we're

1:12:24

not focusing on other types of

1:12:26

treatments, but we aren't going to

1:12:28

be focusing on today, but things

1:12:31

like the debunking of the serotonin

1:12:33

hypothesis of depression and other issues

1:12:35

like that. My question is, how

1:12:37

do you how do you bear

1:12:40

these iatogenic calms in mind in

1:12:42

your clinical practice? How do you

1:12:44

use these medications? And again, we're

1:12:46

not focusing on other types of

1:12:49

treatments, but specifically what is your

1:12:51

approach and in your practice, but

1:12:53

perhaps also more broadly to this

1:12:56

public relations crisis. Yeah, my approach

1:12:58

has generally been to, you know,

1:13:00

talk about patients kind of in

1:13:02

an open and transparent manner about,

1:13:05

you know, about you know, the

1:13:07

nature of these problems as well

1:13:09

as the nature of these treatments.

1:13:11

So I try to convey to

1:13:14

them that the reason, for example,

1:13:16

they are depressed or anxious is

1:13:18

because of a confluence of different

1:13:20

factors coming together. You know, again,

1:13:23

the contribution raised from person to

1:13:25

person. So my form relation of

1:13:27

what is going on in any

1:13:30

particular case is going to be

1:13:32

different. a depressed state or an

1:13:34

anxious state and how this does

1:13:36

not just involve biology but there

1:13:39

are these you know psychological factors

1:13:41

involved there are these life factor

1:13:43

involves there are these developmental factors

1:13:45

involved and and I myself don't

1:13:48

do not use the language of

1:13:50

chemical imbalances or brain disorders because

1:13:52

I because I find that to

1:13:54

be very misleading and deceptive. So,

1:13:57

and once in a while, a

1:13:59

patient might themselves bring up the

1:14:01

language of chemical imbalance. And in

1:14:04

that case, I kind of correct

1:14:06

them. You know, this is actually,

1:14:08

you know, it's not literally true.

1:14:10

Sometimes people, you know, use it

1:14:13

as a metaphor, but it doesn't

1:14:15

represent the nature of the problem

1:14:17

very well. And so we talk

1:14:19

about that. And then with regards

1:14:22

to medication, you know, like in

1:14:24

way that they are not fixing

1:14:26

an abnormality in. in your brain,

1:14:28

rather, you know, we're using them

1:14:31

to control symptoms and make these

1:14:33

conditions more, you know, bearable for

1:14:35

you and to improve your functioning

1:14:37

or shift your emotional processing in

1:14:40

a prominent, you know, in a

1:14:42

positive direction or we're using it

1:14:44

to control your body state of

1:14:47

arousal, etc. You know, whatever the

1:14:49

specifics of, you know, in the

1:14:51

medication condition might be. but they're

1:14:53

not they're not going to be

1:14:56

the magic fix you know they

1:14:58

you know to the extent that

1:15:00

the your mental health problems are

1:15:02

enmeshed with your life circumstances with

1:15:05

your you know psychological patterns the

1:15:07

medication would not change that and

1:15:09

then those things required to be

1:15:11

addressed through other means of help

1:15:14

and I think if we if

1:15:16

we take the time to have

1:15:18

these honest transparent conversations with the

1:15:21

patients, then they would not leave

1:15:23

with these mistaken ideas about chemical

1:15:25

imbalance or medications fixing a chemical

1:15:27

imbalance or them having some kind

1:15:30

of a biological disease in the

1:15:32

same sense as diabetes or biological

1:15:34

dysfunction. So that's what I have

1:15:36

been doing and you know emphasizing

1:15:39

the you know, a more sophisticated

1:15:41

conceptualization of problems as well as

1:15:43

treatment. And again, I emphasize medications

1:15:45

are in for effect tools. They

1:15:48

help, but they also have the

1:15:50

capacity to harm. the balance is

1:15:52

dynamic. You know, if the medication

1:15:55

is helping you right now and

1:15:57

you're not having a lot of

1:15:59

problems with that, that's wonderful, but

1:16:01

it does not mean that three

1:16:04

years later, the balance will not

1:16:06

shift. And if the balance ever

1:16:08

starts shifting, that the medication is

1:16:10

causing more problems than it is

1:16:13

helping with, then we need to

1:16:15

take action and, you know, either,

1:16:17

you know, change treatments or, you

1:16:19

know, figure out other non-medication strategies.

1:16:22

And so I think patients being

1:16:24

aware of the fact that benefit

1:16:26

risks is a dynamic thing over

1:16:28

time is very important too. Dr.

1:16:31

Raftab, thank you so much for

1:16:33

that. toward a force around these

1:16:35

varied and complex topics. I'm really

1:16:38

sad that we are running out

1:16:40

of time, as I think I

1:16:42

could probably carry on quizzing you

1:16:44

for hours. I'm just wondering if

1:16:47

there is anything that you would

1:16:49

want to point people to in

1:16:51

terms of your work, any other

1:16:53

sort of important facets you'd like

1:16:56

to highlight that I haven't brought

1:16:58

out as yet today. Yeah,

1:17:00

I think the, you know, over

1:17:03

the past two and a half

1:17:05

years or so, I've been working

1:17:07

on a sub-stack newsletter called Psychiatry

1:17:09

at the Margins. So that's kind

1:17:11

of a useful public-facing resource that

1:17:13

I've been working on that goes

1:17:15

into some detail around these conceptual

1:17:17

and scientific debates in the field.

1:17:19

So I'll refer people to that.

1:17:21

There's also a recent book from

1:17:23

Oxford University Press called Conversations and

1:17:26

Critical Psychiatry that is an editing.

1:17:28

collection of interviews that examines these

1:17:30

issues in more detail too and

1:17:32

I think those would be useful

1:17:34

resources for people that are interested

1:17:36

in exploring these questions. Amazing. We'll

1:17:38

definitely make sure to link to

1:17:40

those in the show notes as

1:17:42

I've really enjoyed reading your work

1:17:44

there as well myself. Thank you

1:17:47

so much for your time. It's

1:17:49

been a pleasure seeing you today

1:17:51

and speaking to you. And thank

1:17:53

you for having me. I enjoyed

1:17:55

this. Thank you. You

Rate

Join Podchaser to...

  • Rate podcasts and episodes
  • Follow podcasts and creators
  • Create podcast and episode lists
  • & much more

Episode Tags

Do you host or manage this podcast?
Claim and edit this page to your liking.
,

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features