Episode Transcript
Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.
Use Ctrl + F to search
0:01
As a parent, you want what's best for your
0:03
teen. for your teen. You want You want
0:05
them to grow and thrive in
0:08
this world. but you But you also want
0:10
to make sure they're staying safe. safe.
0:12
That's why That's why Instagram is introducing
0:14
teen accounts With with automatic protections for
0:16
who can contact teens and the content
0:18
content they can see. Instagram accounts,
0:20
built -in limits for teens, and
0:22
peace of mind for parents. for parents. Learn
0:25
Learn more at instagram.com.
0:27
teen accounts. After
0:31
the end of a good
0:33
fight, you deserve a nice cold
0:35
reward. Medella, you put in the hours,
0:37
the the energy, tough the tough
0:39
labor, you know, you know the bigger
0:41
the fight, the better the reward.
0:43
the Madeleine, Quick response to the beer reported by
0:45
Crown in Portugal, Illinois. Oh,
0:47
hey, it's it's still at the at the mall
0:50
that's never on. you are here you are
0:52
here for part two of Please tell me
0:54
that you started Please tell me that you
0:56
started with part one. Even if
0:58
you don't remember it, start with part
1:00
one because this is the thrilling
1:02
conclusion. and the director of UC researcher, and the
1:04
director of UC Irvine Center for the
1:06
Neurobiology of Learning We're Memory, he is
1:08
amazing. This We're gonna get into it,
1:10
but first, this is a wall -to
1:12
-wall episode of Questions from patrons. and if if
1:14
you'd like to submit some time, time you
1:16
two join join as low as a buck a
1:18
a month upper tears can can submit audio
1:20
questions. Thank you to everyone getting
1:22
Ologies oligismurch.com and thanks to everyone
1:24
leaving us leaving us boost the show
1:26
so much and each week I remember
1:28
to read them all and I
1:30
pick I just written one one. Such is
1:32
this one from Sule Singh who wrote, wrote
1:34
this podcast saved my life. It's
1:36
like spending an hour at the
1:38
most amazing library ever where you
1:40
find exactly the right book that
1:42
you didn't know that you didn't know
1:44
that you needed. Sule Singh left a review
1:46
I have read it left a review, I
1:49
to the it. And thank you. on
1:51
to part to the we answer
1:53
your questions about two, where to
1:55
remember names and faces, what
1:57
causes Alzheimer's, photographic memory, short
1:59
term memory. -term memory, how how
2:01
to prevent dementia, cannabis
2:03
in memory, goldfish brains, and why
2:06
smelling sunscreen makes you
2:08
wanna cry sometimes. cry
2:10
With with professor, researcher and
2:12
memory expert, expert, nimanologist, Dr. Michael
2:14
Yassa. Oh,
2:33
Oh, I have have questions from
2:35
listeners. Can I ask you
2:37
one million? I Of course. one million?
2:39
Of the best. the best. Okay. So, this
2:41
question was on the minds
2:44
of minds of patrons, Isopardi, who's a
2:46
first -time question question-asker, Anne, face-name, forgetter, Diana, to
2:48
Resinick, Dean, Lily, and audio questions, wanted to
2:50
know. to know. Hi, Ellie. Summer from
2:52
New Zealand here. here. What
2:55
I find really interesting is
2:57
that I remember faces. But
2:59
sometimes I can't remember
3:01
where I've seen that person
3:03
before, that even in what
3:05
country I've seen them.
3:07
And I'm wondering why we're
3:09
so good good at faces
3:11
and remembering remembering face, but
3:13
knowing none of the details
3:15
around that face. face. What
3:17
is it about remembering certain
3:20
aspects of a person
3:22
or a connection? I know
3:24
some people have total
3:26
face blindness as well. Either
3:28
evolutionarily how do we do we
3:30
look at faces where we and
3:32
say, who is person? Yeah,
3:34
that's a fantastic question, Summer.
3:36
summer. So you've already mentioned one version
3:38
of this, which is blindness, right?
3:40
And I'm not I'm not saying
3:43
that Summer has face blindness
3:45
at all. In fact, I
3:47
think all of us us with
3:49
faces to an extent. extent.
3:51
condition is medically known as as
3:53
prosopagnosia a 2023 study in
3:55
the cortex titled, what is the What is
3:57
the prevalence of developmental prosopagnosia
3:59
an impure? of different diagnostic
4:01
cutoffs found that developmental Prasopignasha, meaning its
4:04
lifelong and not caused by injury to
4:06
the brain, is more common than previously
4:08
thought and that it lies on a
4:10
spectrum of severity. I was doing some
4:13
reading on it from people who have
4:15
it to try to figure out what
4:17
it feels like, and I've read it
4:20
described as seeing a tree and then
4:22
trying to pick out that specific tree
4:24
in a forest, or telling the difference
4:27
between two different cows in a field.
4:29
And if this sounds like you, and
4:31
if you watch movies wondering, wait, is
4:34
that the same guy, or is that
4:36
a different guy, you may be one
4:38
in 33 people who have it, which
4:40
is cool, but also awful. So don't
4:43
let anyone make you feel too bad
4:45
about it. Now faces are a very
4:47
important piece of information. When you look
4:50
at somebody's face, you can tell by
4:52
their expression whether they're a threat to
4:54
you, whether they're a friendly person, you
4:57
can choose to approach or avoid. You
4:59
can base a lot of your decision-making
5:01
based on a face. If somebody's face
5:04
has an expression of fear and they're
5:06
looking behind you, they might warn you
5:08
to something and you might react accordingly.
5:10
So we evolved, and not just our
5:13
species, many social species have evolved, to
5:15
try to always interpret faceses and facial
5:17
expressions. But recognizing a face and remembering
5:20
whether a face belongs to a certain
5:22
name is this thing that happened much
5:24
later in evolution. And I don't think
5:27
our brains are just very good at
5:29
it yet. I think we're developing that,
5:31
but we're still sort of half-baked when
5:33
it comes to connecting names and faces.
5:36
So number one complaints that I hear
5:38
from everybody is I remember faces, but
5:40
I could never connect when I see
5:43
it. So face names, associations like that,
5:45
they tend to be the most challenging
5:47
for us, humans. But one of the
5:50
reasons why faces are really important is
5:52
because of this evolutionary survival sort of
5:54
significance, things around fear, things around pleasure,
5:57
reward, all of that. We get our
5:59
cues. our cues from faces. I learned
6:01
a trick once on a film set
6:03
where one guy worked with, it was
6:06
Adam Savage from MythBusters. He knew everyone's
6:08
name on this huge set we were
6:10
on. It was like,
6:12
okay, half this trick where that woman's
6:15
name is Dorothy, and I think of
6:17
Dorothy from the Wizard of Oz. That
6:19
person's name is Ben, and I have
6:21
a brother-in-law named Ben. And so as
6:24
soon as they said their name, you
6:26
had to pay attention, but then you
6:28
made a connection. Absolutely. You know, there's
6:30
lots of folks out there who try
6:33
to train you on how to make
6:35
your memory system better. And many of
6:37
them, you might know, they're memory champions
6:39
out there. They're sort of memory grandmasters
6:42
out there. And they've learned tricks that
6:44
have been used by the Greeks for
6:46
a long time. Ancient Greeks, I should
6:48
say, like mind palaces and so on.
6:51
And all of these methods are based
6:53
on forming strong personal associations. So if
6:55
you have never seen The Wizard of
6:57
Oz, it wouldn't have made sense to
7:00
you to associate it, right? But if
7:02
you've seen it and you can picture
7:04
the Dorothy in the exact movie, right,
7:06
that makes it much more personal. So
7:09
they tell you to try to be
7:11
able to remember people's faces and names,
7:13
make that something that is emotionally significant
7:15
to you, but making those connections and
7:18
making them as vivid as possible, seems
7:20
to be the trick that works for
7:22
most people. So I hope that helps
7:24
Clay Trover, Sarah McEichurn, Eric Gizeg, Jennifer
7:27
Frow, Elise Waiple, Hannah Nolan, Jacqueline Church,
7:29
Noamselorm, or Bennett Vanderbosch, Ellen, Liss, Zuleika
7:31
Pevic, Califong, Kelly, Kelly, Kelly, Kelly Larson,
7:33
Carleen D.H, and Connor, they them, all
7:36
of whom say that they are garbage
7:38
at remembering names. It's okay, obviously, based
7:40
on that list you are not alone.
7:42
And patron Jackie G said that a
7:45
tour guide shared this tip with them,
7:47
that you introduce yourself first, then you
7:49
can actually listen when other people say
7:51
their names instead of mentally rehearsing your
7:54
own introduction. And Jackie G says it's
7:56
amazing and has worked wonders for me.
7:58
So perhaps that'll make some of you
8:00
less anxious. Oh, speaking of anxiety and
8:03
trauma and emotions in general, many, many
8:05
of you. Miss Particular Camilla Gimino, Susan
8:07
Singley, Maddie Cakes, Popsicle Emperor, Joshua Towson,
8:09
Sarah Arguetta, Rowan Tree, Michelle, Gregos, Ali
8:12
Brown, Patricia Evans, Katie Hammond, and Rachel
8:14
Brasteko wanted to know how emotions affect
8:16
retention. And Nick Alston asked, what's the
8:18
chemistry behind emotional memories? And also, patron
8:21
Ryan asked. Hi, this is Ryan from
8:23
Los Angeles. I was wondering if you
8:25
could speak to the effects that negative
8:27
emotions, such as shame and guilt, have
8:30
on working memory and long-term memory. Are
8:32
people who experience heightened emotions, either with
8:34
anxiety or pretty happy people, do they
8:36
keep those memories more because there is
8:39
more of an emotional connection? There's no
8:41
doubt that having an emotional connection strengthens
8:43
the way that you store a memory.
8:45
So certainly if you tend to have
8:48
much more emotional reactions to things or
8:50
the experience that you're having are much
8:52
more emotional, they will have the capacity
8:54
to be stored for longer, to be
8:57
able to influence your actions and decisions
8:59
for longer. And again, there's the evolutionary
9:01
significance for that, of course, more emotional
9:03
things may be a little bit more
9:06
involved in your survival, right? That said,
9:08
emotion doesn't always improve your ability to
9:10
store things. It colors the experience for
9:12
sure, but it also kind of zooms
9:15
in on certain aspects of the experience
9:17
and zooms out from others. So you
9:19
may recall certain details incredibly well, but
9:21
there may be other things that are
9:24
kind of lost on you because of
9:26
the emotionality. So it creates this competition
9:28
between some central features of the experience
9:30
and then a lot of the peripheral
9:32
stuff kind of doesn't win out in
9:35
that competition and that can be forgotten.
9:37
Many of you wondered about what is
9:39
normal given our very chaotic and our
9:41
very chaotic and technological world such as
9:44
ollogist from the caribology episode Megan Lynch,
9:46
Amelia Frank, Ginny Bateman, Katie Britt Klein,
9:48
Margarda Nuska, Miranda Pan irregular K, Wasp's
9:50
ollogist, Errikar, Tiny Nature, and first-time question-askers
9:53
Julie Willy Julie Willy Willy. Loves Chocolate,
9:55
Theokline, Tara Villanova, and some folks who
9:57
are distracted by stress, Don Ewald, Holly
9:59
Cole, Eric Masterson, and Helen Langiel. I
10:02
wonder if our lack of presence, because
10:04
we were distracted a lot with our
10:06
phones and internet stuff, I wonder if
10:08
that lack of presence is making our
10:11
memories a little bit more Swiss cheesy.
10:13
I like this Swiss cheesy. It's a
10:15
good way to think about it. Certainly
10:17
every time that I've lifted up my
10:20
phone and tried to record a video
10:22
of my daughter playing basketball as opposed
10:24
to put the phone down and actually
10:26
watch her play, I feel like I
10:29
have Swiss cheese in my brain. Yeah.
10:31
So 100% I think that you're right
10:33
when we replace actually experiencing something in
10:35
its full glory. three-dimensionality and all of
10:38
that with some two-d version of what
10:40
we're recording on the phone or being
10:42
distracted by looking at something at the
10:44
same time and not being fully aware
10:47
fully cognizant of what's happening of course
10:49
it's going to change how these memories
10:51
are stored and how they're represented. And
10:53
I do worry about that. I think
10:56
that there's no doubt that there's value
10:58
to having the electronics and having, I
11:00
mean, look at us, we have, our
11:02
devices are out all the time. And
11:05
that's just the nature of what we
11:07
have to do to be able to
11:09
deal with the situations around us and
11:11
the rapidly kind of evolving world and
11:14
all of the stimulation. But I think
11:16
it misses something about experiencing something. fully
11:18
and truly with all of its three
11:20
dimension, four dimensionality I should say. And
11:23
with all the emotional contexts that come
11:25
with it, right? And to be able
11:27
to have that genuinely, you have to
11:29
be there. So immersion, I think, is
11:32
really key for good memory storage. So
11:34
those four dimensions are like a 3D
11:36
object on the X, Y, and Z
11:38
axis, but with the added dimension of
11:41
time. It's interesting because we're experiencing it
11:43
less in the moment, but we're able
11:45
to recall it more with that video
11:47
footage, and it's such a weird tradeoff
11:50
that it's like, well, I can remember
11:52
it later, as long as I don't
11:54
fully explain. is a weird tradeoff because
11:56
what you remember based on the video
11:59
is this weird sort of two-dimensional version
12:01
on a screen and you're never able
12:03
to piece back being there fully in
12:05
three dimensions. But if that was your
12:08
experience, that's what you piece back. So
12:10
it comes back with all of the
12:12
pleasure, the sensory experience that came with
12:14
it. as opposed to if you're looking
12:17
at the screen, that's your version of
12:19
that reality, and that's what comes back.
12:21
It's just that two-dimensional, more impoverished version.
12:23
Yeah. And that, to me, I think,
12:26
is the big concern. That's funny because
12:28
I, my husband and I got married
12:30
three years ago, and a good friend
12:32
of mine was like, let's watch the
12:35
video on your first anniversary. My husband
12:37
and I can't watch the video. What
12:39
we remember from it is what we
12:41
remember from it is, what we remember
12:44
from it. Because we are kind of
12:46
always, like you said, reconstructing those memories
12:48
based on input, right? And you don't
12:50
want to change the version that you
12:53
have in your brain. Yeah. Especially if
12:55
that version was beautiful and something that
12:57
you want to hold on to. Now
12:59
I have the, anytime anybody tells me
13:01
to watch a video of myself, so
13:04
I just can't do it categorically, that's
13:06
just something I don't want to do.
13:08
But I can totally resonate with what
13:10
you're saying if you've experienced it fully
13:13
being there. being able to piece it
13:15
back together in this two-dimensional version and
13:17
looking at it on a video is
13:19
just never the same. And it kind
13:22
of alters your, you know, actual recollection
13:24
of that experience. And a University of
13:26
Chicago study recently recruited users of this
13:28
app called One Second Every Day to
13:31
hop at an MRI and look at
13:33
quick videos of strangers' lives versus one-second
13:35
clips that they had recorded themselves using
13:37
that app. And the study found that
13:40
different parts of the brain light up
13:42
if it was their own memory versus
13:44
just in taking a stranger's video. And
13:46
other neurobiologists at the University of Toronto
13:49
are exploring how video diaries like that
13:51
could help Alzheimer's patients connect more to
13:53
their own past. So to patrons Antifa
13:55
and Rosalie Delaforee who recall more. when
13:58
prompted by photos and videos, that's some
14:00
real science right there for you. Everyone
14:02
else, that app was called one second
14:04
every day if you want to log
14:07
little chunks of your whole life. Now,
14:09
what if you don't need physical memorabilia
14:11
or pixels to jog your memory? What
14:13
if your mind is a camera? So,
14:16
Alia Myers and Erica Perryandri, among others,
14:18
had questions about that. Well, on the
14:20
topic of photographic memory, many people asked,
14:22
Mariah Kay said, this question has been
14:25
on my mind for so long, all
14:27
caps. I know there's photographic memory, but
14:29
are there different versions or levels of
14:31
it? Read Barry wants to know what
14:34
actually is photographic memory. This question was
14:36
asked by Cybermans, Erica, Mary Andrie, Rachel
14:38
McGill, Earl of Gramelkin. What is photographic
14:40
memory exactly? Okay, fellas. This is myth
14:43
bust in time. Okay. Ready? Yeah. Photographic
14:45
memory does not exist. How? How? And
14:47
it's so uncomfortable to hear that. Well,
14:49
so photographic memory is, or what we
14:52
sometimes refer to as idetic memory, you
14:54
know, the way that it's defined, it's
14:56
that you have this perfect recall, perfect
14:58
recollection of something that you've seen, potentially
15:01
only once, right? And if it's in
15:03
the context of, say, reading, you're talking
15:05
about like, you're remembering page numbers and
15:07
all of that. The evidence that that
15:10
exists historically is slim to none. We've
15:12
been lied to. There's maybe a very
15:14
small handful of cases of Savant syndrome
15:16
where somebody could legitimately make the claim
15:19
for a true photographic memory. But aside
15:21
from that, there's no real evidence that
15:23
photographic memory to this sense exists. That
15:25
said, there's really, really, really good memory,
15:28
and this really, really, really bad memory.
15:30
There's a whole spectrum. And a lot
15:32
of times, when we're thinking about photographic
15:34
memory, it's not exactly that, but it's
15:37
close. It's really good memory where you're
15:39
memorizing where things are laid out on
15:41
the page, where you may be memorizing
15:43
things like, you know, the page numbers,
15:46
and where the figures were, where the
15:48
pictures were, all that kind of. To
15:50
the extent that we can tell whether
15:52
or not, you know, that's actually helpful
15:55
in a day-to-day learning experience, it's not
15:57
clear whether or not that actually helps
15:59
you. And while a true photographic memory
16:01
is at this point just flim-flam. There
16:04
is such a thing as idetic memory.
16:06
where someone can see a visual and
16:08
look at it or sometimes hear something.
16:10
And once it's removed, they can recall
16:13
it in great detail. It's as if
16:15
visually they're still looking at it, but
16:17
it's not 100% faithful and it does
16:19
not last a lifetime. Your brain's like,
16:22
I don't really need that fancy of
16:24
a feature. Because remember, what we're trying
16:26
to learn and remember on a day-to-day
16:28
basis is not necessarily the details of
16:30
the exact words on a page or
16:33
the exact details of what happened. It's
16:35
the gist. It's sort of the overall
16:37
experience kind of abstracted. It's whatever knowledge
16:39
it can abstract from that experience and
16:42
be able to use it to guide
16:44
my future decision making. So if you
16:46
start to think about memory a little
16:48
bit differently, that it's not really about
16:51
the past, it's all about the future,
16:53
this is no longer uncomfortable. It's okay
16:55
to sit with that, that there's no
16:57
photographic memory, because there's no rationale, there's
17:00
no reason for it to exist. So
17:02
though we debunked learning styles in Part
17:04
1, what about people who don't retain
17:06
things as well visually but through sound?
17:09
Hey, first time question askers Kimberly, Kirsten
17:11
Cornell, as well as E. Jordan, Sean
17:13
Thomas, Kay, Matthew Walter, Lisa Gorman, Maria
17:15
Kay, Josh Walden, S. Vanessa Adams, Daniel,
17:18
Ben Voren, Jennifer Frow, Debda Science, Alicia
17:20
Clarkson, and Sam, wanted to know about
17:22
sound and memory, including song lyrics. Well,
17:24
I know because we have obviously a
17:27
lot of listeners who learn by auditory
17:29
memory, are some people better at recalling
17:31
things if they hear it? Yes. Okay.
17:33
Yes. So, and this takes us back
17:36
to the conversation about learning styles. So
17:38
I want to be very clear, right?
17:40
It is a myth. Learning styles is
17:42
a myth. There is no one way
17:45
to be able to get to your
17:47
brain and it could be visual for
17:49
you or auditory for you. That said,
17:51
people do have individual differences in how
17:54
much they learn visually and how much
17:56
they learn through their ears auditorily and
17:58
how much they learn through other senses.
18:00
But at the end of the day,
18:03
the most effective learning is the one
18:05
that combines the most senses. So as
18:07
a species, we are far more visual.
18:09
As a species, collectively. If you look
18:12
at rodents, for example, rats, they have
18:14
a much stronger sense of smell than
18:16
we ever would be motivated by, right?
18:18
That does not diminish our sense of
18:21
smell, does not diminish our other senses,
18:23
but it says that primarily because we
18:25
are not nocturnal, we operate usually in
18:27
daylight and the sunlight, visual information seems
18:30
to be really important to us. We
18:32
tend to... kind of prioritize that. We
18:34
have a lot more real estate in
18:36
our brain dedicated to processing of the
18:39
visual sense than to other senses. So
18:41
when somebody says, well I learn better
18:43
if I hear it, I'll say I'm
18:45
willing to bet that you'd learn better
18:48
if you heard it and saw it
18:50
at the same time. So don't just
18:52
rely on, oh I just want to
18:54
hear it. So for example if you're
18:57
reading. and you say I'm just not
18:59
a reader, well try reading out loud
19:01
so you're also hearing it and listening
19:03
to yourself say the words and you'll
19:06
notice that that is a bimodal kind
19:08
of learning and things will stick a
19:10
lot more. We did a
19:12
reading episode recently and there was a
19:14
big question as to whether or not
19:16
audio books were reading, whether they count,
19:18
whether you can count them in your
19:20
book. Oh, I sure hope they count.
19:22
Yeah, I know. Me too. So we
19:24
talked about this in the recent anagnosology
19:26
episode all about reading, and I'll just
19:28
give you an excerpt from that with
19:30
Dr. Adery and Johns, who is a
19:33
professor and historian and who authored the
19:35
book, The Science of Reading. It's actually
19:37
interesting that e-books, or actually audio books
19:39
more, the idea that you could as
19:41
it were read a novel or something
19:43
by having it read to you by
19:45
a machine, there are schemes for those
19:47
going back as far as the, pretty
19:49
much the origin of recording. So the
19:51
late 19th, early 20th century, there were
19:53
visionary schemes for having things like, oh,
19:55
you know, vending machines where you could
19:57
put your money in and there would
19:59
be a speaker, you know, a speaking
20:01
trumpet that would speak a book. It's
20:03
not like there's something that is that
20:05
radically new about audio books per se.
20:07
Having said that, I mean, my own
20:09
sense of it, kind of crudely, is
20:11
that I think with audio books, it's
20:13
really that you're having something read to
20:15
you rather than reading. And part of
20:17
that has to do with the control
20:20
of the pace of it. So you
20:22
can slow down recordings, you can pause
20:24
it and all of that kind of
20:26
thing. But it's
20:28
not the same as doing what one
20:30
does with mine's eye all the time
20:32
in reading a page where you're constantly
20:34
shifting the speed and considering things and
20:36
going back, you know, without unnecessarily thinking
20:38
about it. You don't have to press
20:41
a button or something. E-books, on the
20:43
other hand, I think are just reading.
20:45
I mean, I don't have any issue
20:47
with those at all. So Dr. Johns
20:49
is a reading scholar and I am
20:51
a lady recording this while not wearing
20:53
a bra, but whose entire life revolves
20:56
around reading to people. So we'll have
20:58
to arm Russell for dominance. But I
21:00
will say that her an ancient study
21:02
from the late 1960s titled Retention and
21:04
Recall, incidental learning of visual and auditory
21:06
material in the Journal of Genetic Psychology,
21:08
that visual memories tend to be better
21:11
for recall, but there is quote, a
21:13
decline with age for recall of visual
21:15
material. but virtually no deterioration in performance
21:17
on the auditory task. So I'm going
21:19
to amplify that data in my favor.
21:21
Now what about smell? Cody Burdock, Vanessa
21:23
Adams, Christine Hurley, the dark next door,
21:26
Amanda Regan, Guy Hutchinson eating dark hair
21:28
for a living, Renee Vandenhoven, Just H.
21:30
Fiona, Elizabeth, Carol Young, and Estasia Press.
21:32
All wanted to know about smell and
21:34
memory, and Susan Singley asked, why do
21:36
some aromas bring back such clear and
21:38
nostalgic memories, like cut grass, old books,
21:40
coconut sunscreen, ocean waves, and that smell
21:43
after rain? But yeah, earlier Mike mentioned
21:45
that rodents have a much stronger sense
21:47
of smell than us, and I wonder
21:49
if they feel nostalgic for smelling certain
21:51
garbage. I bet they do. Well, you
21:53
mentioned smell and rats. have an excellent
21:55
urban rhodontology episode about rats that made
21:58
me cry with affection for rats. But
22:00
Amanda Regan wanted to know why do
22:02
smells or sounds trigger memories sometimes? And
22:04
I have heard that it's difficult to
22:06
really know what a smell is. You
22:08
have to have a memory of that
22:10
smell that it goes straight to some
22:13
memory center. What's up with that? couple
22:15
things so let me break this down
22:17
because you asked a couple of really
22:19
really interesting questions but they're a little
22:21
bit different from each other so the
22:23
first one is maybe whether or not
22:25
we can sort of label smells you
22:28
can label a sound and people that
22:30
have perfect pitch can tell you exactly
22:32
what note it is and so on
22:34
and you can certainly label visual things
22:36
objects. We have things that colors, right?
22:38
But with smells, it's a little bit
22:40
different. And we don't have a great
22:43
lexicon for smells. A lot of times
22:45
we're relegated to kind of like lavender,
22:47
kind of like and fill in the
22:49
blank, right? Something that you're familiar with.
22:51
But that's the rub. You have to
22:53
go back to something that you're sense
22:55
before. And that may be just an
22:58
evolutionary thing, like we haven't really evolved
23:00
to prioritize this kind of information, smell
23:02
information. We don't use it typically to
23:04
navigate around the world, although if you've
23:06
got a nice baking cake in the
23:08
kitchen you might navigate your way there.
23:10
So in some situations maybe it's helpful,
23:12
but we tend to navigate mostly based
23:15
on visual information. So our sense of
23:17
smell isn't keen enough to save us,
23:19
essentially, so it remains pretty mid. And
23:21
think back to any time you've tried
23:23
to describe a smell, you've probably said
23:25
it smells like and thought of the
23:27
last instance or the strongest instance of
23:30
smelling something like that thing. we tend
23:32
to kind of know what we need
23:34
to do based on visual information. So
23:36
because it was never prioritized, we never
23:38
sort of bothered to create robust labels
23:40
for it. And when you don't have
23:42
labels for things, your brains struggle to
23:45
kind of store it with that fidelity
23:47
because you don't have that verbal thing
23:49
that you can attach to it. I
23:51
know what a coffee mug is because
23:53
I have a verbal label for those
23:55
coffee mugs, right? that's one piece. The
23:57
other piece, which I think is fascinating,
24:00
is that especially smells can trigger memories,
24:02
sometimes long lost memories. There's certain smells
24:04
that will remind me of my grandmother's
24:06
house, certain smells that remind me of
24:08
specific people in my past, because that
24:10
might have been the perfume or clone
24:12
they wear or something like that are
24:15
similar to it. And then the experience
24:17
of going back sort of it like
24:19
washes over you. You kind of go
24:21
back in time, you're immersed, you're exactly
24:23
in that moment. The sense of smell
24:25
almost has this incredibly privileged capacity to
24:27
do this. And we don't know why
24:30
this happens, but we suspect that it's
24:32
possible part of it is the fact
24:34
that your sense of smell, unlike all
24:36
of the other senses, it has direct
24:38
access to your hippocampus, your memory bits
24:40
of the brain. And it's not clear
24:42
why that is, but it's sort of
24:45
co-evolved that way. The sensory systems in
24:47
the brain that are outside of smell,
24:49
so vision, audition, all have to go
24:51
through the thalamus, which is this sort
24:53
of major hub in the brain, before
24:55
they get to the memory parts of
24:57
the brain. There isn't like this direct
24:59
access, whereas everywhere else. This happens for
25:02
our sense of smell, it doesn't happen.
25:04
Our sense of smell doesn't go through
25:06
the thalamus. It, like, directly has this,
25:08
you know, revolving door straight into our
25:10
memory bits of the brain. And we
25:12
have no clue why the hell that
25:14
is. It's just this weird, quizzical thing.
25:17
And I don't know to what extent
25:19
that means it's truly privileged, and that's
25:21
the reason why we remember things so
25:23
vividly. But it's right there. It's like
25:25
an express train. Yeah, yeah. So exciting.
25:27
What about as someone who has had
25:29
a nasty concussion? Hope, Lauren Gallario, Addie
25:32
Capello, Adam Foote's wife Anna, a bunch
25:34
of people wanted to know how studying
25:36
concussions or TBI has influenced work or
25:38
influences our memory. You know, it's challenging.
25:40
concussions or TBI traumatic brain injury because
25:42
there's no two injuries that are the
25:44
same. So that is a particularly difficult
25:47
set of conditions. It depends on the
25:49
severity of the concussion, the location, all
25:51
sorts of things. And we're learning a
25:53
lot more about this. Clearly it impacts
25:55
a variety of different memory systems memory
25:57
being one of the key ones that
25:59
gets impacted. But depending on the kind
26:02
of injury, whether it's a coup, counter
26:04
coup kind of injury, where they're sort
26:06
of stretching and shearing of some of
26:08
the brains, white matter, pathways, the connections
26:10
between different regions, all of those things
26:12
tend to happen. There's inflammation, there's sometimes
26:14
frank injury, you can actually see evidence
26:17
of that. But it's not clear how
26:19
much of that is, first of all,
26:21
common across individuals, because again, the extent
26:23
of the injury is different. The ideology,
26:25
the root of the injury, the cause,
26:27
it can be very, very different. But
26:29
the fact that memory is impacted almost
26:32
in all concussive injuries is an interesting
26:34
phenomenon. And I always go back to
26:36
our memory system, or at least the
26:38
hippocampal memory system, is one of the
26:40
most vulnerable systems in the brain. It
26:42
tends to feel the brunt of pretty
26:44
much anything. Patients Rye of the Tiger,
26:46
Heidi, Brinipolensia, and Christine Hurley asked about
26:49
what is loathsomely called mom brain or
26:51
in Tim Farr's words, I don't feel
26:53
like I know things anymore and it's
26:55
horrible. I myself do not have kids
26:57
but I have damaged my brain with
26:59
a hospital grade blow to the head.
27:01
I feel for you. and not just
27:04
concussions, but even if we're sleep deprived,
27:06
even if we're anxious or depressed or
27:08
anything that's happening to us tends to
27:10
impact the system. It is one of
27:12
our most primitive systems in the brain.
27:14
It's one that we shared with all
27:16
the mammalian species and many others. Even
27:19
though it's tucked in the middle of
27:21
the brain, it seems like it would
27:23
be nice and enveloped and covered up.
27:25
It has kind of weird positioning. The
27:27
vascular around it is quite vulnerable. It's
27:29
also very close to the most poorest
27:31
parts. the blood brain barrier so toxins
27:34
can get into those limbic structures much
27:36
more easily than in other places in
27:38
the brain. So remember from part one
27:40
that memories are stored all over the
27:42
brain, kind of like coins hidden in
27:44
the sand, but the hippocampus, that seahorse-shaped
27:46
organ, acts as a sort of metal
27:49
detector to find and retrieve those memories.
27:51
So the hippocampus is this wicked combination
27:53
of really important and very delicate. Just
27:55
be careful. So it's just a hub
27:57
of vulnerability. And it's one of the
27:59
reasons why we studied so excessively in
28:01
my lab across a variety of different
28:04
conditions because we believe that it is
28:06
very vulnerable. And if we develop ways
28:08
to be able to protect it or
28:10
treat it, that will generalize across a
28:12
number of different conditions. And is it
28:14
rather new that we even know that
28:16
the blood brain barrier is permeable at
28:18
all? Yeah, I would think, I mean,
28:21
we've always known that it's permeable to
28:23
some things. We know this because there's
28:25
a lot of things that are in
28:27
our blood that get through the blood-brain
28:29
barrier very quickly and get into our
28:31
brain easily. Alcohol is one of those,
28:33
right? That gets straight from the blood
28:36
to the brain. So there are things
28:38
that we've known about for quite some
28:40
time that can traverse that barrier with
28:42
great ease. But there are new things
28:44
that we're learning about now that we
28:46
didn't realize can get through the blood-brain
28:48
barrier easily that seem to get through.
28:51
And some of these things may be
28:53
inflammatory in nature, some of them may
28:55
be toxins. So when we talk about
28:57
the connection between the body and the
28:59
brain, that is. real and it's always
29:01
been there but we've only really started
29:03
to study the sort of mind body
29:06
or brain body connection much more in
29:08
recent years and trying to understand how
29:10
our gut for example influences our brain
29:12
how our brain influences our gut this
29:14
back and forth which has to be
29:16
able to kind of get through some
29:18
of those barriers in the blood brain
29:21
barrier being the key one. One of
29:23
the most interesting things that happened in
29:25
terms of technology recently is there is
29:27
actually an approach technique using what's called
29:29
focused ultrasound to open up the blood
29:31
brain barrier to be able to transform.
29:33
through because it's
29:36
always been a challenge
29:38
for us in
29:40
developing drugs and interventions,
29:42
of pharmacological interventions,
29:44
how to package something
29:46
to right to get
29:48
it through the
29:51
blood -brain barrier. it through the
29:53
this may be
29:55
this other approach. It
29:57
seems a little
29:59
scary, I know, but
30:01
with focused scary, I know,
30:03
but what's called microbubbles
30:05
we can actually
30:08
open up the blood
30:10
-brain barrier and get
30:12
some things open up
30:14
be our larger barrier
30:16
that normally wouldn't get
30:18
through maybe our to
30:20
go through the blood
30:23
-brain barrier. that normally wouldn't
30:25
of fun activity these days in research on the
30:27
blood -brain barrier. barrier. Well you something about the
30:29
gut connection the gut People are talking
30:31
a lot about the vagus nerve. the
30:33
Just a quick background on that vagus
30:35
nerve. that the longest nerve in your
30:37
body and it plays this key
30:40
role in your parasympathetic nervous system, which
30:42
is the chill side, which is the the
30:44
rest the rest and digest. the vagus nerve
30:46
carries messages between your heart and your
30:48
brain and your guts. guts. And according
30:50
to this to this study, childhood trauma
30:52
and syncope burden among older adults, researchers
30:54
say that vasovagal syncope, where your heart
30:56
rate and blood pressure fall suddenly, which
30:59
can cause dizziness and sweating or
31:01
fainting, According to the study, it's
31:03
governed by the autonomic nervous
31:05
system and it's often precipitated by
31:07
a highly salient emotional situation. And
31:09
researchers found that the report
31:11
of childhood abuse was independently associated
31:13
with frequent syncope in youth. youth.
31:15
So So what role does this
31:17
physiology have? Does Does that vagus
31:20
nerve play a role in
31:22
memory at all? It certainly does. One
31:24
easy way to think about that known
31:26
has been known for quite some time
31:28
is that this is one of
31:30
the ways by which the adrenal hormones
31:32
can impact the brain. So when
31:34
we think about cortisol and cortisol release,
31:36
when we think about those kinds of those
31:38
kinds of things do have a
31:40
way to be able to impact the
31:42
brain through its impact on the
31:44
vagus nerve. But it's way beyond that
31:46
now. a There's a lot of literature
31:48
now suggesting that vagal stimulation, for
31:50
example, could have some really interesting effects,
31:52
therapeutic in some ways. And we're starting to starting
31:54
to understand a little bit
31:56
more about how that that of
31:58
communication is operating. is operating. there
32:00
and it's again one that we
32:03
stand to learn a lot more
32:05
about. I feel like this is
32:07
all a really good endorsement for
32:09
meditation and deep breathing. Well I
32:11
mean that should be endorsed right
32:13
off the bat all the time.
32:15
You know you're mentioning toxicity in
32:17
the brain, blood brain barrier, alcohol,
32:19
many people wanted to know Matthew
32:21
Walker, Allie and Julian and Neil
32:23
Anderson asked about different opinions of
32:25
alcohol affecting memory, but others have
32:27
more herbal questions, such as Allie
32:29
Myers, Adam, Michael James, Storm, Saricity,
32:31
and Olympia Rental, and first-time question-askers
32:33
Craig Steinberg and Zach Gary, and...
32:35
Chris Bullock asked, why does marijuana
32:37
make memory so shitty asking for
32:40
a friend? Winky face. Okay, asking
32:42
for a friend. Can't say that
32:44
I've had firsthand experience on this,
32:46
but I can say that, first
32:48
of all, there's different kinds of
32:50
memory that are made, you know,
32:52
shitty to different extent by marijuana.
32:54
So it doesn't impact all kinds
32:56
of memory. It might impact your
32:58
recollection for things that you were
33:00
doing previously or sort of around
33:02
that same time. Not exactly clear
33:04
why that happens, but I can
33:06
tell you that one of the
33:08
things that's really interesting about marijuana
33:10
is that when you think about
33:12
how it impacts the brain, there
33:14
are particular receptors in the brain,
33:16
what we call endokinbanoid receptors. that
33:19
are specifically geared to responding to
33:21
cannonoids, which is essentially the active
33:23
species in marijuana. But the interesting
33:25
thing is that endokinoid receptors are
33:27
involved in long-term potentiation in memory,
33:29
in plasticity. So in some ways,
33:31
it's not surprising that it impacts
33:33
your memory. It's surprising if it's
33:35
always shitty, because I think there's
33:37
probably an optimality, I'm not telling
33:39
it, you should use it to
33:41
improve memory. But there may be
33:43
some realm in which you can
33:45
actually improve plasticity as opposed to
33:47
make it worse. That's very difficult
33:49
to get at an individual level.
33:51
But the fact that you have
33:53
receptors in your brain, and especially
33:55
in your memory system, specifically geared
33:58
to responding to the impact of
34:00
marijuana, I think is a very,
34:02
very cool thing. And it also
34:04
tells us that we need to
34:06
invest a lot more energy and
34:08
a lot more time and a
34:10
lot more resources in understanding exactly
34:12
how it's impacting the brain, how
34:14
that changes from individual to individual,
34:16
on what background and what context
34:18
with everything else is happening in
34:20
the brain. So recreational use versus
34:22
use for depression and anxiety, other
34:24
things. some more sort of therapeutic
34:26
uses. All of those are really
34:28
interesting questions. And now that we
34:30
are seeing the legalization sort of,
34:32
you know, across many states and
34:34
a desire from the National Institutes
34:37
of Health to really support research
34:39
on this front, I'm hopeful that
34:41
we'll be able to have a
34:43
lot more answers. A lot of
34:45
my colleagues here actually are studying
34:47
this exact thing. Would there be
34:49
a difference between the CBD component
34:51
and the THC component? Definitely. So
34:53
they're different chemicals and they have
34:55
different potency and different binding properties
34:57
and so on. I don't know
34:59
that there's as much evidence for
35:01
CBD in terms of brain active
35:03
kinds of things or psychoactive kinds
35:05
of things. There may be a
35:07
little bit out there, but certainly
35:09
the impact of THC has been
35:11
the one that's studied much more
35:13
and there's a lot more literature
35:16
on that. So a 2023 paper
35:18
in the journal Biomolecules, titled Effective
35:20
Cannabis on Memory Consolidation, Learning and
35:22
Retrieval, and its current legal status
35:24
in India, acknowledges that, quote, the
35:26
role of cannabis on cognitive functions
35:28
is a matter of long debate.
35:30
but that generally THC is responsible
35:32
for cognition-related deficits while non-psychoactive CDD
35:34
has been shown to elicit neuroprotective
35:36
activity. However, because it's a restricted
35:38
substance, There's not enough research on
35:40
it, they say. And contradictions exist.
35:42
And some reports showed low THC
35:44
dose improved learning and cognition. So
35:46
I guess keep an eye out
35:48
for emerging studies. And as discussed
35:50
in the recent surgical angiology episode
35:52
on veins and arteries, smoking is
35:55
not the best way. ingest it
35:57
if you're going to. So remember
35:59
to protect your blood plumbing for
36:01
the long term. Oh patrons Lila
36:03
Weller, Carleen D.H. and Barb Miller
36:05
had questions about the long and
36:07
the short of it all. Well
36:09
you know I feel like some
36:11
people talk about short-term versus long-term
36:13
when it comes to that but
36:15
the difference between short-term memory and
36:17
long-term when does it become a
36:19
long-term memory? When does it get
36:21
filed? When I talk to people
36:23
about long-term and short-term memory, typically
36:25
they're complaining about their long-term memory
36:27
and they're saying my short-term memory
36:29
is okay. So I can remember
36:31
things from yesterday or the week
36:34
before, but it's like, you know,
36:36
long-term memory that's impaired. Or my
36:38
mother, if they're talking about maybe
36:40
their mother with developing Alzheimer's or
36:42
early dementia, it's her long-term memory
36:44
is okay. And I kind of
36:46
have to stop and say, okay,
36:48
let me just clear up the
36:50
terminology so that when you're talking
36:52
to a physician or talking to
36:54
somebody, they understand what you're talking
36:56
about. Both of those are long-term
36:58
memory. You're talking about recent versus
37:00
remote memories. Short-term memory is very,
37:02
very short. We're talking the span
37:04
of seconds. So that's at least
37:06
the way that these things are
37:08
defined in psychology and neuroscience. Short-term
37:10
memory is extremely short. It's what
37:13
we also call working memory. If
37:15
I were to give you a
37:17
phone number, not that you would
37:19
have to dial a phone number
37:21
these days. But if I were
37:23
to give you a phone number
37:25
and say, hey, hold on to
37:27
this phone number and then you
37:29
have to dial it, you might
37:31
sort of rehearse that phone number
37:33
to yourself for a few seconds
37:35
and then you dial it and
37:37
then what happens to that number?
37:40
goes away. So you stored it very
37:42
briefly in your short-term memory store, your
37:44
working memory, which is there to be
37:46
able to help you store things for
37:49
a very short period of time, you
37:51
can get distracted out of it very
37:53
quickly, and also I can exceed your
37:55
span very quickly if I just yell
37:57
out a whole bunch of numbers out
38:00
to you, things are going to fall
38:02
out, right? it's not intended to store
38:04
any more than just a few, a
38:06
handful of items. People used to say
38:08
seven plus or minus two, but that
38:11
number is likely closer to three or
38:13
four. Okay, he just threw a whole
38:15
bunch of numbers at you, but seven
38:17
plus or minus two means that brain
38:19
scientists used to think we can hold
38:22
five to nine items in our short-term
38:24
memory, but turns out we don't even
38:26
have that much room. It's like three
38:28
or four things. It's like three or
38:30
four things. So then everything beyond that
38:33
is actually long-term memory storage, but when
38:35
we think about memory for yesterday or
38:37
last week or the month before versus,
38:39
you know, years ago, we're talking about
38:41
recent long-term memories versus remote long-term memories.
38:44
And as we get older, our memory
38:46
for things that happened way in the
38:48
past could be very preserved because we
38:50
talked about how memory over time gets
38:52
strengthened and kind of linked to a
38:55
whole bunch of different regions of the
38:57
brain, so it becomes more robust. more
38:59
resilient to forgetting. Those are typically the
39:01
last memories to go. say for a
39:03
patient experiencing dementia, they're going to remember
39:06
those memories much later in the progression.
39:08
But memories of the last few months,
39:10
the last year, the last few years,
39:12
they're going to be the ones that
39:14
are the earliest to go. Because they
39:17
have not been solidified as much. They
39:19
have not been stored in all of
39:21
these parallel networks in the brain and
39:23
made kind of more resilient to forgetting.
39:25
They're still somewhat dependent on the hippocampus.
39:28
And the hippocampus is kind of the
39:30
culprit in early dementia, right? It's one
39:32
of the places that's changing very early.
39:34
So as we think about long-term versus
39:36
short-term, that's typically what people are thinking
39:39
about. Why am I not remembering as
39:41
much of the recent things, but I
39:43
remember memories in the past for longer?
39:45
We experience that as we get older,
39:47
but much more dramatically in the context
39:50
of dementia. Yeah, so it's just a
39:52
little bit of a misnomer, but I
39:54
think, you know, being able to kind
39:56
of divide it into recent versus remote
39:58
covers the same question. Yeah. Oh, I
40:01
had no idea. And I do want
40:03
to ask about dementia. Alzheimer's as well,
40:05
but a quick detour with short-term memory.
40:07
Many people mentioned having the memory of
40:09
a goldfish. Jasmine Patino, first-time question asker,
40:12
whose dear wonderful boyfriend is a goldfish
40:14
in that capacity, and Caitlin Tyndale, who
40:16
compared themselves to a fish cognitively. True
40:18
or false, goldfish remember for two minutes,
40:20
and then they don't know why they're
40:23
in a bowl. You know, I'm trying
40:25
to remember finding Nemo now and thinking
40:27
about whether it was truly two minutes.
40:29
You know, I don't know if it's
40:31
exactly two minutes, but it is very
40:34
short. It is thought to be one
40:36
of the shortest memory spans. How do
40:38
they test that on a goldfish? It's
40:40
difficult. I think that whenever you're testing
40:42
things with animals, you have to be
40:45
clever, right? So you have to figure
40:47
out a way that the animal can
40:49
kind of indicate to you whether or
40:51
not they recognize something. Maybe it's by
40:53
the amount of exploration or the amount
40:56
of time they spend in the vicinity
40:58
of that thing. If they're more familiar
41:00
with it, maybe they'll navigate to something
41:02
that's newer. So you can position things
41:04
in the environment in a way where
41:07
you can see how much they explore
41:09
one over the other and be able
41:11
to tell, oh, yeah, their memory is
41:13
shot, or maybe their memory is really
41:15
good. I love the idea that they're
41:18
like, you know this guy? And they're
41:20
like, no, no, no. The name is
41:22
not coming to my, I remember the
41:24
face though, anywhere, but yeah. We're in
41:27
luck because yeah, people study this, of
41:29
course they do. And according to a
41:31
recent paper, distance estimation in the Goldfish
41:33
in the proceedings of the Royal Society
41:35
B of Biological Sciences Journal, Goldfish can
41:38
accurately estimate distance after learning it. And
41:40
another study in the journal animals titled,
41:42
visual perception of photographs of rotated 3D
41:44
objects in goldfish, trained six goldfish to
41:46
tap either a photo of a frog
41:49
or a turtle for a treat. And
41:51
researchers report that all the fish had
41:53
successful performance showing that they were able
41:55
to distinguish between the turtle and the
41:57
frog photographs, which is evidence of object
42:00
constancy. So flim flam. Goldfish memories are.
42:02
trash, they can be trained to do
42:04
things. And it should be noted that
42:06
in five years of having my daughter,
42:08
who's a poodle mix named Gremlin, I
42:11
have never successfully trained her to do
42:13
anything. So lay off the goldfish and
42:15
lay off yourselves. Although, when it comes
42:17
to future fears and caring for loved
42:19
ones, many patrons had questions about Alzheimer's
42:22
and dementia, including Lisa Gorman, Deb, Deb,
42:24
Debdust, Stephanie, Howfrey, Maddie Cakes, Two Stones
42:26
with one check. Me, Meg McDaniel, Ken
42:28
Edmondson, Aaron White, Camellia B. Brian Reesinger,
42:30
Sarah Crocker, and Stephanie, who wrote, there
42:33
are cases of dementia on both sides
42:35
of my family. Is there actually anything
42:37
we can do to stop or slow
42:39
down this awful disease? A lot of
42:41
people, obviously, concerned about dementia, concerned about
42:44
Alzheimer's, that deserves its own two-part episode.
42:46
Can you describe when it comes to
42:48
memory the difference between dementia and Alzheimer's,
42:50
is Alzheimer's a disease and dementia is
42:52
the symptom of it or how does
42:55
it exactly what's happening? Yeah, so the
42:57
easiest way to think about it is
42:59
that dementia is a larger umbrella term
43:01
and Alzheimer's disease is one of the
43:03
principal causes of dementia. Okay. You're right
43:06
that dementia is a set of symptoms
43:08
and Alzheimer's is a little bit more
43:10
about the biology that leads to those
43:12
symptoms and there are many other types
43:14
of dementia. So Parkinson's disease can lead
43:17
to dementia. There's frontal temporal low bar
43:19
degeneration or frontal temporal dementia. a huntington's
43:21
disease can lead to dementia. So there's
43:23
a number of different causes for dementia,
43:25
but the most prevalent one, the one
43:28
that most people are really concerned about
43:30
is Alzheimer's dementia. So yeah, dementia is
43:32
the umbrella term, Alzheimer's is the subcategory
43:34
or the set of causes that lead
43:36
down the path to dementia, and it's
43:39
among many others, but Alzheimer's the chief
43:41
one. When we think about how do
43:43
we differentiate between dementia and say, you
43:45
know, healthy aging, that's another question that
43:47
pops up a lot, is as I'm
43:50
getting to a certain age, I feel
43:52
like I am losing my memory. I'm
43:54
starting to lose my way when I
43:56
know. I'm having some memory issues, I'm
43:58
being forgetful. And some of that happens
44:01
as we all get older, and the
44:03
majority of it is okay, right? That's
44:05
just the natural part of a normal
44:07
aging brain. But when it becomes pervasive
44:09
and noticeable to not just to the
44:12
person but to others around them, and
44:14
people are sort of missing doctor's appointments
44:16
or getting lost around their neighborhoods and
44:18
they're wandering and then it becomes a
44:20
real concern. And when that's happening already
44:23
things have changed so much in the
44:25
brain that now it's really unable to
44:27
compensate for it because we tend to
44:29
compensate so much for any sort of
44:31
brain deficit for the longest time. So
44:34
for many years. a patient with Alzheimer's
44:36
disease wouldn't technically be a patient because
44:38
they're not reporting symptoms, they're not experiencing
44:40
anything, neither patient nor doctor can say
44:42
anything is wrong with the brain, but
44:45
already the pathology is changing the brain.
44:47
And one of the challenges for us
44:49
in research is trying to develop ways
44:51
that we can detect that pathology, maybe
44:53
with brain imaging, with brain scans and
44:56
so on, very early, even at a
44:58
time when the patient and the doctor
45:00
don't really know that anything is wrong.
45:02
But when it comes to memory deficits,
45:04
experience at that older age, you know,
45:07
the question is always, how do I
45:09
know when it's really kind of tipped
45:11
over? How do I know when it's
45:13
really problematic? And a good rule of
45:15
thumb to think about is that if
45:18
somebody's forgetting things, all natural and fine,
45:20
especially if they can remember it later
45:22
on, if they're reminded and they go,
45:24
oh, now I remember. So it's tracked
45:27
somewhere, it's there, you know that it's
45:29
okay, maybe that's challenging as to some
45:31
extent. But if they're really never able
45:33
to piece it back together, and no
45:35
reminder is helping them, that they may
45:38
be kind of over that cliff, and
45:40
they're going down the path to Alzheimer's
45:42
disease, the somewhat more crass example is
45:44
if you forget where you placed your
45:46
car keys, it's okay, but if you
45:49
forget that you drove the car, you
45:51
know, then that may be a challenge.
45:53
Is that because of plaques in the
45:55
brain, is that parts of the brain
45:57
atrophy? almost spaces spaces be more
46:00
white there used to
46:02
be more white
46:04
or gray matter. We
46:06
happening biologically. this idea, We
46:08
used to have this idea and
46:11
the idea took the field by
46:13
storm I actually resulted in I
46:15
think an over -investment of resources into
46:17
clinical trials rid try to get rid of
46:19
those plaques. We used to think that
46:21
plaques were sort of of the evil, right? And the
46:23
the two pathologies of Alzheimer's disease are
46:26
plaques and tangles. Plaques Plaques are made
46:28
up of amyloid protein. are made up of are
46:30
made up of what's called And protein. time
46:32
And for the longest time, people thought
46:34
if you have amyloid and tau or
46:36
plaques and tangles in the brain, that's
46:38
Alzheimer's disease. And we should be trying
46:40
to break up those pathologies somehow to restore
46:43
the brain or prevent it from restore brain being
46:45
worse. worse. But the
46:47
reality is is idea never really fully panned
46:49
out because just having amyloid in the brain
46:51
is not sufficient for you to experience memory
46:53
problems. This is not sufficient for you to
46:55
have dementia. not about a third of everyone with
46:57
amyloid in their brain a likely never experience
46:59
dementia. amyloid in their brain will likely by itself,
47:02
it's not dementia. So clearly by itself
47:04
else that you mentioned turns out to
47:06
be sufficient and really important, which is
47:08
sufficient and really important, which atrophy, actual
47:10
loss of cells. loss of cells. That doesn't
47:12
happen naturally as we age. age. We We
47:15
lose synapses, we we lose lose connections.
47:17
As we get older, it becomes more
47:19
difficult to make them, difficult to
47:21
maintain them. That happens for sure. that
47:23
But cell loss, cell loss in these massive
47:25
amounts doesn't really happen unless a
47:27
progressive neurodegenerative illness, which is
47:29
what Alzheimer's disease is. disease is. So when
47:32
we find evidence of neurodegeneration,
47:34
say for example on MRI scans,
47:36
we see a very close
47:38
connection, a close link a a
47:40
relationship the the extent of that
47:42
neurodegeneration and memory symptoms or memory
47:44
loss, and cognitive symptoms down
47:47
the line that are not memory
47:49
not So function gets disrupted, all
47:51
of our cognitive faculties, right?
47:53
Because it's progressive. But in the But in
47:55
the absence of that neurodegenerative
47:57
change, it's very difficult to see
47:59
associations with actual or clinical decline,
48:01
which has created a bit of a
48:04
dilemma for the field, even though the
48:06
FDA has been approving anti-amyloid therapeutics, these
48:08
drugs have some nasty side effects. And
48:10
it's not clear that clearing amyloid is
48:13
going to be the solution in the
48:15
long run. Coupled with that, also the
48:17
complexity that Alzheimer's disease is extremely heterogeneous.
48:19
So if I were to get it,
48:22
and hopefully not, but if you were
48:24
to get it, it may look very
48:26
different in your brain than it does
48:29
in my brain. We may have a
48:31
lot of variability. So that means you
48:33
may respond to a drug or therapy
48:35
that I may not respond to and
48:38
vice versa. So the idea of like
48:40
a one-size-fits-all kind of solution is also
48:42
kind of falling away by the wayside
48:44
and people thinking about the complexity of
48:47
maybe Alzheimer's disease is actually diseases and
48:49
we need to be able to tailor
48:51
therapeutics to the individual. Obviously dementia once
48:54
again deserves this own episode. It's a
48:56
complex condition with a lot of emerging
48:58
research, but there are some pharmaceutical treatments
49:00
that can involve modulating neurotransmitters in the
49:03
brain like glutamate. There are dietary modifications
49:05
that can ease some related symptoms and
49:07
psychopharmaceuticals that can help with depression and
49:10
anxiety related to the progression of dementia.
49:12
and Robert G. O'Day and Isabelle LeClerk
49:14
mentioned leeway body dementia in their questions,
49:16
and it is the second most common
49:19
cause of dementia after Alzheimer's, and it's
49:21
caused by aggregations of proteins in the
49:23
brain called leeway bodies. And some symptoms
49:25
of it can include visual hallucinations, trouble
49:28
with sleep, including sleepwalking, mood changes, and
49:30
stiffness. And Isabelle, whose mom had asked
49:32
why public awareness of leeway body dementia,
49:35
was low, but there's a little bit
49:37
of background on it. And we're going
49:39
to get to more questions about how
49:41
you can avoid dementia and memory loss
49:44
in yourself per an expert, but first
49:46
let's donate to a cause of his
49:48
choosing. And he opted to send it
49:50
to a fund at his center called
49:53
the Junior Scholars Fund, which goes to
49:55
supporting graduate students and their professional development
49:57
because he says they are the next.
50:00
of talent and he wants to do
50:02
whatever he can to support them. And
50:04
there's a link in the show notes
50:07
for more on the Junior Scholars Fund
50:09
at the UC Irvine Center for the
50:11
Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, so thanks
50:13
to sponsors of the show for making
50:15
that donation possible. As a parent, you
50:17
want what's best for your teen. You
50:19
want them to grow and thrive in
50:21
this world, but you also want to
50:24
make sure they're staying safe. That's
50:26
why Instagram is introducing teen
50:28
accounts. With automatic protections for
50:31
who can contact teens and
50:33
the content they can see.
50:35
Instagram teen accounts, built-in limits
50:37
for teens, and peace of
50:39
mind for parents. Learn more
50:41
at instagram.com/teen accounts. After
50:46
the end of a good fight, you
50:48
deserve an ice-cold reward. Medella, you put
50:50
in the hours, the energy, the tough
50:52
labor, because you know, the bigger the
50:55
fight, the better the reward. Medella, the
50:57
mark of the fight. Quick response of
50:59
the beer reported by Crown in Portugal,
51:01
Illinois. Why get all
51:03
your holiday decorations delivered through Instacart? Because
51:05
maybe you only bought two wreaths but
51:07
have 12 windows. Or maybe your toddler
51:09
got very eager with the advent calendar.
51:11
Or maybe the inflatable snowman didn't make
51:14
it through the snowstorm. Or maybe the
51:16
Twinkle Heights aren't twinkling. Whatever the reason
51:18
this season, Instacarts here for hosts in
51:20
their whole holiday hall. Get decorations from
51:22
the Home Depot, CVS and more through
51:24
Instacart and more through Instacart and enjoy
51:26
free delivery on your first three orders.
51:28
Service fees and terms apply. Service fees
51:30
and terms apply. Okay,
51:35
back to the million-dollar question here,
51:37
which honestly I think is eating
51:39
away at everyone listening, but it
51:41
was asked by Stephanie Christina Manuge,
51:43
who loves Saunas, Margaret Onuska, Kerat
51:45
Singh, Shishanja Gettinger, Nicole DiG, Cairo
51:48
Young, and Jenny Hoover. So let's
51:50
go. Well, I think, you know,
51:52
last listener question that we got
51:54
so much, obviously is, every day,
51:56
I feel like we're all getting
51:58
older, it's nuts, wow. We're we're
52:00
going to get old eventually if
52:02
we're lucky, right? if we're What actually does
52:05
help? does help us sharp and
52:07
retain our memories. Is it
52:09
Sudoku? Is it going to
52:11
Zuma to Is it reading? it
52:13
reading? Yeah, you know, the you know,
52:15
the challenge of brain sort of body aging
52:17
of body aging. one. I think an interesting
52:20
one. think everybody wants to and longer and
52:22
live happier and healthier lives and
52:24
so on. And the trick is to
52:26
make sure that our brain aging
52:28
is sort of consistent with our body
52:30
aging. You really want to kind
52:32
of maintain health across both of those.
52:34
So So mind longevity is something that
52:36
we think about a lot. a And
52:38
there are some answers. So we know,
52:40
for example, that that maintaining levels of
52:42
physical activity, regular physical activity, hopefully
52:45
not just undertaken when you're 80, right?
52:47
of in midlife and of in midlife and
52:49
continuing. of moderate levels of seems to physical seems to
52:51
to be helpful, seem to be preventative. reduced
52:53
They are associated with disease. If for Alzheimer's
52:55
disease if people get it, they tend
52:57
to get it later in life, but
52:59
they're protected from it for some period
53:01
of time. time. So, exercise. exercise. you
53:03
can for more, you more,
53:05
can see the paper, and
53:07
and Dementia Prevention from the journal
53:09
Practical Neurology, which notes that
53:11
notes third of dementia cases
53:13
are attributable to modifiable
53:16
risk factors, such as physical
53:18
inactivity, smoking, and hypertension. And
53:21
they say that with the
53:23
rising prevalence of dementia, there is
53:25
a renewed focus on prevention
53:27
strategies. And exercise has emerged as
53:29
a key intervention as a influencing
53:31
cognition positive. including reducing the
53:33
risk of age the risk of
53:35
decline and dementia. decline and dementia. So
53:37
as walking time, dance
53:39
around your kitchen, get a
53:41
to walk, take a
53:44
break dancing class, do do
53:46
some arm stretches, just keep
53:48
it moving folks. Okay, what
53:50
else doc? The other other thing that
53:52
people have identified in in large
53:54
scale, not not just trials, but
53:56
also epidemiological studies, what seems to
53:58
help to social? social. and
54:00
this was a challenge in the pandemic
54:02
actually and I got to hear a
54:05
lot about that from folks who felt
54:07
that they had social structures and and
54:09
Zoom was no replacement for it. It
54:11
just did not help as much. You
54:13
really want to be around people and
54:15
being around people in an older age
54:17
is really key. Now we see this
54:19
all the time when people retire. There's
54:21
sort of a fork in the road.
54:23
You are very socially engaged in your
54:25
job in other settings. And then when
54:27
people retire, in some cases they become
54:29
very isolated and they're spending a lot
54:31
of time at home, maybe with just
54:33
a 24-hour new cycle and all that
54:35
kind of stuff, and not going out
54:37
and being around people. And others have
54:39
planned appropriately and said, these are going
54:41
to be my post-retirement plans. I'm going
54:43
to spend more time. in my community
54:45
center and my church and my whatever
54:47
it is to maintain that social level
54:49
of activity and those individuals tend to
54:51
do better cognitively over time. Wow. And
54:53
of course not everyone is able to
54:55
get the same levels of physical activity
54:57
and we have an entire episode about
54:59
disability sociology that discusses accommodations and attitudes
55:01
toward disability. So talk to a doctor
55:03
or a physical therapist about what you
55:05
can do and how your activity level
55:07
is. And we also have an episode
55:09
on chronic pain with some bio-psychosocial interventions
55:11
that have helped some folks. And as
55:13
this is being released right before the
55:15
crush of holiday travel and flu season,
55:17
likely a spike in more COVID cases,
55:19
it's always good to take precautionary measures
55:21
against infection. If I'm on a plane,
55:23
I'm in a mask. So do what's
55:25
right for you. But know that doing
55:28
physical activity that works for you and
55:30
staying social is incredibly important for your
55:32
health. and your brain. She said to
55:34
herself in a side. So maintaining a
55:36
good level of physical activity and a
55:38
good level of social activity, you know,
55:40
a great exercise to do both simultaneously
55:42
is dancing. So we tend to hear
55:44
about that a lot, like if somebody
55:46
is in a dance group or does
55:48
dance classes a couple times a week,
55:50
they are much happier. You know, you
55:52
get a lot of endogenous sort of
55:54
dopamine boosts that happen when you do
55:56
that. But the social exposure and the
55:58
physical activity. be key. Then we go
56:00
on to other things that people are
56:02
really curious about. What about brain games?
56:04
What about stoku? What about this and
56:06
this and that? And, you know, the
56:08
evidence there is a little bit shaky,
56:10
right? So there's some evidence to suggest
56:12
that maintaining sort of cognitive engagement, of
56:14
course, is very helpful, but most of
56:16
it is based on cognitive engagement, again,
56:18
in a social setting. So if you're
56:20
playing chess out in the park with
56:22
somebody and having discussions and all that,
56:24
that seems to be more helpful than
56:26
playing chess against the computer avatar at
56:28
home, right? You might think, well, cognitively
56:30
is the same on playing chess, but
56:32
it turns out to be different if
56:34
you're doing it with a human, right,
56:36
and actually having conversations and actually being
56:38
out and about. So again, I go
56:40
back to the two things that are
56:42
kind of tried and true and I
56:44
think are very helpful. The third thing
56:46
that seems to be helpful has to
56:49
do with diet and being able to
56:51
have sort of a heart healthy diet
56:53
because we know that heart health is
56:55
really important for brain health. So maintaining
56:57
something that looks close to something like
56:59
the Mediterranean diet, smaller amounts of red
57:01
meat and things like that, high levels
57:03
of leafy greens and fruits and vegetables
57:05
and those kinds of things. That also
57:07
seems to be associated with better longevity
57:09
and higher levels of cognition into that
57:11
longevity. Those are massive studies that were
57:13
done where they randomize people to either
57:15
the Mediterranean diet or a typical American
57:17
diet and they see some really decent
57:19
results for something like the Mediterranean diet.
57:21
But the brain game stuff is the
57:23
one that I'm sort of tentative about.
57:25
It's like, I don't know if it'll
57:27
hurt you, but I'm not sure it's
57:29
helping you all that much. You know,
57:31
my husband's grandfather played a lot of
57:33
video games in his later years and
57:35
he would play multiplayer, so he would
57:37
be on the headset with his grandkids
57:39
across the country while he was playing
57:41
like World Warcraft or League Legends or
57:43
League, and he was playing like World
57:45
of Warcraft or League of Legends or
57:47
whatever. And we've heard that from a
57:49
number of folks were involved, even in
57:51
our studies, said, you know, I started
57:53
playing video games and I started to,
57:55
and they felt like, like, even the
57:57
learning, the learning, a block, So wireless
57:59
challenging there's no incorporating the social aspect
58:01
into this has made a huge difference.
58:03
This is why I was also reluctant
58:05
to like tell my kids not to
58:07
play games and do things because I
58:09
felt like if they're doing it with
58:12
their friends and it's sort of communal
58:14
then it's a little bit different than
58:16
just droning in front of the computer
58:18
and playing a game by yourself against
58:20
the computer. Yeah, I love that you
58:22
are, you're not only researching this on
58:24
the daily, but also seeing it as
58:26
your children are growing up and watching
58:28
how memory might change, but I imagine
58:30
there are some difficult things about your
58:32
job or studying it. I always ask
58:34
what sucks, what sucks the most about
58:36
your job. Oh, what sucks the most.
58:38
Let's see. I think sometimes the pace
58:40
by which things move frustrates me. And
58:42
in research, you know, I think in
58:44
science in general, there's this notion that
58:46
we're going to do the best science
58:48
that we can, or we're going to
58:50
put it out there, we're going to
58:52
publish our work, and then hope that
58:54
somebody else is going to come and
58:56
take that work and build on it,
58:58
and then be able to translate things
59:00
and get them to be helpful to
59:02
somebody out in the real world. That
59:06
takes forever. And it's so frustrating that
59:08
it takes forever. And so one of
59:10
the things that I've started to do
59:12
in recent years and get my lab
59:14
more involved in is say to hell
59:16
with that, we're not going to wait.
59:19
We're actually going to try to do
59:21
it ourselves. So I've started to bridge
59:23
a little bit between sort of the
59:25
academic environment and more of the industry.
59:27
But for the longest time, it sucked.
59:29
It sucked. It felt like we're so
59:32
removed. as academics doing the science and
59:34
there's not enough of that science that's
59:36
getting out there and helping people, even
59:38
though it has the potential to help
59:40
people. And then the other thing that's
59:42
frustrating, I will say that, you know,
59:45
you can't change everything. You always want
59:47
to do what you feel is scientifically
59:49
very rigorous and also like morally and
59:51
ethically right and you have kind of
59:53
a code by which you operate but
59:55
there are certain systems in place that
59:58
are really difficult to change. And some
1:00:00
of them you can change and you
1:00:02
have to kind of figure out who
1:00:04
to work with to make sure the
1:00:06
messages communicated very well outside of academia
1:00:08
to be able to influence people. But
1:00:10
I'm very fortunate that I'm surrounded by
1:00:13
people who are equally frustrated and also
1:00:15
believe that that sucks. So at least
1:00:17
we can kind of rip off of
1:00:19
each other a little bit and commiserate
1:00:21
slash come up with ways that we
1:00:23
can try to address it. Well, there's
1:00:26
that community aspect too, right? Exactly. That's
1:00:28
helping your brain. That's the thing. Also,
1:00:30
science, you know, when it started out,
1:00:32
hundreds of years ago, when people were
1:00:34
doing science, it really was kind of
1:00:36
a solo practice. When you look at
1:00:39
Nobel laureates, it was always sort of
1:00:41
singular winners, and that's not a thing
1:00:43
anymore. Science is so communal now. It
1:00:45
really requires teams and communities of people
1:00:47
that are dedicated to solving these big,
1:00:49
you know, challenging questions. They're extremely challenginging.
1:00:52
And I think that's one of the
1:00:54
reasons why things have been exponentially growing
1:00:56
in recent years, right? It's not just
1:00:58
that we have better technology. We have
1:01:00
smarter people and hordes of them that
1:01:02
are dedicated to answering these questions and
1:01:04
doing it together. So when you look
1:01:07
at the number of authors on a
1:01:09
paper or the number of co-investigators on
1:01:11
a grant, those numbers have also shot
1:01:13
up. So that community aspect of it,
1:01:15
I think, is what keeps a lot
1:01:17
of us at the table and it's
1:01:20
what makes it worthwhile, worthwhile, despite the
1:01:22
occasional sucking. Well, if someone
1:01:24
wanted to go into this field or
1:01:26
just curious, what is the best thing
1:01:28
about your job? Oh my gosh, there's
1:01:30
so many things. I mean, I'm like
1:01:32
a Kidna candy store most of the
1:01:35
time. I can tell you that for
1:01:37
someone who is naturally very curious and
1:01:39
inquisitive, this is like heaven. Right? Because
1:01:41
you're learning new things every day and
1:01:43
it never stops. And there's no real
1:01:45
retirement also for people like me. Like
1:01:47
you retire, but you're still on recall,
1:01:50
you consult, you do things, you're constantly
1:01:52
continuing to learn. So if that's something
1:01:54
that appeals to you, being a constant
1:01:56
student, other thing also is being an
1:01:58
academic, I have incredible freedom to pursue
1:02:00
the questions of my interest and my
1:02:02
lab's interest. If tomorrow I decided I
1:02:04
wanted to study fruit flies, I will
1:02:07
not lose my job. I can do
1:02:09
that. I have to go support that
1:02:11
effort somehow. But no one tells me
1:02:13
what to study. No one tells me
1:02:15
what science to do. I get to
1:02:17
decide that and I do it communally
1:02:19
with my lab because we're all collectively
1:02:22
and so we get to decide on
1:02:24
the science that we want to do.
1:02:26
We get to write the grants and
1:02:28
papers together. So that's liberating and I
1:02:30
don't know of any other job out
1:02:32
there where you can decide what you
1:02:34
want to do at any given day
1:02:36
and just go do it. Right? That's
1:02:39
incredibly empowering. You can continue to do
1:02:41
what you want to do and continue
1:02:43
to be in love with it for
1:02:45
as long as you want to. This
1:02:47
has been such a journey into my
1:02:49
own brain and anyone listening. So thank
1:02:51
you for just inspiring us also to
1:02:54
treat our brains a little better. You're
1:02:56
very welcome. This was so much fun.
1:02:58
Thank you so much. This is a
1:03:00
great. So once again
1:03:02
ask neuroscientists neurotic questions if you are
1:03:04
me and thank you to Dr. Michael
1:03:07
Yassa and everyone down in Irvine for
1:03:09
helping arrange this. There's more links to
1:03:11
their lab and his work they're in
1:03:13
the show notes and up on our
1:03:16
website at alleyward.com/nimanology. and we're at ologies
1:03:18
on Instagram in our blue sky where
1:03:20
everyone seems to be headed. See you
1:03:23
over there. We also have smologies which
1:03:25
are shorter kid-friendly episodes that you can
1:03:27
find anywhere you get podcasts. We put
1:03:29
them in a new feed so it's
1:03:32
easier for parents and teachers or anyone
1:03:34
who's looking for shorter. Clean language versions
1:03:36
of ologies to find them. You can
1:03:38
look for the new green artwork and
1:03:41
the Smalagee's logo. Thank you also to
1:03:43
Aaron Talbert for admitting the ologies podcast
1:03:45
Facebook group. Aveline Malick makes our professional
1:03:48
transcripts. Kelly Arduired is the website. Noel
1:03:50
Dilworth is our birthday girl this past
1:03:52
week and she's our wonderful scheduling producer.
1:03:54
Susan Hale managing directs it all chick
1:03:57
chafie edits and lead editor and another
1:03:59
great brain is Mercedes-Maitland of mainland audio.
1:04:01
With some assistsists, the last couple from
1:04:03
Jared Sleeper of Mine Jam media when
1:04:06
I'm late on things. Nick Thorburn wrote
1:04:08
the theme music and if you stick
1:04:10
around until the end I tell you
1:04:12
a secret and this week it's that
1:04:15
I have a theory that if you
1:04:17
have a good friend who has a
1:04:19
party like they're close buds you should
1:04:22
either be the first one there to
1:04:24
help set up and just kind of
1:04:26
like set the mood so they're not
1:04:28
worrying about when people are going to
1:04:31
start showing up or you should be
1:04:33
the last one standing to help tidy
1:04:35
up and say hey man great party.
1:04:37
Relatedly, I love ice. I love having
1:04:40
any cold beverage with an absolutely egregious
1:04:42
amount of ice. So much so that
1:04:44
our freezer could not keep up with
1:04:47
my ice consumption. Our ice maker was
1:04:49
like, I don't know what to tell
1:04:51
you. Now, during early quarantine, there were
1:04:53
a lot of cafes that were closing,
1:04:56
and Jarrett surprised me when I was
1:04:58
out of town, I was helping my
1:05:00
dad. and he bought an ice machine
1:05:02
from a closing cafe. We have it
1:05:05
in our garage, and I use it
1:05:07
every single day, even in winter. Now,
1:05:09
what does this have to do with
1:05:12
parties? So we have become the friends
1:05:14
who show up first with a giant
1:05:16
bucket of ice, and it feels heroic.
1:05:18
So if you are someone who goes
1:05:21
to bed early, be the first to
1:05:23
show up at a party, pick up
1:05:25
some bags of ice on the way.
1:05:27
Everyone will love you forever, you get
1:05:30
to bed early. Okay. Just in general,
1:05:32
be safe out there, okay? Bye-bye. Packadermontology,
1:05:34
cryptozoology, lithology, meteorology, nephology, seriology, teleology, seriology,
1:05:37
cellology, cellology. Let's make some members, huh?
1:05:39
If your day sounds like. We need
1:05:41
the report A-SAP. You
1:05:44
deserve Medella, if you've persevered
1:05:47
through. You deserve this rich
1:05:49
golden logger with a crisp
1:05:51
and refreshing taste. Or if
1:05:54
you overcame... too
1:05:56
warm. You deserve
1:05:59
this ice cold reward. Madella,
1:06:01
the markable fight. Drink responsibly.
1:06:03
You're imported by
1:06:06
Crown Port Chicago, Illinois.
Podchaser is the ultimate destination for podcast data, search, and discovery. Learn More