Episode Transcript
Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.
Use Ctrl + F to search
0:00
This BBC podcast is supported
0:02
by ads outside the UK. Hey,
0:04
it's Amy Brown from the Bobby I
0:06
lost in conditions 18 plus. Hey,
0:09
it's Amy Brown from the Bobby
0:11
Bones show. Join me in supporting
0:13
St. Jude Children's Research Hospital for
0:15
a chance to win a trip
0:17
to meet Megan Moroni at the
0:19
2025 I-Hart Country Festival in Austin,
0:21
Texas on May 3rd hosted by
0:24
Bobby Bones. We're gonna hook you
0:26
up with tickets, flights, hotel, food
0:28
credits, and a meat and greet
0:30
with Megan Moroni. Take action now
0:32
to support St. Jude and help
0:34
cure and help cure childhood. to learn more.
0:37
BBC Sounds, music
0:39
radio podcasts.
0:41
Welcome to our bonus
0:44
episode with more from
0:46
our expert who's
0:48
dedicated lots of
0:50
time to answering, why
0:53
do we do that?
0:55
I'm Al Ashamahi and
0:57
if you're after, more
0:59
play with Brenna Hassett,
1:01
you're in the right
1:03
place. Do check out
1:11
delve into the gender differences we
1:14
see in play across different cultures,
1:16
both in hunter-gatherer communities. and in
1:18
the West. But play isn't just
1:21
a human thing. And of course,
1:23
thinking about animals play is pretty
1:25
damn cute. But it also means
1:27
we can study the behavior outside
1:30
of ourselves. Dr Brenner has
1:32
a anthropologist and archaeologist at
1:34
the University of Central Lancashire
1:37
explains to me how scientific
1:39
thinking on animal play has
1:41
evolved over time. The
1:45
original sort of research was done quite some
1:47
time ago and people said, oh, I really
1:49
did see this animal doing something absolutely ridiculous.
1:51
You know, here's a squirrel messing with a
1:54
stone. And people just came along later and
1:56
said, well, that's not very scientific. I said,
1:58
your study sample is one squirrel. and he
2:00
was probably drunk on fermented pumpkins
2:02
that you left out on your
2:04
porch or something. So just leave
2:06
the squirrel alone. And then later
2:08
studies have come back and actually
2:10
really there's been a resurgence in, actually,
2:12
no, animals play. I mean, we
2:15
know that birds play, crows famously love
2:17
to play. They will happily sort
2:19
of toss things around. And it's
2:21
not just what we think of simple
2:23
play. So pouncing, pretend pouncing like
2:25
your cat would do, that's play. You
2:27
know, anyone with a kitten or a
2:30
puppy has experienced animal play. But
2:32
other animals do also do the
2:34
kind of social play, which we
2:36
used to think was just us.
2:38
But sadly, science is proving we
2:40
are really not the special cases
2:42
that we thought we were. And
2:44
social play is more interactive. Basically,
2:46
social play is just when you
2:48
involve someone else in your game.
2:50
So social play is where you've
2:53
had to go off and interact
2:55
with another individual. The thing we
2:57
play in our species is that
2:59
no species that has ever
3:01
existed has had a juvenile
3:03
period as long as us.
3:05
We are children much longer
3:07
than any other species. So
3:10
by extension that means that
3:12
we're playing presumably or we
3:14
have played more than any
3:16
other species. Why is that? What
3:18
is the purpose of that? Well,
3:20
this is... You're smirking a lot
3:23
and smiling. It's because this is
3:25
my home, this is something I've
3:27
been really interested in, research-wise, because
3:29
as humans, we do have an
3:31
extraordinarily long childhood, we have proportionately
3:33
longer childhood than any other species
3:35
really on the planet. We have,
3:38
let's say, about 25 years, where
3:40
we're really not quite fully adult,
3:42
possibly in the legal sense, but
3:44
even biologically, we're not really physically
3:46
finished growing. last bone in your
3:49
skeleton finishes ossifying around your early
3:51
30s. That's the little end of
3:53
your sternum there. It's not quite
3:55
done until your sort of early
3:58
30s. So we have this extraordinary.
4:00
childhood and another animal who has
4:02
a similar length childhood is something
4:04
like a bowhead whale. They are
4:06
much bigger than us. They are
4:08
very large animals, but they're taking
4:10
that long to grow. And they're
4:12
going to live, actually, for 200,
4:14
300 years. We're not going to
4:16
make it that far. So we've
4:18
actually taken childhood as proportion of
4:20
our lifespan and extended it massively.
4:22
And this is the kind of
4:24
really interesting evolutionary point, is what
4:26
are we doing? Why have we
4:28
done this? And the answer can really
4:30
only be that it's given us an
4:33
advantage. It's given us a chance. essentially
4:35
to play. And that play is helping
4:37
us learn to be a better
4:40
monkey. That is our time to
4:42
do all of this learning. It's
4:44
also the time when all of
4:46
our lovely play partners and our
4:48
adults and everyone else who's indulging
4:50
us in this play can invest
4:52
in us, can give us extra
4:54
resources, food, time, training. teach us
4:56
how to bash a rock into
4:58
a sharp cutting implement, teach us
5:00
how to speak, tell stories, learn
5:03
things about our environment. These are
5:05
all human things that seem to
5:07
take such a very long time
5:09
to learn, but they must be at
5:11
advantage because we are certainly the best
5:13
primate. We're not the best primate. We
5:16
are the most numerous and most successful
5:18
primate on the planet. Well, yeah, I
5:20
mean, if you think about this in
5:23
a very basic sense. Being
5:25
an adult in most other
5:27
species isn't quite as complex
5:29
as being a homo sapian
5:31
adult. We have a lot
5:33
more to learn. Our societies
5:36
are very, very social. There's
5:38
language. Actually, you're kind of
5:40
really building the case for
5:42
play. Play. Having a much more
5:44
interesting undercurrent than people might expect
5:47
and actually being much more important,
5:49
I think as much as we
5:51
romanticize play in children, there is
5:54
also an element of, the child
5:56
should be learning something, that child should
5:58
be doing this, that's... should be doing
6:00
that and I have to say being
6:02
out in America so much I see the
6:05
extreme of that where a lot of parents
6:07
kind of almost feel like they're being
6:09
judged if they don't hyper structure child's week
6:11
so you know Monday nights is ballet
6:13
Tuesday nights is this activity Friday
6:16
nights is football etc etc etc
6:18
etc are we actually shooting ourselves
6:20
in the foot by not just
6:23
giving children you know scheduled playtime
6:25
Well, I think the question to
6:27
throw back there is, are those
6:29
children going to have to follow
6:32
a rigorous schedule as adults? Those
6:34
children who are being shuttled between
6:36
mandarin, ballet, viola, whatever it is,
6:39
may in fact be being trained
6:41
for a society that values the
6:43
kind of showing on time to
6:46
appointments having done the homework. These
6:48
are all skills that, while we may
6:50
not... enjoy them in the present.
6:52
You know, they may be actually
6:54
what's needed to train for the
6:57
particular adult social role that their
6:59
adults are trying to get them
7:02
into. There's a really interesting difference
7:04
between both the foragers and farmers
7:06
in the way they're taught. So
7:09
forager children among the bothie tend
7:11
to pick things up by watching
7:13
or participating. There's almost
7:15
never a formal instruction, what we would
7:17
consider teaching. And this is a really
7:20
big sort of theory of education, theory
7:22
of play, sort of battle, about whether
7:24
or not formal instruction, where it comes
7:27
into our lives, when it arrives, and
7:29
what it means for how we interact.
7:31
But the farmers, they often tell their
7:33
children. what they need to be doing.
7:36
So they also do, you know,
7:38
sitting and watching to learn something
7:40
and participating to learn something. But
7:42
they're often given formal instruction as
7:44
well. And this is just a
7:46
different way of conveying information, but
7:48
it should be pointed out that not
7:50
all societies have what's called a didactic,
7:52
you know, sort of, there's an instructor
7:55
who knows things who will formally
7:57
convey information to you. That's not
7:59
a... universal. And so when we
8:01
look at how our children learn, we
8:03
beat ourselves up over every aspect of
8:06
parenting, and I don't think anyone should,
8:08
because all we're doing is trying to
8:10
use sort of methods that, whether we
8:12
know it or not, are aimed at
8:14
making the adults in our society. They're
8:17
aimed at reproducing our own culture. If
8:19
you want to stop scheduling between Mandarin
8:21
and Viola, you're going to have to
8:23
look at your own culture first. Oh,
8:25
so that's a complex answer. I was
8:28
expecting you to kind of give me a
8:30
yes, they should, no, they shouldn't. And you're
8:32
like, well, actually, well, okay, let
8:34
me question you on another thing
8:36
about our modern existence, which
8:38
is technology. Tech is clearly changing
8:41
the way kids play. What do you
8:43
think the impact of that is?
8:45
So I think it's really interesting
8:47
that technology is always sort of,
8:50
it's always a slight moral panic.
8:52
And I once went to a
8:54
lovely talk, which was a beautifully
8:56
illustrated presentation where the speaker just
8:58
presented headlines from the 1880s, from
9:01
the 1910s, radio waves will melt
9:03
your children's brain. Please limit radio
9:05
time to under an hour a
9:07
day or your children will be
9:10
morons, which was a term common
9:12
at the time. television. If you
9:14
allow your child to watch television, they
9:16
will be. Video games in the
9:18
90s, I'm the generation that was absolutely
9:21
going to, society was going to be
9:23
destroyed by violent video games. And now,
9:25
all of my students play Minecraft. It's
9:28
a really interesting thing where we
9:30
sort of, I think we're quite apprehensive about
9:32
a lot of technology. If we
9:34
think about, you know, adults training
9:36
their children to be the adults
9:38
that they were, if technology changes.
9:40
We can't necessarily teach our children
9:42
to play in the way we
9:44
played. And that's a source of
9:46
anxiety. My children are never going
9:48
to know what the AOL dial-up
9:51
sound sounded like. That's probably a
9:53
good thing. But you know, they've
9:55
got apps, they've got games, and
9:57
you just have to realize that
9:59
humans... adapt Hey,
12:06
it's Amy Brown from the Bobby
12:09
Bones show. Join me and
12:11
supporting St. Jude Children's Research
12:13
Hospital for a chance to
12:15
win a trip to meet
12:17
Megan Moroni at the 2025
12:19
I-Hart Country Festival in Austin,
12:21
Texas on May 3rd hosted
12:23
by Bobby Bones. We're going
12:25
to hook you up with
12:27
tickets, flights, hotel, food credits,
12:29
and a meat and greet
12:31
with Megan Moroni. Take action
12:33
now to support St. Jude
12:35
and help cure. it.
12:41
So you're a mom, and I feel
12:43
like I might already know the answer
12:45
based on some of this, but you
12:47
do have children and you study play
12:50
academically, intellectually. Does that mean that you
12:52
overthink? You're almost studying your children. So
12:54
the unfortunate history to my last book,
12:57
which was sort of a look at
12:59
the evolution of pregnancy and child rearing
13:01
and all of this, is that I
13:04
really wanted the hard data. I wanted
13:06
the anthropological facts. I wanted to research
13:08
this. I was going to be amazing.
13:11
at this whole sort of, you know,
13:13
child-rearing thing. I was going to find
13:15
out, you know, what is the actual
13:18
truth in terms of like universal practice.
13:20
And I came away realizing, A, I
13:22
was far too busy for that. And
13:25
B, that there just, there really is
13:27
no universal. We're all in kind of
13:29
individual, yeah, social, cultural places. I'm not
13:32
sure that any of it really makes
13:34
me a better mother, though I will
13:36
say that my four-year old is super
13:39
good at opening Excel. Which I feel
13:41
like, you know, she's definitely learned to
13:43
exist in the society she's going to
13:46
go into. We're trying to keep them
13:48
away from archaeology just for sanity's sake.
13:50
Well, okay. Speaking of archaeology, let me
13:52
ask you. Young Brenner, your memories of
13:55
your own play as a child. Did
13:57
they ever involve digging in the dirt
13:59
like can you see the clear trajectory
14:02
to who you've become? You know, that's
14:04
that's a worrying question because I was
14:06
just thinking of I sort of as
14:09
a small child Tragically when you have
14:11
small pets, you know, they do expire
14:13
on a fairly regular basis sort of
14:16
goldfish and that and I did spend
14:18
a lot of time building them zigger
14:20
arts. Oh God, where's this going? No
14:25
animals were harmed in the making
14:27
of my professional career What I
14:29
became a forensic archaeologist I needed
14:31
to kill them and take them
14:34
apart No, no, just I just
14:36
built them very large monumental architecture
14:38
in order to sort of and
14:40
and then you know spent spent
14:42
the next number of decades going
14:44
off and trying to find cool
14:46
things to dig up, you know,
14:48
digging up skeletons in the ancient
14:51
Near East. So, you know, I'm
14:53
not sure you could have seen
14:55
it coming, but perhaps the signs
14:57
were there. Play is this training
14:59
ground where we forge our adult
15:01
selves, where we learn and develop
15:03
vital skills required to survive and
15:06
thrive in our environments. It's a
15:08
necessity. As Brenner puts it, being
15:10
a better monkey. But the absolute
15:12
beauty is as a kid, you're
15:14
not aware of any of that.
15:16
It isn't even a thought in
15:18
your mind, because you are simply
15:21
having fun. How absolutely adorable. Thank
15:23
you for joining me for more
15:25
play with Brenner Hassett. The producer
15:27
was Olivia Johnny, with additional production
15:29
from Emily Bird. The series producer
15:31
is Geraldine Fitzgerald. The executive producer
15:33
is Alexandra Fetchum. The commissioning editor
15:35
is Rian Roberts. And to keep
15:38
up to date with all the
15:40
other episodes in the series, subscribe
15:42
to Why do we do that
15:44
on BBC Sounds? And have push
15:46
notifications turned on. That way you
15:48
won't miss. Why we fall for
15:50
the bad boy? Get upset when
15:53
our football team loses. and
15:55
blush. Why do
15:57
we do that
15:59
from BBC Radio
16:01
4 is produced
16:03
by BBC Studios
16:05
Audio? Hey,
16:11
it's Amy Brown from the Bobby
16:13
Bones show. Join me and
16:15
supporting St. Jude Children's Research
16:17
Hospital for a chance to
16:19
win a trip to meet
16:21
Megan Moroni at the 2025
16:23
I-Hart Country Festival in Austin,
16:25
Texas on May 3rd hosted
16:27
by Bobby Bones. We're going
16:29
to hook you up with
16:31
tickets, flights, hotel, food credits,
16:33
and a meat and greet
16:35
with Megan Moroni. Take action
16:37
now to support St. Jude
16:39
and help cure. more.
Podchaser is the ultimate destination for podcast data, search, and discovery. Learn More