Episode Transcript
Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.
Use Ctrl + F to search
0:00
This is Thanks
0:29
to our amazing patron sponsors
0:31
for their generous support of
0:34
TWIS. You can become a
0:36
part of the patron community
0:38
at patron.com slash This Week
0:40
in Science. Disclaimer! Disclaimer!
1:48
Instead, get ready for
1:50
the soothing science sounds
1:52
of this weekend science.
1:55
Coming up next. I've
2:00
got the kind of mind I
2:02
can't get enough. I wanna learn everything.
2:05
I wanna fill it all up
2:07
with new discoveries that happen every
2:09
day of the week. There's only
2:11
one place to go. science
2:33
to you Kiki! And
2:35
a good science to you too
2:37
Blair! out
2:40
there. Welcome to another episode
2:42
of This Week in Science.
2:44
We are back again with
2:47
so much more science! The
2:49
whole another week of science
2:51
has occurred while we were doing
2:54
whatever it is we do.
2:56
Were you the one scienceing
2:58
out there? If you were. Hi! Thanks
3:01
for science! Mm-hmm. Thanks
3:03
for bringing us this stuff
3:05
that we love talking about
3:07
and learning about. Justin is
3:09
still moving, doesn't have internet,
3:11
but hopefully next week we'll be able
3:13
to join us, but we'll see what
3:16
happens on that front. This is like
3:18
the best job opera ever. Yeah, he's
3:20
just hanging out on the ethereal plane.
3:22
You know, maybe he'll roll the right
3:25
number to get out of there, maybe
3:27
not. Just to wait for
3:29
his next turn. What can
3:31
you do? Roll the dice.
3:33
Hopefully they're like 21 sided.
3:35
Maybe 18. I don't know.
3:37
See what you got. Maybe some
3:40
dragons? I don't know. How
3:42
about I have stories this
3:44
week about the stellarator?
3:47
Love the stellarator. A possible disease
3:49
therapy and oxygen pill. More
3:51
memory. It's what I all
3:53
have, what we all need
3:56
maybe, and how not to
3:58
sleep better at night. What's
4:00
in the animal corner Blair?
4:03
I have some ideas about
4:05
how to not sleep better
4:07
at night. I could give
4:09
you one or 50. Really
4:12
just one I think. I
4:14
have, what did I even
4:16
bring? Oh, the AI emotions
4:18
that you mentioned before, friendly
4:21
and stressed macax and animal
4:23
consciousness. I was conscious of
4:25
that. Does it snakes? Does
4:27
it? No, I just had
4:30
trouble saying the word consciousness.
4:32
Say it three times fast,
4:34
everyone, and you'll feel just
4:36
like us. No. No. All
4:39
right, everyone, as we jump
4:41
in the show tonight, yes,
4:43
we're gonna up right in.
4:45
Yes. to this case of
4:48
the Science Wednesdays, no jumping
4:50
to conclusions. We're hopping into
4:52
the show. I want to
4:54
remind you that subscribing to
4:57
the TWIS podcast is pretty
4:59
easy to do. Just look
5:01
for us on pretty much
5:03
any podcast platform that's out
5:06
there. Look for this weekend
5:08
science or TWIS. We also
5:10
are live weekly Wednesdays at
5:12
8 p.m. Pacific Time or
5:15
whenever else. We tell you
5:17
we're going to be live
5:19
on YouTube, Twitch, and Facebook.
5:21
And those videos are there
5:24
for you to watch as
5:26
well. But if you want
5:28
to know more, you can
5:30
go to our website, twist.org.
5:33
That's where we have our
5:35
Zazzle store link and additionally
5:37
a link to our patron.
5:39
But now, it's time for
5:42
the science! You
5:44
ready for the big
5:47
news? I'm ready for
5:49
you have for me.
5:51
Yeah, the stellarator Is
5:54
that in the latest
5:56
Rocky film or? No,
5:59
I mean, I just
6:01
really love it. They
6:03
call this this magnetic,
6:06
this fusion power device
6:08
like a Tokamak, except
6:11
a little twisty like
6:13
a pretzel, that this,
6:15
this, what's otherwise known
6:18
as the Wendelstein 7X,
6:20
that they call it
6:22
the stellarator. And they've
6:25
just published a paper,
6:27
this company. Proxima fusion,
6:30
which again, they've got
6:32
their names, all dialed
6:34
in, I love it,
6:37
infusion, engineering, and design,
6:39
entitled stalaris, a high-field
6:42
quasi-isodynamic stellarator for a
6:44
prototypical fusion power plant.
6:47
Oh, that kind of stellarator.
6:49
Got it. Yes, yes, yes.
6:51
Okay. So you, if you
6:54
are into the fusion news
6:56
thing, you might have heard
6:58
recently. Oh, wow. China, they
7:00
turned on their fusion, the
7:02
generator, nuclear fusion generator, and
7:04
they ran there for some
7:06
long period of time that
7:09
was really great. And then
7:11
there is a news. No,
7:13
no, eater, eater, the French
7:15
and European Union, they turned
7:17
theirs on, and they ran
7:19
it for longer than the
7:21
French. Yes, go, go, do
7:24
it, do it, yeah. Anyway,
7:26
that's really great, but this
7:28
Proxima fusion and their stellarator
7:30
that has been, they've been
7:32
working on it since they
7:34
first announced their prototype design
7:36
back in like 2007, the
7:39
Wendelstein 7X, W7X. For short,
7:41
this is in Germany. It's
7:43
out of the Max Planck
7:45
Institute for plasma physics and
7:47
the German federal government and
7:49
European Union has been funding
7:52
this design since its initiation
7:54
for over $1.3 billion in
7:56
funding to date. And so
7:58
this particular type of fusion
8:00
is based on the technology
8:02
being used in Tokamax. So
8:04
those are the donut plasma
8:07
fusion generators in which the
8:09
design, it looks like a
8:11
donut. It's a round donut
8:13
thing and they use magnets
8:15
around the outside to contain
8:17
energy and smoosh it. And
8:19
so it heats up and
8:22
it gets contained in plasmas
8:24
inside the Tokamac. plasma spins
8:26
around, kind of like how
8:28
you would imagine the plasma
8:30
doing those loopy whorls on
8:32
the top, on the, you
8:34
know, on the outbursts that
8:37
come from the solar flares
8:39
on the sun. So the
8:41
plasma is just getting heated
8:43
up and it's getting pressured.
8:45
It's really awesome. And if
8:47
you could keep it contained
8:49
and stable for a long
8:52
enough period of time, it
8:54
potentially could be the conditions
8:56
in which you could fuse.
8:58
ions and or atoms and
9:00
be able to actually create
9:02
power as opposed to just
9:04
use power to smush plasma.
9:07
Yes. The problem with Tokamax
9:09
that artificial intelligence has been
9:11
helping a lot to understand
9:13
there are instabilities in the
9:15
plasma and that they have
9:17
been having to put more
9:20
energy into the magnets. to
9:22
be able to control and
9:24
contain the plasma so that
9:26
the plasma is happy. Otherwise
9:28
it has outbursts and the
9:30
outbursts, hmm, it's not happy,
9:32
right? So the stellarator goes
9:35
at it from a little
9:37
bit different perspective. The stellarator,
9:39
because it is like a
9:41
twisty donut, what it's trying
9:43
to do is do all
9:45
the hard work in the
9:47
design phase as opposed to
9:50
in the magnet phase. And
9:52
so the design is actually
9:54
more following the path of
9:56
the magnetic whorals and what
9:58
the plasma wants to... do.
10:00
So the shape of the
10:02
stellarator is designed to be
10:05
more easier for the magnets
10:07
and the plasma to flow
10:09
through. So it's a lot
10:11
of upfront control, but then
10:13
less energetic control when you
10:15
actually get the plasma flowing.
10:17
Huh. Yeah. So
10:20
the exciting thing about these
10:22
the stellar and this article
10:24
about the stalaris is that
10:26
this is the first design
10:29
for a commercial. Stellarator fusion
10:31
plant and they have published
10:33
online and they are saying
10:35
that they will have their
10:37
pilot. plant in process and
10:39
creating energy by uh... two
10:41
thousand thirty one which is
10:44
six years away less than
10:46
and that it at that
10:48
point in time should be
10:50
ready for uh... for adoption
10:52
around the world so the
10:54
races on inertial fusion the
10:56
races on Whatever kinds of
10:59
fusion that you would like
11:01
to be using right now.
11:03
Oh good, you found a
11:05
great picture there Blair. Yeah,
11:07
I was looking around. So
11:09
there's a really clear compare
11:11
contrast of a Tokamak versus
11:14
a stellarator. Which the arrows
11:16
really do it for me.
11:18
Sometimes all it takes is
11:20
a really good diagram for
11:22
me to understand what's going
11:24
on. So you're doing a
11:27
great job explaining it, but
11:29
it's like such a it's
11:31
such an abstract thought for
11:33
me that so if I
11:35
can describe to the listener
11:37
basically it's showing that if
11:39
it's if it's going around
11:42
the donut in it kind
11:44
of a straight line there's
11:46
a lot of opportunity for
11:48
for the the particles to
11:50
wander. But it's not like
11:52
a school of fish. They
11:54
don't want to stay or
11:57
they don't want to stay
11:59
packed tightly together. They're like,
12:01
I'm going to go. I
12:03
have space. I'm going to
12:05
move around. And so it
12:07
destabilizes the plasma. It like
12:10
probably also messes with conserving
12:12
all the energy that you're
12:14
trying to use, right? Yeah,
12:16
it disrupts the whole thing,
12:18
which is bad. Yeah. And
12:20
so for some reason this
12:22
twisted pattern makes the. plasma
12:25
particles all feel the force
12:27
in the same way, so
12:29
they all go in the
12:31
same direction. Which, yeah, it
12:33
kind of makes sense. Like,
12:35
if you're on a water
12:37
slide, you're going in, and
12:40
it's kind of like twisting
12:42
to the left, it's not
12:44
going to be very easy
12:46
for you to be like,
12:48
oh, I'm going to go
12:50
over to the right instead.
12:52
No, like the force is
12:55
compelling you to twist around
12:57
and follow the water slide,
12:59
right? So. Yep. Yeah. It's
13:01
pretty cool. Yeah, and so
13:03
this is exciting because, you
13:05
know, I remember when the
13:08
stelator was first reported and
13:10
I was like, what is
13:12
this? Mushi twisted like, like
13:14
somebody in the chat has
13:16
said, Mobius strip of a
13:18
Tokamak. And the science behind
13:20
it is. just very interesting
13:23
and I've been wanting to
13:25
hear something for a while
13:27
so it's really interesting to
13:29
see the stelaris work become
13:31
a big player now within
13:33
this commercial fusion environment which
13:35
you know if fingers crossed
13:38
if they can actually get
13:40
commercial fusion power plants out
13:42
the door by the early
13:44
2030s I mean that is
13:46
I I wonder, nobody's gotten
13:48
close enough yet, but the
13:50
advancement is now, it's very
13:53
obvious as opposed to the
13:55
questions of the past as
13:57
to how these technologies would
13:59
work. Yeah, so
14:01
we'll see where it goes.
14:03
The structure will definitely continue
14:06
to be adapted and refined,
14:08
but this. their stellar radar
14:10
model coil demo magnet in
14:13
2027 proximate fusion is going
14:15
to what they say de-risk
14:17
its technology for stellarators the
14:20
company will demonstrate that stellarators
14:22
are capable of net energy
14:24
production with its demo stellarator
14:27
alpha in 2031 like I
14:29
said and lead to a
14:31
fusion energy in the grid
14:33
in the 30s. Well let's
14:36
see what will the roaring
14:38
30s be like for us
14:40
here on earth? We'll find
14:43
out. But it's very exciting
14:45
also that they've published their
14:47
design, published publicly, and that
14:50
they are putting the science
14:52
out there for everyone to
14:54
see, putting the engineering out
14:57
there as well, which I
14:59
think is great. So this
15:01
actually, this raises a really
15:03
interesting question I hadn't really
15:06
thought about. I'm calling my
15:08
own question interesting. It's funny.
15:10
Well, it probably is. Well,
15:13
I think it's interesting. Otherwise,
15:15
I wouldn't open my mouth
15:17
about it. Is there is
15:20
there copyright on the design
15:22
of the stellarator? They've released
15:24
it. Is it now something
15:27
that other people can build
15:29
or they have to kind
15:31
of have a payout to
15:33
these researchers if they build
15:36
a stellarator? I'm imagining that
15:38
this is intellectual property and
15:40
that there are... probably a
15:43
number of companies involved in
15:45
this already. So I'm imagining
15:47
that there are licensing and
15:50
intellectual property partnerships that are
15:52
already underway and that, you
15:54
know, by the time power
15:57
plants become an option, that
15:59
that that path will be
16:01
pretty clear. Right, because at
16:03
first I was like, man,
16:06
what is it? matter just
16:08
let people do the research
16:10
but that's why because if
16:13
fusion power happens they want
16:15
to be able to have
16:17
control over the money to
16:20
be made from that. Yeah,
16:22
so you've got you've got
16:24
German and EU investment, you've
16:26
got the Max Planck Institute,
16:29
so there's technology transfer out
16:31
of the university and government
16:33
holdings, and then you have
16:36
the technology transfer into those
16:38
partnerships that are probably constantly
16:40
being hashed out. I'm sorry,
16:43
I'm going way off target
16:45
here. That's okay. It's an
16:47
interesting question. I never really
16:50
looked at it from that
16:52
angle. Is that why fusion
16:54
is taking so long? Because
16:56
don't be sharing. No. That
16:59
is not why. Okay. I
17:01
just care. Because, you know,
17:03
maybe if everybody shared all
17:06
their homework, maybe it would
17:08
happen faster. No, everybody's been
17:10
sharing their homework. There are
17:13
certain types of fusion that
17:15
are advancing more quickly than
17:17
others, and what one type
17:20
of fusion learns from their
17:22
experiments and that they're willing
17:24
to share publicly in their
17:26
scientific writings and other ways,
17:29
then that knowledge can be
17:31
useful. to the other methodologies
17:33
and everybody trying these multiple
17:36
different solutions to the question
17:38
the problem of fusion is
17:40
hopefully getting us there faster
17:43
right it's divide divide try
17:45
all the things and then
17:47
see what works best you
17:50
know some might be better
17:52
than others We'll see I
17:54
mean ignition fusion is probably
17:56
just gonna stay really really
17:59
great at Designing and testing
18:01
nuclear warheads for a very
18:03
long time But there's Z-Pinch
18:06
fusion there with a company
18:08
up in Seattle, Zapp, and
18:10
then there's the inertial fusion,
18:13
which I think I reported
18:15
on Commonwealth being in Virginia,
18:17
state of Virginia, saying they're
18:20
going to be the first
18:22
commercial fusion plant in the
18:24
United States, but that's inertial
18:26
fusion. And then, you know,
18:29
we've got the... the Tokamax,
18:31
and then we've got the
18:33
stellarators, which are the plasma
18:36
fusion. So there are many
18:38
options. The end result is
18:40
how can we create a
18:43
reproducing and sustainable design to
18:45
fuse atoms together in a
18:47
way that gives us more
18:49
energy than we put in?
18:52
How can we do that?
18:54
Yeah. Interesting. And it's still
18:56
not there. So, you know,
18:59
this is exciting. I'm excited
19:01
because there's this progress, but
19:03
I mean, come on. NIF
19:06
said it was going to
19:08
have us having commercial power
19:10
plants by now. Everybody was,
19:13
we're going to totally be
19:15
doing it, everybody. Grain assault,
19:17
everyone, grain of salt. Oh,
19:19
and Blair, honestly, because there's
19:22
a study. I really wanted
19:24
to just, you know, throw
19:26
out here and get your,
19:29
well, du response on. Oh,
19:31
great. Okay, I'm ready. Yeah,
19:33
published in the journal Environmental
19:36
Research Communications, the researchers found
19:38
that in countries like the
19:40
United States and the United
19:43
Kingdom, far more land is
19:45
allocated to golf courses than
19:47
renewable energy facilities. Oh, well,
19:49
duh. The
19:52
amount of land spent on
19:55
golf courses is sickening. I
19:57
don't understand. Okay. It's the
19:59
part about... Mmm, I got,
20:02
the part of, this is
20:04
the one thing I will
20:06
say, and we can move
20:09
on. You got it. No,
20:11
golf courses strike a nerve
20:13
for me because they are
20:16
spaces that not only are
20:18
just a terrible use of
20:20
resources because it's all grass.
20:23
Right? Which needs? Water and
20:25
fertilizer and all these things.
20:27
But they are spaces that
20:30
historically have been designed to
20:32
exclude people. Yeah, be it
20:34
women, be it specific racial
20:37
groups, be it specific societal
20:39
groups, golf courses, and yes,
20:41
they take up space that
20:44
could be used. in ways
20:46
that benefit the entire community,
20:48
the environment, the neighborhoods around
20:51
it. Yep. Whoo! You'll leave
20:53
it! You're welcome Blair! Dr.
20:55
Jan Weinand, lead author of
20:58
the study, head of the
21:00
Integrated Scenarios Department. I love
21:02
that. I want to be
21:05
in an Integrated Scenarios Department.
21:07
This is the Institute Julek
21:09
Systems Analysis at Vorschung Zen
21:12
from Julek. It says, our
21:14
study does not advocate for
21:16
the direct conversion of golf
21:19
courses, but it highlights the
21:21
vast potential for renewable energy
21:23
on similarly large and underutilized
21:26
areas. It's crucial to consider.
21:28
I advocate for the conversion
21:30
of golf courses. Yeah, it's
21:33
crucial to consider how we
21:35
allocate land overall and especially
21:37
when significant space is dedicated
21:40
to activities that benefit only
21:42
a limited segment of the
21:44
population. Yep. Just like you
21:47
said. And the Reddit group,
21:49
the science group, the commentary.
21:51
the way it's fantastic everybody
21:54
says well I mean really
21:56
wouldn't it just add to
21:58
the game to have wind
22:01
turbines or to have you
22:03
know like out in the
22:05
rough your solar panels the
22:08
entire thing is rough but
22:10
it's like it's like it's
22:12
like it would be like
22:15
a pet golf course oh
22:17
no the feral king ran
22:19
away from with my ball
22:22
I don't know it could
22:24
be like mini golf in
22:26
gravity falls But like, yeah,
22:29
but it's big golf. So
22:31
it's like Maxie golf. Exactly.
22:33
Extreme golf, right? Yeah. Everybody's
22:36
into extreme sports. Let's take
22:38
it to the next level.
22:40
Extremely resourceful and sustainable golf.
22:43
All right Blair. Yes. Oh,
22:45
you want me to talk
22:47
now? My turn? Yeah, your
22:50
turn. Okay, I have a
22:52
story about AI, but it's
22:54
not really AI. AI can
22:57
be used to determine the
22:59
emotions of animals. It's not
23:01
really AI. Researchers from the
23:04
Department of Biology at the
23:06
University of Copenhagen, they trained
23:08
a machine learning model to
23:11
distinguish between positive and negative
23:13
emotions in seven different undulate
23:15
species, including cows, pigs, and
23:18
wild boars. By analyzing the
23:20
acoustic patterns of their vocalizations,
23:22
the model achieved an impressive
23:25
accuracy of 89.49% marking the
23:27
first cross-species study to detect
23:29
emotional valence using AI. So
23:32
of course, my first question
23:34
reading this press release was,
23:36
how do they know? Yeah.
23:39
So they're saying... It was
23:41
89% accurate. What are you
23:43
measuring? Budgeted by? Yes. So
23:46
I had to dig into
23:48
the actual paper, which I
23:50
was very lucky that actually
23:53
I didn't have to pay
23:55
to read. Thank you. So
23:57
the calls that they used.
24:00
from the longer list is
24:02
domestic and wild undulates including
24:04
cows, sheep, horses, per waltzki's
24:07
horses, which let's bookmark that
24:09
and come back to that
24:11
in a minute, pigs, wild
24:14
boars, and goats, they used
24:16
calls that were known already
24:18
for specific emotional states. So
24:21
they knew the emotional valence,
24:23
they knew the emotional state
24:25
of the animal at the
24:28
time. So then they were
24:30
able to say like, this
24:32
animal is scared. Let's feed
24:35
the scared sound into the
24:37
machine learning model. It identified
24:39
that that animal was scared.
24:42
And so that was validated
24:44
during previous studies based on
24:46
behavioral indicators. So there were
24:49
these larger studies done that
24:51
based on like based on
24:53
this. and this and this
24:56
we know that this animal
24:58
is excited or hungry or
25:00
like aggressive right and so
25:03
then when they make a
25:05
sound you can associate those
25:07
two things but This
25:10
doesn't even feel like that
25:12
complicated of a machine learning
25:14
model to me if I'm
25:16
being honest because it's really
25:18
just like an in-out machine
25:20
like it's really just matching
25:22
I feel like you don't
25:24
even need a machine learning
25:26
model to do that you
25:28
just need to be like
25:31
judge this against this right?
25:33
Yes you can I will
25:35
tell you having experienced not
25:37
this exact like valence judgments
25:39
for bird song, but the
25:41
identification of syllables in bird
25:43
songs by listening and then
25:45
based on like spectrometry and
25:47
the work that goes in
25:49
to trying to get individual
25:51
people to validate and to
25:53
actually create a methodology that's
25:55
repeatable and that is. as
25:57
accurate as possible is you
25:59
can't get as many data
26:01
points if you're having humans
26:03
verify all of the things
26:05
as if you're using the
26:07
machine learning. So yes, it
26:09
is in out. But in
26:11
terms of, I mean, I
26:13
certainly hope that programming and
26:15
creating this model and putting
26:17
it together was less work
26:19
than the other graduate students.
26:22
I went to graduate school.
26:24
Yeah, so I mean, you're
26:26
right. It's ultimately to make
26:28
things easier and instantaneous and
26:30
something that doesn't have to
26:32
be verified by a bunch
26:34
of different people. So you're
26:36
totally correct about that. And
26:38
so they say there's a
26:40
potential to revolutionize all sorts
26:42
of welfare, veterinary uses, livestock
26:44
management, conservation efforts. I know
26:46
that if you work with
26:48
a small set of animals
26:50
yourself, like people who work
26:52
in livestock, I would say,
26:54
a lot of them know
26:56
what these sounds mean already,
26:58
but that comes with experience,
27:00
right? And I think that
27:02
if you have, for example,
27:04
the thing that they talked
27:06
about in this press release
27:08
quite a bit, was about
27:10
having something monitoring real-time 24-7.
27:13
And that's the difference. You
27:15
don't have to go like,
27:17
oh! I don't think something's
27:19
wrong out there in the
27:21
field. You have your bot
27:23
listening and your phone goes
27:25
off. Ping, something... What's that,
27:27
Lassie? Oh, Jimmy's in the
27:29
well? Right, yes. So there's
27:31
real-time monitoring of animal emotions
27:33
that could revolutionize the way
27:35
that we care for animals,
27:37
study animals, working conservation, there's
27:39
lots of different things that
27:41
you could do with this.
27:43
Yeah, I mean thinking about
27:45
it from a zoo keeper's
27:47
perspective. How do you think
27:49
this kind of monitoring could
27:51
have helped with situations, say,
27:53
in the primate house or
27:55
in, with like the giraffes
27:57
who never talked? Well, they
27:59
do, remember? That was something
28:01
we talked about on TWIS.
28:04
So here's the thing. Primates.
28:06
Specifically the primates that I
28:08
worked with. If there's 20
28:10
of them. Is the machine
28:12
learning also going to tell
28:14
me that it's banana Sam
28:16
that's bothered at this moment?
28:18
Maybe. Maybe not. Sounds of
28:20
voices are different. And if
28:22
you train it up, I
28:24
mean, I have now a
28:26
note-taking AI that Marshall gave
28:28
me to use in my
28:30
Zoom calls that if you
28:32
train it, it will identify
28:34
every single person's voice. Every
28:36
time they come in without
28:38
having them have to identify
28:40
themselves. So you're absolutely right.
28:42
So that's I mean, that's
28:44
possible. I think that when
28:46
somebody's first entering into a
28:48
care area, it could be
28:50
really helpful. I think that
28:52
when you spend a long
28:54
time with animals, at least
28:57
while you're physically there, that's
28:59
what I'm saying. Like. very
29:01
specific animals if you spend
29:03
a lot of time around
29:05
them you definitely know what
29:07
those vocalizations mean inherently and
29:09
I don't know if you
29:11
could even put words on
29:13
it but it's it's almost
29:15
like an impulsive or a
29:17
or a your own emotional
29:19
response when you hear the
29:21
sounds you can you can
29:23
kind of get the vibe
29:25
from it that's like with
29:27
your cat or your dog
29:29
or your own animal that
29:31
you learn to or your
29:33
your animals train you You
29:35
train your animals. You learn
29:37
how to communicate, right? There
29:39
are sounds. Yeah. Yeah, but
29:41
I'm wondering for, I was
29:43
thinking, I'm wondering for conservation
29:45
if they could actually get
29:48
to a more accurate confirming
29:50
of, you know, how realistic
29:52
some of our zoos and
29:54
like public parks, public animal
29:56
parks are as opposed. to
29:58
the wild spaces that we're
30:00
trying to, you know, you
30:02
know, make something that, you
30:04
know, that it's like that,
30:06
you know, by, okay, we'll
30:08
listen to the animals in
30:10
this natural setting, we'll listen
30:12
to the animals in our,
30:14
you know, pseudo natural setting
30:16
and see how, see if
30:18
they make the same sounds.
30:20
Yeah, I think that's a
30:22
really interesting point. And this
30:24
actually plays into a story
30:26
I have for the animal
30:28
corner. So we'll bookmark some
30:30
of this. But I think
30:32
that there is a benefit
30:34
to knowing that stress is
30:36
part of an animal's day,
30:39
excitement is part of an
30:41
animal's day, you know, all
30:43
these kind of different things,
30:45
frustration is part of an
30:47
animal's day. Life gets very
30:49
boring without those things if
30:51
you just are thrown everything
30:53
that you need right to
30:55
your face all the time.
30:57
That's why like in Richmond
30:59
exists and stuff like that.
31:01
But it's not a one-to-one.
31:03
I would say animals that
31:05
are constantly in fear of
31:07
being predated upon don't need
31:09
that in their animal exhibits.
31:11
There's a famous story about...
31:14
I'm not going to get it
31:16
exactly right, but there was a
31:18
zoo that hired these architects to
31:20
build this North America region. And
31:23
they had this, they were like,
31:25
oh, this is a great idea.
31:27
We have a section for the
31:29
grizzly bears, we have a section
31:31
for the wolves, and we have
31:33
a section for the deer, right?
31:36
And then we'll rotate them through
31:38
because they want to have different
31:40
experiences all the time. And rotating
31:42
exhibits is a very common practice
31:44
in zoos, and it allows them
31:46
to kind of have a different.
31:49
scenery every so many days. Okay,
31:51
but what about the way they
31:53
smell or so there was I
31:55
don't recall from the story if
31:57
they actually got to production and
31:59
they actually this but I think
32:02
it got stopped after the the
32:04
blueprints were shared I'm not sure
32:06
but basically there was a whole
32:08
question of okay if we rotate
32:10
the deer into the wolf exhibit
32:12
they're gonna spend all day terrified
32:14
I know it's here I know
32:17
they're here I don't think so
32:19
yeah could you imagine you only
32:21
have like one herbivore and you've
32:23
got like two predator species and
32:25
the herbivore is just surrounded my
32:27
predators all the time Yeah.
32:30
So the other thing I thought
32:32
that was really interesting about this
32:35
is it can help us do
32:37
research on wild animals and social
32:39
dynamics. There's a lot of research
32:41
that's done on, for example, primates
32:44
in the wild, but they count
32:46
on just kind of mostly visual
32:48
cues of how relationships work and
32:50
how animals feel about things. More
32:53
on that later. But if they
32:55
had a speaker that was recording
32:57
calls and they could say, oh,
32:59
so and so feels such and
33:02
such way about this based on
33:04
the call, that is an extra
33:06
layer of information for social strategies
33:08
and dynamics when you're doing research.
33:12
So the long and short
33:14
of this was they did
33:16
this on a bunch of
33:18
different animals including Perwalski's horses,
33:21
which I said I would
33:23
mention They're an extremely endangered
33:25
animal that you should Google.
33:27
They're just a weird-looking horse.
33:29
That's it Just a weird-looking
33:32
horse. They are not a
33:34
horse. They are their own
33:36
species and I've seen them
33:38
in zoos a couple times.
33:41
They're really striking but they're
33:43
an endangered species regardless. All
33:45
these different undulate species, very
33:47
different, pigs and horses. The
33:49
machine learning model was able
33:52
to kind of universalize these
33:54
responses. They were common. They
33:56
were very similar. They're excited
33:58
sound versus they're scared sound.
34:00
So it is something that
34:03
is conserved amongst undulous. It's
34:05
a pretty big group of
34:07
animals. So that also has
34:09
some cool implications for using
34:12
a machine learning model like
34:14
this. I think it's really
34:16
cool. I think, you know,
34:18
it is a simple idea,
34:20
simplistic at his core, but
34:23
because of the, you know,
34:25
the, the amount of data
34:27
and the... the ways it
34:29
can be applied more easily
34:32
than people, then it might
34:34
actually be something that can
34:36
be used in these really
34:38
broad contexts. I'd like to
34:40
see it above 89% but
34:43
otherwise. Yeah, that'd be great.
34:45
A horse is a horse,
34:47
of course, of course, unless,
34:49
oh, especially if it's a
34:51
Prowalski's horse. Even if horse
34:54
is a horse, of course,
34:56
of course, even if it
34:58
looks like a per Ralski's
35:00
horse. Yeah, that song doesn't
35:03
work as well as I'd
35:05
like it too. Okay. Moving
35:07
on from your strange horses
35:09
and really, really cute wild
35:11
boar babies. Let's round out
35:14
the end of this part
35:16
of the show with a
35:18
story about... hopeful new transplant,
35:20
not transplantation, but new implantation
35:23
technology for type 1 diabetes.
35:25
Researchers just publishing in Science
35:27
Direct this last week reported
35:29
their work to to reverse.
35:31
They're really trying to reverse
35:34
type 1 diabetes and they
35:36
succeeded in doing so in
35:38
mice, but using human cells.
35:40
So one of the issues
35:42
with treating diabetes is a
35:45
type of cell within the
35:47
pancreas that is called the
35:49
islet cell, pancreatic islet cell.
35:51
It is the only human...
35:54
tissue that produces insulin, which
35:56
is needed to respond to
35:58
rising blood glucose levels, and
36:00
in type one diabetes, the
36:02
body goes, I don't like
36:05
you, islet cells, and destroys
36:07
them, and so then the
36:09
body cannot make its own.
36:11
insulin and becomes insulin deficient
36:14
so people end up having
36:16
to monitor their their blood
36:18
glucose their insulin and very
36:20
often will have insulin pumps
36:22
take insulin shots to be
36:25
able to manage their their
36:27
deficiency on their own. So
36:29
How do we get more
36:31
islets into the pancreas? How
36:33
can we reverse the process
36:36
of that degradation? One of
36:38
the problems is that the
36:40
islets, they just get it,
36:42
you put any transplanted islets
36:45
back in the pancreas and
36:47
the body just goes, no,
36:49
don't like you and they're
36:51
attacked. killed by the bodies
36:53
some more. There's also inflammation
36:56
that causes worse problems. So
36:58
it's just been really difficult
37:00
to get these islet cells
37:02
to survive and replicate this
37:05
part of the pancreas that
37:07
is that supports the islet
37:09
cells even that part of
37:11
the pancreas gets a lot
37:13
of inflammation in it. And
37:16
so it can't even just
37:18
support these newly transplanted cells.
37:20
So while Cornell medicine just
37:22
published their study, which they
37:24
They took human islet cells,
37:27
grew them in a dish
37:29
with, what did they grow
37:31
them in a dish with?
37:33
They grew them in a
37:36
dish with human pancreatic, they
37:38
took human pancreatic islet cells,
37:40
grew them in a dish
37:42
with blood vessel cells so
37:44
that the support tissue of
37:47
the blood vessels. that would
37:49
bring blood to the islet
37:51
cells and support them and
37:53
give them all of the
37:56
nutrients and keep them safe
37:58
and hopefully keep them from
38:00
being inflamed and angry, they
38:02
were... grown into a dish,
38:04
and then they were transplanted
38:07
into the skin of mice.
38:09
Now, the mice had been,
38:11
had type 1 diabetes induced
38:13
upon them, and when they
38:15
put this tissue that included
38:18
very healthy islet cells and
38:20
blood vessels into the skin
38:22
of the mice, they found...
38:24
that after transplantation, the islet
38:27
cells in the skin, they
38:29
survived and they produced insulin
38:31
and they were actually able
38:33
to reverse the effects of
38:35
type 1 diabetes on these
38:38
mice and the mice did
38:40
not have to have any
38:42
further modification or addition of
38:44
insulin to be able to
38:46
manage their insulin levels. So
38:49
the idea is that we
38:51
are taking the human cells
38:53
and putting them in mice
38:55
and one day instead of
38:58
maybe having an insulin pump
39:00
that is, you know, transplanted
39:02
into the surface of your
39:04
skin, maybe it's actually a
39:06
bunch of your own cells
39:09
that are able to be
39:11
there and manage your blood
39:13
glucose. So one step at
39:15
a time, they think they
39:18
are going to get there.
39:20
I think it's a very
39:22
exciting step forward here. Yeah,
39:26
that's awesome. Yeah. And what's interesting
39:29
to me also is that they
39:31
didn't, that this doesn't have to
39:33
be a pancreatic graft or transplant,
39:36
that the transplant isn't just into
39:38
this into the skin. It's on
39:40
the in the abdomen of the
39:42
of the mice. And so something
39:45
like this is something that could
39:47
easily be done in it with
39:49
in humans. It could be. it
39:52
wouldn't have to be a hugely
39:54
invasive surgery and it would also
39:56
be easily accessible should there be
39:59
any, you know, any issues moving
40:01
forward. But that said, again, years
40:03
and years of research to go.
40:05
Don't expect it to be like
40:08
right away. This is still in
40:10
mice, but it's a very, very
40:12
neat advancement. Getting just getting the
40:15
blood vessels and the pancreatic islet
40:17
cells to be happy together. And
40:19
then for them to be like,
40:21
we'll be happy in the skin
40:24
of a mouse. Yeah, that's great.
40:26
So, you know, hopefully it'll all
40:28
work really well moving forward. I
40:31
think a lot of people, it
40:33
would be, it would help out
40:35
a lot of people. Treatment for
40:38
type 1 diabetes is something that
40:40
could help to save a lot
40:42
of lives. Make a lot of
40:44
lives better for longer. Usually. Yes.
40:47
All right. This is
40:49
this week in science. I hope
40:51
that we make your lives better,
40:54
longer, that you come here for
40:56
your little science talking points, that
40:58
you can, or thinking points, you
41:00
can take with you for the
41:03
rest of the week, and enjoy
41:05
the science, enjoy conversing about these
41:07
ideas with other people throughout your
41:10
week. You take it with you.
41:12
If you take twists with you
41:14
through your week, please make sure
41:16
to share where you got these
41:19
tidbits, these talking points from. Please
41:21
tell people to subscribe to TWIS.
41:23
It would be really wonderful if
41:26
we, if you could get us,
41:28
a friend of yours, a co-worker,
41:30
to join us in listening in
41:32
the conversation in the discussions that
41:35
we have. Additionally, if you head
41:37
over to twis.org, you can click
41:39
on our patron link if you'd
41:42
like to become a patron supporter
41:44
or the Zazzle link if you're
41:46
interested in any of our cool
41:49
t-shirts, mugs, pillows, I don't know,
41:51
beach towels. It's almost spring, right,
41:53
Blair? I don't know. Get down
41:55
to the pool with your twist
41:58
stuff. That's right. You know. You
42:00
keep, you really do, everyone, you
42:02
keep. this show going. Blair, I
42:05
can't do it without you. And
42:07
everyone out there, we can't do
42:09
it without you. Thank you for
42:11
your support. All righty Blair. All
42:14
righty everyone out there. You know
42:16
what time it is? It's time
42:18
for us to go to sleep.
42:21
No, it's time for Blair's. They
42:23
have no more corner! With Blair!
42:34
What's you got Blair?
42:37
I'm so stressed out
42:39
Kiki! No! No, why?
42:42
Are you alive? No.
42:45
Japanese macacks are stressed.
42:47
Why? Because they have
42:50
to eat with their
42:53
mother. Previous research has
42:55
shown that primates exhibit
42:58
lower stress levels when
43:01
they are with close
43:03
relatives and important social
43:06
partners. Oh, you're being
43:08
in the kids table.
43:11
Previous research has shown.
43:14
Primates in general exhibit
43:16
lower stress levels when
43:19
they're with their close
43:22
relatives and important social
43:24
partners and higher stress
43:27
levels in presence of
43:30
dominant or unfamiliar individuals.
43:32
So known entities hanging
43:35
out with them, good.
43:38
However, in this study...
43:40
They studied 11 adult
43:43
females in a wild
43:46
group of Japanese macax
43:48
on Kinsican Island in
43:51
Meiji Prefecture. So they
43:53
looked at 205 hours
43:56
of observation. total. This
43:59
is one group in
44:01
one location. So that's
44:04
my big asterisk on this.
44:06
But they look at skin scratching.
44:09
So skin scratching or
44:11
first scratching is a sign
44:13
of stress in Japanese macax.
44:16
Not just parasites. Right.
44:18
And so they looked at
44:20
self-scratching activity as
44:22
a behavioral indicator
44:24
of stress throughout
44:26
these two hundred and five
44:29
hours. And they found that
44:31
wild female Japanese
44:33
macax, they exhibit
44:36
higher stress levels
44:38
when a closely
44:40
related female is
44:42
nearby, if they are
44:45
foraging. If they are resting,
44:47
anybody nearby is
44:49
great. So, during resting, the
44:52
frequency of self-scratching was higher
44:54
when no other individuals were
44:56
nearby compared to when they
44:58
were present, so they wanted
45:00
company when they were just
45:03
hanging out. But during foraging,
45:05
self-scratching was higher when a
45:07
related female, a mother or sister,
45:09
was proximate, then when a
45:11
non-related female was present.
45:13
And no effective kinship was
45:15
observed. So, huh. Being
45:18
around a mother or a
45:20
sister, it's just about the
45:22
suggests that co-feeding with a
45:25
related female might have to
45:27
do with foraging competition.
45:30
But just generally the point
45:32
of this study is that
45:35
it breaks. Yeah, sure. Maybe
45:37
you're not doing that right.
45:39
Hold the stick differently. Oh
45:42
boy. It provides a novel view
45:44
that... Social stress is more complicated
45:46
than previously thought. So basically, the
45:48
whole point of this study is
45:51
just to break that first expectation
45:53
based on a breadth of research
45:55
that has been done previously. That's
45:58
the whole point of the study.
46:02
I think that's good. Doesn't
46:04
that what we want? Right?
46:06
Like not everything we thought
46:09
in the old days is
46:11
going to be, is right.
46:13
We did a limited number
46:15
of studies and this is
46:18
what we assumed and let's
46:20
look at it differently. But
46:22
here is my problem with
46:24
this and I should have
46:26
opened the actual, let me
46:29
see if I can see,
46:31
do they have a family
46:33
tree for the group that
46:35
they looked at? Because they
46:38
looked at one group of
46:40
11. monkeys. And I don't
46:42
see a family tree. I
46:44
really want to see a
46:47
family tree for these 11
46:49
monkeys because is there one
46:51
mother? Right. Are there multiple
46:53
mothers? So related. Is this
46:56
one mother just really not
46:58
nice? Like there's so many
47:00
questions. group where mom stresses
47:02
everybody out. Is that the
47:05
norm or is that that
47:07
one mom? Well it did
47:09
say kin also be it
47:11
was like sisters too. Sisters
47:14
too. But that could be
47:16
a reflection of a stressful
47:18
mom. Yeah. What's
47:20
the order of the birth order
47:23
of the of the kin? What
47:25
is the? Yeah, what's the age?
47:27
What's the relatedness? What is, you
47:30
know, if it is a female
47:32
competition in a certain way, like,
47:35
have you shared a partner at
47:37
some point in the past or
47:39
is there, you know, how does
47:42
that go into foraging competition? What
47:44
does that have to do? There
47:47
are so many more questions to
47:49
answer here, but. Yeah, well and
47:51
is 11 a normal size for
47:54
Japanese macaques? Is it a larger
47:56
group? Is it a smaller group?
47:59
If it's a smaller group than
48:01
normal, did they just run into
48:03
their family way more? than other
48:06
Japanese macaques and they're like, huh,
48:08
you again. I know you. Or
48:11
did they spend a lot of
48:13
time really trying to stay away?
48:15
Right. Really? Mom, leave me alone.
48:18
No, I'm going to this tree.
48:20
Just leave me alone. It's one
48:23
group. It's one group over one
48:25
series of tests with one group
48:27
of individuals, right? So it's very
48:30
specific. But the point is. Not
48:32
big enough, but yeah. The point
48:34
is that just because you're around
48:37
related monkeys does not mean you're
48:39
inherently less stressed. I think that
48:42
is applicable. In the human primate
48:44
world as well. Right. Yeah. And
48:46
if you, I don't know if
48:49
you relate to those monkeys at
48:51
all. then you might find yourself
48:54
wondering how much in common you
48:56
might have with monkeys or with
48:58
ducks or with dogs. We're just
49:01
animals in general and so you
49:03
might wonder what is consciousness? Am
49:06
I conscious? Is it just so
49:08
often you've no idea of Blair?
49:10
and turn down the overhead and
49:13
hang up your beaded curtain, let's
49:15
talk about it. No, there's an
49:18
essay that was released this week
49:20
in science and it talks about
49:22
some potential methods for measuring consciousness
49:25
because until now it has been
49:27
an imperfect system and I... pause
49:30
it, it will continue to be
49:32
an imperfect system because I'm going
49:34
to argue pretty much everything's conscious
49:37
depending on how you want to
49:39
define consciousness, but I think everything's
49:42
kind of aware it exists. Some
49:44
people even argue that rocks... some
49:46
degree. I would argue that consciousness
49:49
is a quantum or... Right? Yeah.
49:51
There are people who argue that
49:54
with fervor, indeed. So... Not just
49:56
plants, but the phonges. Right, right.
49:58
Absolutely. Absolutely. So, you know, I
50:01
mean, does it matter? Here's why
50:03
it matters, actually. Let me answer
50:06
my own question. Last year, I
50:08
think I reported on it. I
50:10
don't recall. New York, there was
50:13
a New York declaration on animal
50:15
consciousness. And that demonstrated scientific backing
50:18
for consciousness among firstly all vertebrates
50:20
and then many invertebrates. And so
50:22
this kind of created the shock
50:25
waves in the scientific community because
50:27
if you address consciousness, that changes
50:30
how you can research on those
50:32
individual animals. Not to say that
50:34
scientists were upset about it, but
50:37
it just does change things. Right.
50:39
Scientists, they're going to debate, continue
50:42
to debate it. Right. And it
50:44
does make things more complicated. And
50:46
what if you applied for a
50:49
grant for a specific study and
50:51
now you can't perform that study
50:54
as you had applied to the
50:56
grant to do? Right. So there's
50:58
lots that could be implicated there.
51:01
There's lots to be considered, but
51:03
ultimately there was this kind of
51:06
sweeping declaration that all vertebrates are
51:08
conscious and many invertebrates are conscious.
51:10
And so... the question is how
51:13
do you test that? Yep. And
51:15
so this essay... posited a new
51:18
method called a marker method, which
51:20
essentially works to identify behavioral and
51:22
anatomical features associated with consciousness processing
51:25
in humans, and then you search
51:27
for similar properties and non-human. And
51:30
you don't just find one, because
51:32
you could find one pretty easily,
51:34
but you have to find multiples
51:37
and try to map them on
51:39
each other and make them follow
51:42
some sort of rhyme or reason
51:44
that is consistent with consciousness across
51:46
all. these different markers. So that
51:49
might be experiencing pain. or seeing
51:51
an object, things that you do
51:54
bodily to respond to that, doesn't
51:56
animal do that? Like if you're
51:58
in pain, do you pull your
52:01
hand away from the thing that's
52:03
causing pain? Does an animal do
52:06
that? Yes, absolutely. Does that mean
52:08
they're aware of their own body?
52:10
Unclear. So now you have to
52:13
stack these different markers, right? And
52:15
so they say the degree to
52:18
which a particular marker can increase
52:20
or decrease confidence in particular dimensions
52:22
of animal consciousness depends on the
52:25
context. So basically just depends. For
52:27
instance, linguistic behavior is a marker
52:30
of specific kinds of conscious thought
52:32
and emotion in humans. But as
52:34
demonstrated by large language models. that
52:37
simulate human conversation, linguistic behavior alone
52:39
is not strong evidence of consciousness
52:42
and non-human systems. So it is
52:44
a marker. So you have to
52:46
stack markers and go, I have
52:49
enough markers that I find consciousness.
52:51
So this is what was kind
52:54
of promoted here. And that's why
52:56
it's important. It's important to know
52:58
how we care for these animals,
53:01
how we do experiments on these
53:03
kinds of animals. Interesting. But it's
53:06
obviously just been kind of proposed
53:08
it hasn't even been tested yet
53:10
But I thought it was a
53:13
really interesting question because I know
53:15
we've talked a bunch about like
53:18
the dot test in the mirror
53:20
and whether that proves a sense
53:22
of self or consciousness And that's
53:25
really complicated because like you have
53:27
to understand how a mirror works
53:30
You have to understand that that
53:32
dot isn't supposed to be there
53:35
But then there's the question
53:37
also is, you know, like
53:39
this is getting at, is
53:41
there a distribution of complexity
53:43
of consciousness? Yeah, where there's,
53:45
you know, like minimally conscious,
53:47
right, a plant possibly, you
53:49
know, and then at very,
53:52
as you add different markers
53:54
together in different ways, there
53:56
are different kinds. of consciousness
53:58
maybe, similarly to intelligences and
54:00
maybe even, you know, I
54:02
don't know, I'm imagining a
54:04
Venn diagram, you know, you
54:06
know, multiple, multiple different domains
54:08
that can overlap or not.
54:10
Yeah, no, absolutely. Yeah. That's,
54:12
I immediately think about when
54:14
you're talking about the plants,
54:16
how, you know, I mentioned
54:18
the idea that you shy
54:20
away from pain or you
54:22
pull your hand away from
54:24
pain. Plans do do that.
54:26
It just takes a long
54:28
time. You have to be
54:30
patient. Except for the mimosa
54:33
pudica. Right. Oh, they're more
54:35
biased. mimosa. We'll always have
54:37
an important part in my
54:39
heart. Yeah, so I hope
54:41
to hear more about this
54:43
in the coming years. I
54:45
hope to hear some researchers
54:47
using the marker method so
54:49
that we can decide if
54:51
it's worth anything or not
54:53
from our armchairs over here.
54:55
it has to you have
54:57
to come up with the
54:59
idea before you can test
55:01
it right so yes absolutely
55:03
it maybe gives a method
55:05
a methodology for comparison across
55:07
various species is you know
55:09
to build it yeah because
55:11
that's one of the hard
55:14
parts is going how do
55:16
you know that this is
55:18
consciousness as opposed to just
55:20
sensory awareness and are they
55:22
really aware? I mean, is
55:24
it just a reflex? Is
55:26
it, you know, is it
55:28
that the animal is conscious
55:30
of what's going on or
55:32
the plant is conscious? Is
55:34
there an awareness of it
55:36
at any level? Does it
55:38
matter that it is only
55:40
a chemical signal or a
55:42
nervous signal that it happens?
55:44
Like when the doctor hits
55:46
your knee cap to make
55:48
the kick reflex? You're aware
55:50
of the kick having happened,
55:52
but you are not aware
55:55
of, you know, the things
55:57
that you, that that whole
55:59
process of the tendon on
56:01
your knee being tapped and
56:03
then your leg kicking out.
56:05
is not something that you
56:07
are in control of. So
56:09
there's, you know, I think
56:11
there, you look at it
56:13
and you go, oh, that's
56:15
interesting. And that felt strange.
56:17
Hmm. Higher consciousness looking at
56:19
lower nervous, oh, processes. And
56:21
are those nervous processes somehow
56:23
chemically aware of themselves? So
56:25
many layers layer. You open
56:27
one of my favorite boxes.
56:29
Let's pull it all out.
56:31
Keep it open. Yeah. Yeah,
56:33
like is planning for the
56:36
future is that consciousness, right?
56:38
Like if I'm a squirrel
56:40
and I'm hiding my acorns.
56:42
Am I aware? Or am
56:44
I following a kind of
56:46
an impulse? Yeah, exactly. Yeah.
56:48
If I'm a monarch butterfly
56:50
who only flies one way
56:52
and then dies of a
56:54
migration, am I aware of
56:56
what I'm doing or am
56:58
I just compelled to go
57:00
somewhere? And are you even
57:02
aware of that compulsion? You
57:04
know, it's just, yeah, just,
57:06
it happens, it's as if
57:08
it happens to you, in
57:10
a sense. Yeah, yeah, these,
57:12
I think these are very
57:14
fascinating questions, you know. I
57:17
think therefore I am. Am
57:19
I? So good. We am.
57:21
We am what we am.
57:23
My goodness. Yeah, it's interesting.
57:25
Squirrels, I don't know about
57:27
their storing behavior, but I
57:29
know in chickities and nut
57:31
hatches, they are from the
57:33
minute they're out of the
57:35
nest and they are like,
57:37
summer happens, they're like, summer
57:39
happens, they're like, I can
57:41
pick up little pieces, and
57:43
I'm gonna hide a bigger,
57:45
and I'm gonna hide things,
57:47
they're like, put them places.
57:49
But do they remember? where
57:51
it is, right? Some birds
57:53
do, some birds don't. Well
57:55
those birds, because that's what
57:58
they're, that's what they do.
58:00
They do remember. So squirrels
58:02
often forget, which is very
58:04
funny. We did a story
58:06
on the show about our
58:08
squirrels were hiding nuts in
58:10
Transformers and people were losing
58:12
power because squirrels kept hiding
58:14
nuts in the Transformers. Anyway,
58:16
a great place for a
58:18
larder, but you know. You
58:20
like smoked acorns? Yeah. I
58:22
love that electricity taste. Yes.
58:24
Yes. Oh, I like Ar
58:26
and Laura's comment in the
58:28
chat here. Squirrels don't hide
58:30
acorns. They dig holes and
58:32
sometimes come across a buried
58:34
acorn. Oh, you're talking about
58:36
ground squirrels, yes. That's wonderful.
58:39
Moving on from the animal
58:41
brain, let's stay with the
58:43
animal brain, the human brain.
58:45
Yeah, that's an animal. So
58:47
I was going to go
58:49
to an oxygen pill for
58:51
this next story, but since
58:53
you brought up brain stuff,
58:55
I figured, let's just let's
58:57
just go with the brain.
58:59
Let's do it. Publishing in
59:01
the open-axis journal E-Life, researchers
59:03
from Brown University this week,
59:05
have published a model, so
59:07
it's not actually looking at
59:09
an animal doing any... action.
59:11
It was creating a circuit,
59:13
a numerical simulation, a neural
59:15
network model of interactions between
59:17
prefrontal cortex and the basal
59:20
ganglia. And they were looking
59:22
at models of how these
59:24
circuits might work together to
59:26
do what they call implementing
59:28
adaptive resource allocation in working
59:30
memory. And so for those
59:32
of you who are involved
59:34
in programming or in circuitry,
59:36
you understand maybe the idea
59:38
of gating where it's the
59:40
and or on off. and
59:42
different circuits, different components in
59:44
the circuit can be turned
59:46
on or off and or
59:48
at various times in the
59:50
processing. And if you have
59:52
a neural network, it becomes
59:54
much more complex as they
59:56
start working together. And if
59:58
they respond in relation to
1:00:01
each other because of sort,
1:00:03
if this one's on, then
1:00:05
I should be off. Or
1:00:07
if this one's on, then
1:00:09
I need to do more
1:00:11
open or whatever. I don't
1:00:13
know. I'm making things up
1:00:15
right now. The question really
1:00:17
is, what are the limits
1:00:19
of working memory? And working
1:00:21
memory specifically is the amount
1:00:23
of memory that your brain
1:00:25
is using at any particular
1:00:27
time to make sure you
1:00:29
can keep going. It's the
1:00:31
six-digit number or seven-digit number
1:00:33
limit that we're supposed to
1:00:35
have for remembering phone numbers,
1:00:37
right? The working memory capacity
1:00:39
has been tested for a
1:00:42
very long time. It's like,
1:00:44
okay, that thing you can
1:00:46
hold in your head someone's
1:00:48
name, maybe if you're, you
1:00:50
know, don't have a significant
1:00:52
executive disfunction like some of
1:00:54
us. or the topic that's
1:00:56
being discussed. Or I'm listening
1:00:58
to you Blair and I
1:01:00
remember I want to ask
1:01:02
a question but I can't
1:01:04
be rude so please don't
1:01:06
just shout it out all
1:01:08
the time and wait my
1:01:10
turn and ask a question
1:01:12
like a polite person. You
1:01:14
know holding that question in
1:01:16
my head, part of the
1:01:18
resource allocation to do that
1:01:20
is working memory and I
1:01:23
have to keep listening to
1:01:25
the story that's being told
1:01:27
at the same time. So
1:01:29
I can be like, oh,
1:01:31
okay, all right, oh, how
1:01:33
do I put it, how
1:01:35
do I put it all
1:01:37
together? It's not short-term memory.
1:01:39
It's not long-term memory. It's
1:01:41
the memory that is the
1:01:43
operating system function that is
1:01:45
basically holding things together from
1:01:47
moment to moment. And so
1:01:49
how big is that? And
1:01:51
people are like, well, yeah,
1:01:53
it's the number of circuits,
1:01:55
right? It's how much is
1:01:57
in there. It's the size.
1:01:59
That's going to determine. how
1:02:01
much you can remember. But
1:02:04
these researchers, even though they
1:02:06
didn't look at actual animals,
1:02:08
and they did this neural
1:02:10
network, their question how and
1:02:12
why is working memory capacity
1:02:14
limited, their question they determined
1:02:16
based on the model that
1:02:18
they were putting that they
1:02:20
put together, that because of
1:02:22
how certain chunking of information,
1:02:24
so if you're remembering a
1:02:26
phone number. What
1:02:29
is it? What was it?
1:02:31
What was it? 86739. The
1:02:34
chunks, the three numbers at
1:02:36
the front and the four,
1:02:38
or 867. 53009. Your memory,
1:02:41
your working memory has chunked
1:02:43
that information into little groupings.
1:02:45
So the other. capacity of
1:02:48
it. So your brain is
1:02:50
using a compression strategy to
1:02:52
improve recall efficiency. They also
1:02:55
found that they put dopamine
1:02:57
instructions into their working memory
1:02:59
model and they also found
1:03:02
that learning shapes memory limits.
1:03:04
all told what they summarize
1:03:06
and what they what they
1:03:09
say is that working limitation
1:03:11
working memory limitations are more
1:03:13
from learning challenges. So do
1:03:16
you have executive dysfunction? Is
1:03:18
dopamine not working properly to
1:03:20
allow you to continue to
1:03:23
pay attention to something long
1:03:25
enough to be able to
1:03:27
get your working memory to
1:03:30
hold on to it? And
1:03:32
if your chunking is disorganized
1:03:34
or disordered, that doesn't allow
1:03:37
you to have a higher
1:03:39
resolution capacity for that working
1:03:41
memory. So you can get
1:03:44
into overload more quickly. chunking
1:03:46
that all these things potentially
1:03:48
work together and Now that
1:03:51
they're looking at this new
1:03:53
interpretation of working memory, difficulties
1:03:56
in patients, they can suggest
1:03:58
this, instead of this anatomical
1:04:00
limit, that people, really, we
1:04:03
just chunk more, manage your
1:04:05
dopamine, your working memories, just
1:04:07
you're fine, keep going everyone.
1:04:10
Keep your brain a workout,
1:04:12
is that what you're saying?
1:04:14
That's right. Or, you know,
1:04:17
as per a couple of
1:04:19
studies from this past week,
1:04:21
maybe go on a mobile
1:04:24
internet diet for a little
1:04:26
time. Smart. One study took
1:04:28
people away from mobile internet,
1:04:31
so they still had phone
1:04:33
calls and texting, so they
1:04:35
could stay in touch with
1:04:38
people, and for two weeks,
1:04:40
they found that people became
1:04:42
happier. and had better working
1:04:45
memory, and they had, and
1:04:47
they had better mood control.
1:04:49
Similar study, they took people
1:04:52
away from their mobile devices
1:04:54
entirely, mobile detox for 72
1:04:56
hours, and they found that
1:04:59
there were changes to the
1:05:01
dopamine circuitry in the stimulus
1:05:03
reward centers of the brain.
1:05:06
So I was just thinking
1:05:08
last night that I should
1:05:10
get myself one of those
1:05:13
cricket, or not cricket phones.
1:05:15
What are they called? The
1:05:17
kids phones, the ones that
1:05:20
are like, you can give
1:05:22
yourself parental control. Exactly. So
1:05:25
it has like the six
1:05:27
buttons on it. It's like,
1:05:29
this is my phone now.
1:05:32
Brian. Yeah. That's it. See
1:05:34
about that? Like, what if
1:05:36
I just turned my phone
1:05:39
into nothing about the phone
1:05:41
app and the text app?
1:05:43
What if that's all it
1:05:46
was? I think it would.
1:05:48
I think it would be.
1:05:50
I think it would be.
1:05:53
That would be great. It'd
1:05:55
be a good thing. It'd
1:05:57
be very, very, very great.
1:06:00
I held out so long
1:06:02
on getting a smartphone, so
1:06:04
long. But it's now, yes.
1:06:07
It's, you can't, everybody has
1:06:09
it now, it's just kind
1:06:11
of expected, and I don't
1:06:14
know, maybe, maybe the next
1:06:16
generation will be like, you
1:06:18
know, it's cool, flip phones.
1:06:21
I think they already are.
1:06:23
Yeah, I don't know. Yeah,
1:06:25
we don't need all of
1:06:28
the, we don't need all
1:06:30
the things. Neural networks, very
1:06:32
exciting stuff. you know if
1:06:35
you were feeling very tired
1:06:37
and like oh my gosh
1:06:39
I must be at like
1:06:42
high altitude or something and
1:06:44
I didn't do any like
1:06:46
pressure training for this hypoxia
1:06:49
I'm feeling or you know
1:06:51
maybe you are an individual
1:06:53
who has sickle cell anemia
1:06:56
or you are someone who
1:06:58
has a disorder of your
1:07:01
mitochondria Perhaps this new work
1:07:03
out of the Gladstone Institute
1:07:05
with mice with lay syndrome,
1:07:08
L-E-I-G-H-S syndrome, with a drug
1:07:10
called hypoxysat, maybe it might
1:07:12
be something that will interest
1:07:15
you if they ever get
1:07:17
it out to market, which
1:07:19
they hope to do. I
1:07:22
don't know. Hopefully sometime soon.
1:07:24
The idea though is that...
1:07:26
looking to work on the
1:07:29
problem of hypoxia, which is
1:07:31
the inability of oxygen to
1:07:33
be getting to the cells
1:07:36
of the body. And the
1:07:38
underlying issue with that very
1:07:40
often is an inability of
1:07:43
the hemoglobin to grab onto
1:07:45
oxygen within the blood. So
1:07:47
these Gladstone Institute researchers were
1:07:50
like, hey, not everybody who...
1:07:52
who has sickle cell anemia,
1:07:54
you know, can get the
1:07:57
treatments that they need and
1:07:59
in Lee syndrome this is
1:08:01
is a childhood mitochondrial disease,
1:08:04
the treatment is actually, they
1:08:06
have to go live at
1:08:08
high altitude because if they
1:08:11
live at areas where there's
1:08:13
a higher oxygen, saturated oxygen
1:08:15
concentration, then it's too much
1:08:18
oxygen and their mitochondria can't
1:08:20
handle it. So it's the
1:08:22
opposite. So anyway, they were
1:08:25
working on these issues of
1:08:27
mitochondrial disease and. hypoxia and
1:08:30
people, you know, you go
1:08:32
up to the mountains and
1:08:34
you're not ready for it
1:08:37
and you don't get enough
1:08:39
oxygen into your blood. You
1:08:41
can inhale more oxygen if
1:08:44
you get one of those
1:08:46
nice canned oxygen thingies. But
1:08:48
they found that they were
1:08:51
able to create a drug
1:08:53
that they call hypoxy stat
1:08:55
and it's a repurposed compound
1:08:58
that was initially designed for
1:09:00
an unrelated aspect of sickle
1:09:02
cell anemia. And when they
1:09:05
used it, the drug made
1:09:07
hemoglobin bind oxygen more tightly
1:09:09
than it normally does. And
1:09:12
so it's the remarkable thing
1:09:14
about it that I think
1:09:16
is just the... the glacier
1:09:19
on the top of the
1:09:21
mountain for this particular for
1:09:23
this particular study is that
1:09:26
it's a small molecule that
1:09:28
can be easily put into
1:09:30
a pill form so it
1:09:33
could potentially be an easily
1:09:35
given drug to treat hypoxia
1:09:37
and it works it works
1:09:40
super easily. And here I'm
1:09:42
going to see about sharing
1:09:44
this tab here with everybody.
1:09:47
So you can see the
1:09:49
pretty pictures that they came
1:09:51
up with for this concept
1:09:54
of hypoxysat, which is a
1:09:56
small molecule hypoxia therapy. And
1:09:59
it allows the oxygen to
1:10:01
be grabbed onto and and
1:10:03
moved around more readily than
1:10:06
the hemoglobin for these individuals
1:10:08
with the mitochondrial disorder. It
1:10:10
could also be a treatment
1:10:13
for people who actually have
1:10:15
hypoxia for whatever reason. And
1:10:17
so in its hypoxia over
1:10:20
time can lead to damage
1:10:22
to the organs and to
1:10:24
the body. this hypoxy stat,
1:10:27
this is the glacier on
1:10:29
the top of the mountain,
1:10:31
hopefully it won't melt away,
1:10:34
it treated the damage to
1:10:36
the brain. When they were
1:10:38
given this drug it actually
1:10:41
reversed damage to the body
1:10:43
and to the brain, not
1:10:45
just to whatever was acutely
1:10:48
happening at the time it
1:10:50
reversed damage. So can I
1:10:52
take that pill instead of
1:10:55
breathing? No, right? No, no,
1:10:57
it wouldn't be a replacement
1:10:59
for breathing, no. The first
1:11:02
thing I thought of was
1:11:04
oxygen gum, that's from something,
1:11:06
right? I'm like, oh, sounds
1:11:09
like something in doctor who,
1:11:11
no, I looked it up,
1:11:13
duck tails. It was the
1:11:16
reboot of duck tails, is
1:11:18
what it's from. Oxy, too.
1:11:20
and yeah oh man oxygen
1:11:23
gum like oxygen gum could
1:11:25
be possible based on this
1:11:28
oxygen gum could be possible
1:11:30
so the idea for in
1:11:32
space on a mountain underwater
1:11:35
oh not underwater wherever good
1:11:37
times we're had wherever not
1:11:39
enough oxygen is had potentially
1:11:42
this drug could be a
1:11:44
treatment that would be made
1:11:46
available. So I think it's
1:11:49
very exciting. Thanks a lot.
1:11:51
Researchers who are working on
1:11:53
these awesome treatments and thanks
1:11:56
for remembering Ducktail's Blair. Yeah.
1:11:58
I have one last story.
1:12:00
You went ready for... for
1:12:03
our last story for the
1:12:05
night. Oh yeah. Yes, everybody
1:12:07
ready to go get some
1:12:10
good sleep? Yes. Yes. Is
1:12:12
the story about sleep? Oh.
1:12:14
Well, it is, yeah. Do
1:12:17
you have a certain person
1:12:19
in your life who is
1:12:21
continuing to work night shift?
1:12:24
Not currently. So, going back.
1:12:26
But he did for... So
1:12:28
going back on night shift,
1:12:31
going back on night shift,
1:12:33
acclimating to the night shift
1:12:35
is really rough. So then
1:12:38
we've talked a lot about
1:12:40
the problems of metabolism, circadian
1:12:42
issues, when people do have
1:12:45
night shift work jobs and
1:12:47
the health detriment that come
1:12:49
as a result of that.
1:12:52
New study suggests you could
1:12:54
take melatonin. He
1:12:58
definitely tried that. All right.
1:13:00
Here's the here's the deal
1:13:02
for this study. Published in
1:13:04
BMJ British Medical Journal. The
1:13:07
researchers looked at melatonin supplementation
1:13:09
and oxidative DNA repair capacity
1:13:11
among night shift workers. This
1:13:13
was a randomized placebo controlled
1:13:16
trial. Basically they found. We're
1:13:18
using appropriate melatonin supplementation that
1:13:20
it improved oxidative DNA damage
1:13:22
repair capacity. So the damage
1:13:25
that we were talking about
1:13:27
that occurs, if melatonin is
1:13:29
taken, the intervention is taken
1:13:31
appropriately with the right quantity
1:13:34
and in the right timing,
1:13:36
that capacity can be improved
1:13:38
among night shift workers. And
1:13:40
they say further larger scale
1:13:43
trials are needed to evaluate.
1:13:45
varying doses of melatonin. And
1:13:47
also to look into questions
1:13:49
of long-term use of melatonin
1:13:52
supplements. But the commentary on
1:13:54
this is very much a
1:13:56
lot of, now people pop
1:13:58
a melatonin and think it's
1:14:01
going to make them sleepy.
1:14:03
And they're like, okay, I'll
1:14:05
be asleep in a half
1:14:07
an hour because I took
1:14:10
a melatonin and they're taking
1:14:12
like maybe a one milligram
1:14:14
or even a five milligran
1:14:16
melatonin. Like a half an
1:14:19
hour before you go to
1:14:21
bed. Well. the brain releases
1:14:23
melatonin approximately two hours before
1:14:26
you go to sleep. So
1:14:28
if you really want to
1:14:30
use melatonin for its purpose,
1:14:32
which is to entrain your
1:14:35
body clock, your circadian rhythm,
1:14:37
to a particular sleep and
1:14:39
light cycle, dark and light
1:14:41
cycle, if you take your
1:14:44
melatonin, which is released at
1:14:46
like 0.01 milligrams to 0.03
1:14:48
milligrams. per body weight. By
1:14:50
the body, by the brain,
1:14:53
a couple of hours, and
1:14:55
it is released a couple
1:14:57
of hours before you go
1:14:59
to sleep, that's the signal
1:15:02
that, oh, in the next
1:15:04
couple of hours, this is
1:15:06
going to start happening. Non,
1:15:08
hey, get sleepy in a
1:15:11
half an hour, go to
1:15:13
bed. Melatonin is not a
1:15:15
go-to-bed pill. It is a
1:15:17
train your clock. Interesting.
1:15:20
Treatment. And so as an
1:15:22
intervention, if you take it
1:15:25
at a good time period
1:15:27
and not too much of
1:15:29
a dose before you go
1:15:32
to sleep, you can set
1:15:34
your clock, but you need
1:15:36
to stick to your clock.
1:15:39
Because that's what it is.
1:15:41
That's what it's for. Oh,
1:15:44
I see Brian. Hi, Brian.
1:15:46
Who me? How come
1:15:48
doctors are prescribing it right
1:15:50
then? Is it just because
1:15:52
there hasn't been enough research?
1:15:54
And so this is some
1:15:56
of the stuff that now
1:15:58
is going to go on
1:16:00
a medical journal and now
1:16:02
doctors will start prescribing. it
1:16:04
differently? Hopefully, yeah, because people
1:16:06
learn, doctors learn, yes, melatonin,
1:16:08
it's good for getting sleep,
1:16:11
but maybe they forget, you
1:16:13
know, it's like, there are
1:16:15
specifications for how you're supposed
1:16:17
to take it, but yeah,
1:16:19
this is for sleep, right?
1:16:21
This is to help you,
1:16:23
you know, and it's not,
1:16:25
it's not something that is
1:16:27
addictive, it doesn't have like...
1:16:29
You know, it's not supposed
1:16:31
to have terrible side effects,
1:16:33
right? Well, it's over the
1:16:35
counter. It's over the counter.
1:16:37
You just buy it in
1:16:39
the supplement eye. There are,
1:16:41
you know, drinks that you,
1:16:43
little powder drinks, you take
1:16:45
a big old scoop, put
1:16:47
it in a cup. It's
1:16:49
like magnesium and melatonin and
1:16:51
who knows how much is
1:16:53
in there. You add water,
1:16:55
you drink it, go to
1:16:57
sleep. In honor of Justin
1:16:59
who is not here tonight,
1:17:01
I have to mention then
1:17:03
that a lot of melatonin
1:17:05
is packaged and sold by
1:17:07
supplement companies who are not
1:17:09
regulated. Great great points for
1:17:11
that. So is that melatonin
1:17:14
you're taking actually melatonin is
1:17:16
it actually the amount that
1:17:18
it says that it is?
1:17:20
Is there anything else in
1:17:22
there? Right. What else is
1:17:24
in there? As that, magnesium
1:17:26
is going to react, is
1:17:28
going to relax you as
1:17:30
well. How do all these
1:17:32
things work together? And if
1:17:34
the amount is so important
1:17:36
and we can't confirm that
1:17:38
each pill has the exact
1:17:40
milligrams that they say that
1:17:42
it has. That's exactly it
1:17:44
as well and how much
1:17:46
is going to be bio
1:17:48
available? So say it's a
1:17:50
one milligram. pill, but not
1:17:52
that much is actually bioavailable
1:17:54
by the time it gets
1:17:56
broken down and gets into
1:17:58
your body. Maybe it is
1:18:00
closer to point one or
1:18:02
point three. You know, that's
1:18:04
still, I think, more than
1:18:06
what your body's releasing, but
1:18:08
it's at least at least
1:18:10
closer by a couple of
1:18:12
factors of 10. Yeah, so
1:18:14
there are a lot of
1:18:17
things that I think that
1:18:19
yeah, the information and education
1:18:21
on how melatonin actually works
1:18:23
and how it how it
1:18:25
influences the entrainment of your
1:18:27
circadian rhythm. You know, that
1:18:29
is very important. But melatonin,
1:18:31
when used by shift workers,
1:18:33
it improves. there a capacity
1:18:35
to repair DNA oxidative damage
1:18:37
which suggests that there are
1:18:39
ways that if you're doing
1:18:41
regular work and you can
1:18:43
get yourself into the cycle
1:18:45
that you need to be
1:18:47
in that you can that
1:18:49
melatonin can be an intervention
1:18:51
to help to help with
1:18:53
that you know off-shift cycle.
1:18:55
Interesting. Yeah that's really not
1:18:57
how it's... advertised or
1:19:00
prescribed currently. So yeah, think
1:19:02
about the things that you
1:19:04
take over the counter. It
1:19:06
will help you sleep for
1:19:08
sure, but it's not, but
1:19:11
if you're taking it at
1:19:13
different times every day and
1:19:15
you're just using it as
1:19:17
a sleeping pill or a
1:19:19
sleeping aid that is not
1:19:22
how it should be used.
1:19:24
It's for your clock. It's
1:19:26
for your clock people. Your
1:19:28
internal clock. It would be
1:19:31
like. It's like you're supposed
1:19:33
to take that and then
1:19:35
when you wake up you're
1:19:37
supposed to take vitamin D,
1:19:39
right? So if you're a
1:19:42
shift worker. Mm-hmm. Yeah, I
1:19:44
was looking at those for
1:19:46
a whole spectrum UV light
1:19:48
to be able to stimulate
1:19:50
it. Yes. Because that's nice
1:19:53
and then it's also I
1:19:55
really like the idea of
1:19:57
waking up to a solar
1:19:59
light to that like gentle,
1:20:01
gentle, and gummies for
1:20:04
the yummy sugar. It's wonderful
1:20:06
in the winter. I mean,
1:20:08
those kinds of UV lights
1:20:10
when they come on like that
1:20:13
with your morning. It's like,
1:20:15
ah, you want like sleeping
1:20:17
beauty birds to be tweatling
1:20:20
at you? Yes. But on
1:20:22
that, uh, zombie Tom
1:20:24
Hanks. Don't eat melatonin
1:20:26
gummies for the yummy sugar.
1:20:28
As long as it's a
1:20:30
night, it's probably very little
1:20:32
in each gummy bear as it
1:20:34
is. We'll see about that. Make sure
1:20:37
you, we have no idea, who knows,
1:20:39
who made sure to quantify
1:20:41
that supplement that you got
1:20:43
over the counter. Who's regulating? This
1:20:45
is gummy bear, does not have
1:20:47
enough. This gummy bear has too
1:20:50
much. This gummy bear has too
1:20:52
much. This gummy bear. This gummy
1:20:54
bear has too much. This gummy
1:20:56
bear. This gummy bear. This gummy
1:20:59
bear. He's just right.
1:21:01
That's right. On
1:21:03
that note. We had
1:21:06
enough gummy bears,
1:21:08
Blair. Yeah, yeah. Those
1:21:10
gummy bears are
1:21:13
kicking in. I gotta
1:21:15
go! Well, don't leave
1:21:17
until we say a
1:21:20
whole bunch of thank
1:21:22
yous. Before we go, I do want
1:21:24
to say thank you so much to
1:21:27
everyone who's here watching the show live
1:21:29
with us as we are broadcasting. Thank
1:21:31
you to everyone in the chat room.
1:21:33
Thank you to our discorders. I know
1:21:36
I'm not in there today, but Blair
1:21:38
is. I will get my computers and
1:21:40
everything back up again to be able
1:21:42
to get into my discord. It's going
1:21:45
to happen one thing at a time.
1:21:47
Shout outs! Vada, thank you so much
1:21:49
for your time for the social media,
1:21:51
for the show notes, for all that you
1:21:53
do. You do. Gord, Arnleur, thank you
1:21:55
for manning the chat rooms and for everyone
1:21:57
who helps that keep the chat rooms
1:21:59
nice. happy, safe places for everyone
1:22:02
to be. Identity Four, thank you
1:22:04
for recording the show. Rachel, thank
1:22:06
you for editing the show. And
1:22:08
as always, I really absolutely need
1:22:11
to thank our TWIST patrons. Thank
1:22:13
you too! Lauren Gifford, Dana Lewis,
1:22:15
Eden, Mundell, Adam Biola, Aaron anathema.
1:22:18
I am on the wrong page,
1:22:20
even though you are on the
1:22:22
right page. I gotta move things
1:22:25
around over here on my little
1:22:27
tiny screen. Wee, okay. Eden, Mundell,
1:22:29
Alan, Biola, Arthur, Arthur, Alan Biola,
1:22:31
Arthur, Arthur, Arthur, Kepler, Craig, Bob
1:22:34
Coles, Kent, Northco, George, George, George,
1:22:36
George, P.L, P.L, Vél, Vél, Vél,
1:22:38
Vél, Vél, Vél, Vél, Vél, My
1:22:41
Shack Andrew Swanson, credits, 104, Skylie,
1:22:43
Paul, Ronevich, Kevin Reardon, noodles, Jack,
1:22:45
Brian Carrington, David, Youngblood, Sean Clarence
1:22:48
Lamb, John McKeh, Rayleigh, Mark Hesseliesman,
1:22:50
A.K.A.C. McEnhays, Howard, Dan, Christopher, Rappin,
1:22:52
Richard Brendan, Minish, Johnny, Gridly, Johnny,
1:22:54
Gridley, G'd, and Richard, Minish, Johnny,
1:22:57
Gridley, Johnny, Gridley, Johnny, Gridden, and
1:22:59
Richard, Johnny, Johnny, Grid, Johnny, Grid,
1:23:01
Johnny, Johnny, Grid, Johnny, Grid, Johnny,
1:23:04
Grid, Johnny, Grid, Johnny, Grid, Johnny,
1:23:06
Grid, Johnny, Grid, Grid, Minish, Johnny,
1:23:08
Grid, Johnny, Grid, Johnny, Grid, Grid,
1:23:11
Minish, Grid, Grid, Johnny, Grid, Grid,
1:23:13
Lawn makes E.O. Adam Michcon, Kevin
1:23:15
Parachan, Aaron Luthan, Bob Calder, Marjorie,
1:23:17
Paul D. Disney, Patrick Pecoraro, and
1:23:20
Tony Steele. Thank you. Thank you,
1:23:22
one and all, for all of
1:23:24
your support on Patreon. And if
1:23:27
you would like to become one
1:23:29
of the people listed at the
1:23:31
end of the show, head over
1:23:34
to tw.org. Click on the Patriot
1:23:36
link and support us at $10
1:23:38
or more per month. on next
1:23:40
week's show. We will be back.
1:23:43
Like where is it? Where is
1:23:45
it? I lost it. We will
1:23:47
be back next Wednesday at 8
1:23:50
p.m. Pacific Time broadcasting live from
1:23:52
our Twitch YouTube and Facebook channels.
1:23:54
Hey do you want to listen
1:23:57
to us as a pod? Perhaps
1:23:59
while you categorize various animal sounds.
1:24:01
Just search for this weekend science
1:24:03
for our podcasts are found. If
1:24:06
you enjoy the show, get your
1:24:08
friends to subscribe as well. For
1:24:10
more information than you heard here
1:24:13
today, show notes and links to
1:24:15
stories will be available on our
1:24:17
website. That's at www.twist.org. You can
1:24:20
also subscribe to our newsletter. We
1:24:22
love your feedback. If there is
1:24:24
a topic you would like us
1:24:26
to cover or address or a
1:24:29
suggestion for an interview. Please let
1:24:31
us know on one of our
1:24:33
social media accounts or send us
1:24:36
an email. Just be sure to
1:24:38
put TWIST in the subject line
1:24:40
or your email will oversleep and
1:24:43
never arrive in our inbox. Took
1:24:45
too much millitone. We look forward
1:24:47
to discussing science with you again
1:24:50
next week and if you've learned
1:24:52
anything from the show, remember? It's
1:24:54
all in your head. This
1:24:57
week in science. This week
1:24:59
in science. This week in
1:25:02
science, it's the end of
1:25:04
the world. So I'm setting
1:25:07
up shop, got my batter
1:25:09
unfurl. It says the scientist
1:25:11
is in, I'm gonna sell
1:25:14
my advice. Show them how
1:25:16
to stop the robots with
1:25:19
a simple device. A reversible
1:25:21
warming with a wave of
1:25:23
my hand. This
1:25:30
week
1:25:32
in
1:25:34
science.
1:25:36
This
1:25:38
week
1:25:40
in
1:25:42
science.
1:25:45
This
1:25:47
week
1:25:49
in
1:25:51
science.
1:26:00
science. This week in science.
1:26:02
This in science
Podchaser is the ultimate destination for podcast data, search, and discovery. Learn More