Episode Transcript
Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.
Use Ctrl + F to search
0:01
to you buy Missouri. Missouri does
0:03
fine jewelry differently. They're all about
0:05
buying for yourself where you decide
0:07
the occasion. Everything is handcrafted with
0:10
quality, craftsmanship, and responsible sourcing in mine.
0:12
So these are pieces you can feel good
0:14
about in more ways than one. Plus, there
0:16
are so many designs you can mix and
0:18
match to create a stock for every look.
0:20
Shopins. in-app or online at majeury.com
0:23
today. Banking with Capital One
0:25
helps you keep more money in your
0:27
wallet with no fees or minimums on
0:29
checking accounts and no overdraft fees. Just
0:31
ask the Capital One bank guy. It's
0:34
pretty much all he talks about. In
0:36
a good way. He'd also tell you
0:38
that this podcast is his favorite podcast
0:40
too. FDIC.
0:53
Trust Trust isn't just
0:55
earned, it's demanded. Whether you're a
0:57
startup founder navigating your first audit
0:59
or a seasoned security professional scaling
1:02
your GRC program, proving your commitment
1:04
to security has never been more
1:06
critical or more complex. That's where
1:08
Vanta comes in. Businesses use Vanta
1:11
to establish trust by automating compliance
1:13
needs across over 35 frameworks like
1:15
SOC II and ISO 2701 centralized
1:17
security workflows, complete questionnaires up to
1:19
five times faster and proactively management.
1:21
vendor risk. Vanta not only saves
1:24
you time, it can also save
1:26
you money. A new IDC white
1:28
paper found that Vanta customers achieve
1:30
$535,000 per year in benefits and
1:32
the platform pays for itself in
1:35
just three months. Join over 9,000
1:37
global companies like Atlasian, Cora, and
1:39
Factory who use Vanta to manage
1:41
risk and improve security in
1:44
real time. For a limited
1:46
time, get $1,000 off Vanta
1:49
at vanta.com/TED audio. That's via
1:51
nt.a.com/TED audio for $1,000 off.
1:54
for $1,000 off. When I think
1:56
about the things I value in
1:58
the world, if you take them
2:00
away from me, I will be
2:02
utterly devastated forever. The devastated part
2:04
is right, the forever part is
2:06
wrong. Hey everyone, it's Adam Grant.
2:09
Welcome back to Rethinking, my podcast
2:11
on the science of what makes
2:13
us tick with the TED Audio
2:15
Collective. I'm an organizational psychologist, and
2:17
I'm taking you inside the minds
2:19
of fascinating people to explore new
2:21
thoughts and new ways of thinking.
2:23
My guest today is Dan Gilbert.
2:25
He's a Harvard psychologist and the
2:28
best-selling author of Stumbling on Happiness.
2:30
He's given three popular TED Talks
2:32
and hosted the award-winning PBS show,
2:34
This Emotional Life. His research suggests
2:36
that we're not very good at
2:38
predicting what will make us happy.
2:40
The best data suggests that, as
2:42
my father would have said, happiness
2:44
increases when the kids leave home
2:47
and the dog dies. So how
2:49
do we get better at finding
2:51
happiness? I wanted to ask Dan
2:53
what he's discovered. I
2:58
have to ask you because I've
3:00
never asked you about this before.
3:02
Are you the only Harvard professor
3:04
who dropped out of high school? You
3:06
know, it's funny when I first
3:08
came to Harvard 1996, the Harvard Gazette
3:11
always runs a little puff piece on
3:13
new professors. And my lack of
3:15
a high school diploma came up and
3:17
I said to the journalist, I
3:19
guess I'm probably the only professor at
3:22
Harvard without a high school diploma. I
3:24
promptly received emails from several other
3:26
professors at Harvard who did not have
3:29
high school diplomas, some of whom
3:31
became good friends, one of whom went
3:33
on to win the Nobel Prize in
3:35
economics. So no, my friend, there
3:37
are more people without high school diplomas
3:40
at Harvard than at the post
3:42
office where you really have to have
3:44
one. Wow. I had no idea. So
3:46
tell me the backstory. How did
3:48
you end up dropping out of high
3:51
school? It's not really complicated. Like
3:53
if you don't go, they look for
3:55
you and eventually if they can't find
3:57
you, they give... up. My father
3:59
was a professor, molecular biologist, my mother
4:02
was an artist, and this is
4:04
the early 70s, which, you know, is
4:06
really still the 1960s. And so I
4:08
started reading Eastern Philosophy and decided
4:10
that my teachers in high school didn't
4:13
know anything that I wanted to
4:15
know. And so I just decided I
4:17
would stop going. And I started hitchhiking
4:19
around the country and playing music
4:21
and meeting other people. I just, you
4:24
know, I began a life that
4:26
wasn't the life that my parents were
4:28
hoping for. Clearly. Yes. Going high school
4:30
is hard. Dropping out really is
4:32
very easy. How long did the dropout
4:35
last before you decided you wanted
4:37
to reenter formal education? It was a
4:39
couple of years and in that interim
4:41
I got married I had a
4:43
child I became a science fiction writer
4:46
and started writing and selling my
4:48
stories and publishing and one day I
4:50
went down to a local community college
4:52
because they had advertised that there
4:54
was a writers workshop that you could
4:57
join and I thought that would
4:59
be a great thing and when I
5:01
got there The woman at the desk
5:04
said no I'm sorry it's closed
5:06
it's full already but it had been
5:08
a long bus ride I said
5:10
well what else is open and she
5:12
looked on the list and luckily for
5:15
me introduction to psychology was not
5:17
popular it still had a spot so
5:19
I said oh that might be
5:21
fun sign me up for this course
5:23
and the dominoes all fell from there
5:26
I fell in love with what
5:28
I was learning and very shortly thereafter
5:30
found out I could go to
5:32
a real university if I just took
5:34
a test called the GED. And I
5:37
did, then made the transition from
5:39
writing fiction to writing nonfiction, which actually
5:41
isn't nearly as big a jump
5:43
as most people think. Not if you
5:45
write like Dan Gilbert. That's kind of
5:48
you to say, thanks. So tell
5:50
me now that you're a psychologist, are
5:52
you happier than you were writing
5:54
sci-fi? I think I loved writing science
5:56
when I was doing that. than this,
5:59
the answers clearly know, for many
6:01
reasons, one of which is I don't
6:03
think I had a particular talent
6:05
for writing fiction. I think most people
6:07
inside our field and probably outside it
6:10
too, know you best for your
6:12
work on effective forecasting and all the
6:14
mistakes we make when we try
6:16
to predict what's going to make us
6:18
happy or feel good in the future.
6:21
When did you get interested in
6:23
the fundamental questions of happiness? Like almost
6:25
everything in my life, it's just
6:27
a dumb accident. I just stumbled on
6:29
it. Were you drunk? I have never
6:32
needed drugs in order to stumble.
6:34
I have a natural propensity for turning
6:36
the wrong way and walking into
6:38
doors and falling through windows. In 1992,
6:41
I went out for lunch with a
6:43
friend of mine. I wasn't studying
6:45
anything vaguely related to affective forecasting or
6:47
happiness or decision making. I studied
6:49
how people made... judgments about each other,
6:52
like my mentor had in graduate school.
6:54
I hadn't seen this friend in
6:56
a year and he said, how are
6:58
things going? And I said, how
7:00
are things going with you? He said,
7:03
actually, things have been going terribly. My
7:05
uncle died. My girlfriend and I
7:07
broke up. I had my papers rejected.
7:09
I went, wow, how are you
7:11
doing? He said, actually, I'm doing pretty
7:14
well. How about you, Dan? Well, it
7:16
turned out I had the same
7:18
story and had been a terrible year
7:20
for me. All sorts of bad
7:22
things had happened. I was getting a
7:25
divorce. My son had dropped out of
7:27
high school. And I was actually
7:29
doing okay. And my friend looked at
7:31
me and he said, do you
7:33
think we could have ever predicted that
7:36
when all these things happened to us,
7:38
we would still be fine. And
7:40
I realized that for me, the answer
7:42
was no. So I ran back
7:44
to my office after that lunch. I
7:47
typed up notes on the conversation, which
7:49
is how I know. I'm not
7:51
inventing this story to tell you. I
7:53
still have the piece of paper
7:55
that... Wow. And I wrote the question,
7:58
do people know what will make them
8:00
happy? how happy it will make
8:02
them and how long that happiness will
8:05
last. I thought truly psychologists have
8:07
a good answer to this. It's a
8:09
fundamentally important question. I couldn't find
8:11
a good answer. So I called my
8:13
friend and collaborator Tim Wilson. I said,
8:15
hey, maybe we had to do a
8:17
study on this. Fast forward 30 years.
8:19
We published, I don't know, 75
8:21
papers with hundreds of studies on
8:23
this topic, all because of one
8:25
stupid lunch with bad Chinese food.
8:28
It's a great case for going
8:30
to lunch. There's so many demonstrations
8:33
of these affective forecasting failures. If
8:35
you were to choose your top
8:37
three, what are your favorites? Certainly,
8:40
one of the mistakes that's caught
8:42
my attention the most is her
8:44
inability to imagine adaptation.
8:46
My friend Danny Kahneman used to say,
8:49
when you ask somebody how they would
8:51
feel if they were blind, they imagine
8:53
going blind. But the day you lose
8:56
your eyesight. probably a very very bad
8:58
day. But it's not like all the
9:00
hundreds and thousands of days that will
9:03
follow because human beings are remarkably adaptive.
9:05
They adapt to almost any new situation.
9:07
And yet when we look forward, if
9:10
I say, how would you feel if
9:12
you lost your children, if you lost
9:14
your legs, if you lost your eyesight,
9:16
if you lost your wife, what you
9:19
imagine as a calamity. And you are
9:21
right to imagine it. That is what's
9:23
going to happen at first. But if
9:25
you are like most people, the data
9:27
say. you're going to get used to
9:29
it. And most people who go through
9:32
traumatic events, even the most traumatic events,
9:34
adapt, come back to their baseline
9:36
of happiness. Now to me, that
9:38
was mind-blowing. And so Tim and
9:40
I did quite a few studies
9:42
trying to understand why and when
9:44
this happens and with what effect.
9:46
So that would probably be my
9:48
number one nomination for an effective
9:50
forecasting error, believing that if you
9:52
get knocked down, you will not
9:54
get up again. You almost surely will.
9:57
How do you square that body of
9:59
evidence with? Come
12:10
for details. Some tech presents the
12:12
ins and outs of caring for
12:15
your home, out, procrastination, putting it
12:17
off, kicking the can down the
12:19
road, in, plans and guides that
12:22
make it easy to get home
12:24
projects done, out, carpet in the
12:26
bathroom, like why, in, knowing what
12:29
to do, when to do it,
12:31
and who to hire. Start caring
12:33
for your home with confidence. Download
12:35
thumbtack today. Hey Prime members, are
12:38
you tired of ads interfering with your...
24:14
Banking with Capital One
24:16
Bank Guy helps you keep more
24:18
money in your wallet with no
24:20
fees or minimums on checking accounts
24:23
and no overdraft fees. Just ask
24:25
the Capital One Bank Guy. It's
24:27
pretty much all he talks about.
24:30
In a good way. He'd also
24:32
tell you that this podcast is
24:34
his favorite podcast too. Ah, really?
24:37
Thanks Capital One Bank Guy. What's
24:39
in your wallet? Term supply. See
24:41
Capital one.com/ Bank. Hey,
24:45
it's me Paige DeSorbo, and I'm
24:47
so excited to share my new
24:49
shoe collection at DSW, filled with
24:51
my favorite styles and trends for
24:53
spring. Because if you know me,
24:55
you know I'm kind of obsessed
24:57
with shoes. And by kind of
25:00
obsessed, I mean head over heels.
25:02
You're going to love these shoes.
25:04
So snag super cute styles like
25:06
cute flats, fun heels, and cool
25:08
sneakers from the Paige DeSorbo collection
25:10
right now at your DS store
25:13
or dsw.com. You're
25:16
tired of ads interfering with
25:18
your favorite podcasts. Good news.
25:21
With Amazon Music, you have
25:23
access to the largest catalog
25:25
of ad-free top podcasts, included
25:28
with your prime membership. To
25:30
start listening, download the Amazon
25:32
Music app for free. Or
25:35
go to amazon.com/ad-free podcasts. To
25:37
catch up on the latest
25:39
episodes, without the ads. The
25:43
verdict is murder on these days.
25:46
Let's go to a lightning ground. One
25:48
sentence or one word reaction. Oh my
25:50
God, you're telling a professor to answer
25:53
in one sentence? I have faith in
25:55
you. Doesn't the Geneva Convention prohibit this?
25:57
Okay, go ahead. What's the worst career
26:00
advice you've ever gotten? Follow your heart.
26:02
Why is that bad advice? Because a
26:04
successful career is the intersection of your
26:07
passion and your talent. If I had
26:09
followed my heart, I'd be a
26:11
very bad guitar player right now. I
26:13
mean, no, be realistic. Satisfaction is going
26:16
to come from doing something really well
26:18
that you love. And it might be
26:20
the fifth thing on your love list
26:23
is what you're actually good at. How
26:25
about the best life advice? My father
26:27
was a professor, and when I became
26:30
a professor, he was a scientist, molecular
26:32
biologist. He said to me. You
26:34
will think for your entire career that
26:36
your research matters most. And then you
26:39
will get old and realize it was
26:41
the students. And I think there's deeper
26:43
advice in there, which is you may
26:46
think all the things you're up to
26:48
in your life are what matter most.
26:50
But in the end, it was the
26:53
people you touched and connected with. My
26:55
dad gave me very sage advice.
26:57
I had to get very old before
26:59
I really heard it. It makes me
27:02
think about Vonnegut. and play a piano
27:04
in the line that if it weren't
27:07
for the people, the damn people, the
27:09
world would be an engineer's paradise. I'd
27:11
forgotten that. That's beautiful. Those engineers clearly
27:14
miss this lesson. All right, what's something
27:16
you've rethought lately? I've been rethinking for
27:18
the last couple of years the
27:20
model of scientific publishing in the world.
27:23
I used... to participate in it, and
27:25
now I see it as an obscenity
27:27
that needs to be destroyed, brought down
27:30
an empire that needs to lay in
27:32
ruins. So having gone from, boy, I
27:34
hope I can find a publisher to
27:37
let's kill them all, I would say
27:39
that's a rethinking. Big time. Can you
27:41
give us a taste of your
27:43
vision for how it might work differently?
27:46
Well, sure. I mean, if we're out
27:48
of the lightning around and wanting to
27:50
just chat again, I can say that
27:53
my latest project, as you know, has
27:55
been to produce a major reference work
27:57
in our field, social psychology, that's free
28:00
and accessible to anyone with an internet
28:02
connection. You know, for four centuries, publishers
28:04
have been cellists. science needs publishers,
28:06
like modern music needs AM radio. It
28:09
doesn't whatsoever. They were doing something we
28:11
couldn't do ourselves. They were printing our
28:13
ideas and words on paper and distributing
28:16
them out throughout the land. We don't
28:18
need them anymore. Modern science needs publishers,
28:20
like modern music needs AM radio. It
28:23
doesn't whatsoever. The only reason that AM
28:25
radio went away and publishers haven't is
28:27
there a multi-billion dollar business that
28:29
does not want to go gently into
28:32
that good night. But everyone listening should
28:34
know. You are taxpayers, you are paying
28:36
for scientific research to be done, and
28:39
then you can't even read about it
28:41
without paying. Why? Why can't you read
28:43
about the research that you funded? Answer?
28:46
It's owned by Scientific Publishers. I just
28:48
think the world would be better if
28:50
everybody had access to the information
28:52
that science offers, but it's being held
28:55
hostage by people who are making money.
28:57
I'm in full agreement with you on
28:59
this Dan, and the solution that publishers
29:02
are trying right now is offensive. I
29:04
think the most recent article I had
29:06
accepted. There's a little box I could
29:09
check right before we finalized the publication
29:11
saying, if you want this to be
29:13
open access, please pay $3,500. Well,
29:15
you got away for 3,500, but I
29:18
published a two-page article in Nature and
29:20
it was $11,000. $11,000 to make it
29:22
free. But there's a very easy way
29:25
to get around it. We just publish
29:27
without them. And that's what we're doing
29:29
now with the Handbook of Social Psychology.
29:32
It's just a demonstration project that it
29:34
can be done. By the way, we're
29:36
talking about scientific publishing. Everything I'm
29:38
saying is not true of Barbara King
29:41
Solver and her publisher, or the publishers
29:43
who... publish your books, Adam, they are
29:45
doing it. an important service for you
29:48
and they deserve to be paid. They're
29:50
finding audiences, they're sending pay, but scientists
29:52
don't need any of that. You've had
29:55
a lot of time to reflect on
29:57
stumbling on happiness. Is there something you've
29:59
changed your mind about since you
30:01
published it? Thankfully, I don't know of
30:04
anything in it that requires revision, but
30:06
many things could certainly use addition. What
30:08
would be your biggest expansion? Well, I
30:11
think we knew very little about... How
30:13
to solve the problems that Stumbling Unhappiness
30:15
identifies. Stumbling Unhappiness basically is an indictment
30:18
of your imagination. It says, you're going
30:20
to try to close your eyes and
30:22
think about the future, and you're
30:24
going to be prone to a bunch
30:27
of errors. I'll spend my whole book
30:29
telling you what they are and why
30:31
they matter. And the very end, it
30:34
says, is there anything we can do
30:36
about it? And I think we now
30:38
know the answer, and it's a paradoxical
30:41
answer. The answer is yes. There's actually
30:43
a pretty good way to make predictions
30:45
about what will make you happy,
30:47
and the paradox is, people don't like
30:50
this method at all. It's a method
30:52
that we call surrogation, and it just
30:54
means using other people as surrogates. If
30:57
I want to know how happy I'm
30:59
going to be if I go to
31:01
law school, what I really ought to
31:04
do the best data I can get
31:06
is to find out how happy people
31:08
who went to law school are.
31:10
I mean, a lot of them would
31:13
be good, but even some of them
31:15
would probably be better than my own
31:17
imagination. But we've shown in experiments that
31:20
when you give people this opportunity, if
31:22
they just shake their heads and go,
31:24
what do you mean? Those people aren't
31:27
me. Knowing people are remarkably similar in
31:29
what makes them happy and unhappy. Nobody
31:31
says I'd rather be hit by
31:33
a two-by-four than have a weekend in
31:36
Paris. Nobody would rather eat cardboard than
31:38
chocolate. We're very similar in our hadonic
31:40
reactions to things. So other people's reactions
31:43
to events you're only imagining are a
31:45
very good guide to your own reaction.
31:47
I'm going to have to fight you
31:50
a little bit on this stand because
31:52
it wanders into the realm of organ.
31:54
psychological psychology and I'm thinking about
31:56
now over half a century of evidence
31:59
that vocational interests differ significantly between people
32:01
and that the kind of work that
32:03
would make me happy might make you
32:06
miserable. Particularly if I as an introverted,
32:08
agreeable, highly conscientious person happen to love
32:10
a little bit of teaching and a
32:13
lot of thinking and writing and you
32:15
have the opposite traits and that job
32:17
sounds like a death sentence for
32:19
you. So at minimum. Shouldn't you go
32:22
out and find people with similar interests
32:24
and taste to yours? Well, that would
32:26
be preferable. But my contention is, even
32:29
a randomly selected other person will be
32:31
better than your own imagination. Remember, I
32:33
never said that other people's experiences are
32:36
a perfect guide to your own. I
32:38
just said they're better than the alternative.
32:40
Right? Winston Churchill's wonderful line that
32:42
democracy is the worst form of government
32:45
except for the others. Well, surrogation is
32:47
a terrible way to predict your future,
32:49
except for the other way, which is
32:52
to use your own imagination. So we
32:54
showed in a series of experiments, for
32:56
example, that people could improve the accuracy
32:59
of their affective forecasts by listening to
33:01
one randomly selected stranger. On average. Imagine
33:03
how much you could improve if
33:05
they weren't randomly selected. They were selected
33:08
to be like you in many important
33:10
ways. And if it was more than
33:12
one stranger, it was 10 or 12
33:15
people. I think that also suggests that
33:17
the questions you ask really matter. So
33:19
I might not go to that stranger
33:22
and ask how happy you are you
33:24
in this work. I might be more
33:26
inclined to ask, tell me the
33:29
highlights and the low lights, and then
33:31
see how those map on to what
33:33
I'm looking for. Well, yes, you really
33:36
identified one of the rubs here, which
33:38
is, okay, I'm convinced by this Gilbert
33:40
and Wilson paper that says I should
33:43
use other people's past experience as a
33:45
guide to my future experience. How can
33:47
I find out about their past experience?
33:50
And unfortunately, you often... have to
33:52
ask them. And really, unfortunately, they often
33:54
can't tell you accurately. So how about
33:56
parenthood? You're thinking, I'm trying to decide
33:59
if I should have children. I don't
34:01
know if they would make me happy.
34:03
And so you go to some parents
34:06
and you go to children, make you
34:08
happy, and parents will say, oh, of
34:10
course they do. They're the best thing
34:13
about life. They make me happier
34:15
than anything else. We know, from tons
34:17
of data, that those parents are wrong.
34:19
that children in fact have a small
34:22
but negative impact on the happiness of
34:24
their parents. But their parents don't know
34:26
it. So simply asking them that question,
34:29
do your children make you happy is
34:31
the wrong way to find out if
34:33
their children make them happy. What's the
34:36
right way? The way that economists
34:38
and psychologists do it, which is just
34:40
to go up to people with children
34:42
and go, hi, how happy are you
34:45
right now? If you go up to
34:47
a thousand people with and without children,
34:49
I assure you, the average response you
34:52
get from the non-parents will be either
34:54
equal to or slightly higher than the
34:56
average response you get from the parents.
34:59
And those data tell you something
35:01
about parenthood and happiness. There may be
35:03
many reasons that have children, but making
35:05
yourself more happy in the moment is
35:08
not one of them. Yeah, which I
35:10
think also tracks with the evidence on
35:12
spikes in happiness when people become empty
35:15
nesters. Say more. Well, I'm just thinking
35:17
about if kids make you happy, then
35:19
kids leaving your house permanently should be
35:22
a source of misery. And I
35:24
feel sad when I think about our
35:26
kids leaving. In fact, I've already started
35:28
to tell our oldest teenager that I'm
35:31
disappointed in her preemptively for choosing to
35:33
leave. But I also know from the
35:35
research that when the last child leaves
35:38
the house, on average parents get happier,
35:40
don't they? Yeah, the only symptom of
35:42
emptiness syndrome is nonstop. smiling. I mean,
35:45
as far as we can tell, you
35:47
still will have those children. You will
35:49
still be their dad and they
35:51
will continue to give you joy and
35:54
maybe even a greater ratio of joy
35:56
to worry once they're living their lives
35:58
as successful adults. Then they do when
36:01
they're kids who are whining, you know,
36:03
are we there yet? How come she
36:05
got more? So it's not exactly a
36:08
fair comparison, but you're exactly right. What's
36:10
the question you have for me? Do
36:12
you ever worry that with all
36:14
of your outwardward facing? activities, all the
36:17
public exposure from podcast, television, etc. that
36:19
you could be in danger of losing
36:21
some part of yourself that I assume
36:24
for you is very valuable and very
36:26
important, and it's that part of you
36:28
that is a scientist. that sits and
36:31
carefully goes through data and does work
36:33
and discovers new facts. I asked this
36:35
only because I've had some experience
36:37
doing outward facing things. And as I
36:40
did those things, I often had to
36:42
remind myself how to remain me and
36:44
not become a chameleon that was responding
36:47
to applause. It's very hard. It's a
36:49
very addictive thing. It's an extremely important
36:51
question. And... I think I worried about
36:54
it a lot in the first few
36:56
years of this public-facing role. And now
36:58
I reflect on it a lot,
37:00
but it doesn't come with anxiety like
37:03
it used to. It felt like I
37:05
was going through a portal, and I
37:07
didn't know what was on the other
37:10
side. Am I still going to be
37:12
able to get back if I want,
37:14
was the thing that I worried about.
37:17
I guess I've been doing this for
37:19
a dozen years now. What I've discovered
37:21
is what gives me the most
37:23
joy is teaching and teaching and teaching
37:26
of that. So without the time in
37:28
the classroom, without the deep immersion and
37:30
data, I wouldn't have anything to say.
37:33
And so I'm constantly going back to
37:35
that well, both to serve the external
37:37
facing work that I do, but more
37:40
importantly, to feed what energizes me. And
37:42
I've repeated that pattern enough that it's
37:44
clearly my revealed preference. It makes
37:46
my day when someone tells me to
37:49
stop stringing citations in my random sentences
37:51
in a keynote or in an interview
37:53
or even in a podcast for that
37:56
matter because it's just so my impulse.
37:58
It's the way I think it's the
38:00
way I love to learn. And so
38:03
I think I've grown comfortable with the
38:05
fact that it's just it's too wired
38:07
into me to ever lose sight
38:09
of it. We both know as scientists
38:12
and researchers and university professors. that the
38:14
kind of outward-facing things we've done raise
38:16
eyebrows among many colleagues. It's just their
38:19
impression was, once you're on television and
38:21
you're on camera and you're doing these
38:23
sorts of things, you can't possibly be
38:26
a serious scientist anymore. You're saying that's
38:28
not true, and I know you're right.
38:30
Do you worry about the fact
38:32
that in the academy, the perception is
38:35
often you can't do both of these
38:37
things? A lot of our colleagues who
38:39
do brilliant research are now... entering the
38:42
public domain. And I've watched a lot
38:44
of people who 10 years ago might
38:46
have looked down their nose on us
38:49
as sellouts, writing books and giving their
38:51
work and sharing it. And so I
38:53
think the norms in our field
38:55
have changed a little bit. It's really
38:58
a generational shift. People my age still
39:00
arch in eyebrow. It's like, you did
39:02
a PBS show, I don't really know.
39:05
But people your age just say, oh,
39:07
well, they say, what's PBS. But you
39:09
did a TV show marvelous. Can I
39:12
do one too? That would be fun.
39:14
And I do, you know, I think
39:16
your answer is right. I see
39:18
all of these as an extension of
39:21
teaching. I always love teaching. And in
39:23
2004, this little conference called TED said,
39:25
would you like to come give a
39:28
talk? We're thinking maybe we'll videotape them
39:30
and put them on the internet. to
39:32
which I think I responded, how do
39:35
you put videos on the internet? And
39:37
I said, oh, we think it's coming.
39:39
So what I learned when that
39:41
went on the internet and lots and
39:44
lots of people watched it was that
39:46
the world is my classroom and I
39:48
could give psychology away to a much
39:51
much bigger audience. So that's how I
39:53
seal. all of these efforts. And probably
39:56
like you, I turn down lots of
39:58
opportunities to do things, might be kind
40:00
of fun and sexy, but don't feel
40:03
like they're true to the mission
40:05
of giving science away to the world.
40:07
Dan, before I let you go, I
40:09
have to ask, you've done research on
40:12
ending conversations. Yes. How do I wrap
40:14
this thing up? What do most of
40:16
us do wrong in light of your
40:19
data? Well, most conversations don't have a
40:21
natural termination point and the people in
40:23
them usually like each other and they
40:26
don't want to offend the other
40:28
person by saying, I'm now tired of
40:30
talking with you. And so in our
40:32
research, what we found is conversations actually
40:35
go on. Oftentimes, much longer than either
40:37
of the people in them wants them
40:39
to. And when I say that, everybody
40:42
shakes their head and they remember those
40:44
conversations. But what we also found is
40:46
because we can't be honest with each
40:49
other about when we want to
40:51
end, many conversations end long before either
40:53
person wants them to. They go on
40:55
too short. Saving grace doesn't seem to
40:58
matter that much to people's happiness. Conversations
41:00
go on too long. They end a
41:02
little too early. What we find is
41:05
everybody leaves them pretty much happier than
41:07
they were before. Well, then I'm going
41:09
to resist the temptation to ask you
41:12
how we can get in sync
41:14
on that. Not necessary. Better that we
41:16
should all go away with the illusion
41:18
that our conversation lasted exactly the right
41:21
amount of time, if not for us,
41:23
than for the other person we were
41:25
talking to. Well, I think on that
41:28
note, it's been too short for me,
41:30
but I'll assume that it was the
41:32
right length for you. Too short for
41:35
me as well, let's do it.
41:37
Let's do it again. My
41:40
main takeaway from Dan is that
41:43
the impact of negative events rarely
41:45
lasts as long as we expect.
41:47
The changes in our lives matter,
41:50
but how we adapt matters more.
41:52
Rethinking is hosted by me, Adam
41:54
Grant. The show is part of
41:57
the TED Audio Collective, and this
41:59
episode was produced in mixed... by
42:01
Cosmic Standard. Our producers are Hannah
42:04
Kingsley Ma and Asia Simpson. Our
42:06
editor is Alejandro Salazar. Our fact
42:08
checkers Paul Durbin, original music by
42:11
Hansdale Sue and Allison Layton Brown.
42:13
Our team includes Eliza Smith, Jacob
42:15
Winnick, Samiah Adams, Roxanne High Lash,
42:18
Banban Chang, Julia Dickerson, and Whitney
42:20
Pennington Rogers. Be
44:27
Fearless in the Sun with
44:29
Blue Lizard Australian sunscreen. Whether you're
44:31
diving into ocean waves, you're simply
44:33
chasing your kids around the backyard.
44:36
Blue Lizard has you covered so
44:38
you can enjoy every moment or
44:40
free. Design for your whole family
44:43
and free from fragrances, parabins, and
44:45
ballades. Blue Lizard is your go-to
44:47
mineral sunscreen trusted by dermatologists, pediatricians,
44:50
and parents. Shop now on amazon.com
44:52
and Be Fearless in the Sun
44:54
with Blue Lizard Australian sunscreen.
44:56
sunscreen.
Podchaser is the ultimate destination for podcast data, search, and discovery. Learn More